The Moon Illusion and Size–Distance Scaling—Evidence for Shared Neural Patterns

The Moon Illusion and Size–Distance Scaling—Evidence for Shared Neural Patterns

The Moon Illusion and Size–Distance Scaling—Evidence for Shared Neural Patterns Ralph Weidner1*, Thorsten Plewan1,2*, Qi Chen1, Axel Buchner3, Peter H. Weiss1,4, and Gereon R. Fink1,4 Abstract ■ A moon near to the horizon is perceived larger than a moon pathway areas including the lingual and fusiform gyri. The func- at the zenith, although—obviously—the moon does not change tional role of these areas was further explored in a second ex- its size. In this study, the neural mechanisms underlying the periment. Left V3v was found to be involved in integrating “moon illusion” were investigated using a virtual 3-D environ- retinal size and distance information, thus indicating that the ment and fMRI. Illusory perception of an increased moon size brain regions that dynamically integrate retinal size and distance was associated with increased neural activity in ventral visual play a key role in generating the moon illusion. ■ INTRODUCTION psia; Sperandio, Kaderali, Chouinard, Frey, & Goodale, Although the moon does not change its size, the moon 2013; Enright, 1989; Roscoe, 1989). near to the horizon is perceived as relatively larger com- One concept of size constancy scaling implies that the pared with when it is located at the zenith. This phenome- retinal image size and the estimated distance of an object non is called the “moon illusion” and is one of the oldest are conjointly considered, thereby enabling constant size visual illusions known (Ross & Plug, 2002). Despite exten- perception of objects at different distances (Kaufman & sive research, no consensus has been reached regarding Kaufman, 2000). With respect to the moon illusion, appar- the underlying perceptual and neural correlates (Ross & ent distance theories, for example, propose that “the per- Plug, 2002; Hershenson, 1989). ceived distance to the moon at the horizon is greater than Many researchers agree that the moon illusion is caused to the zenith moon” (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2000), and by mechanisms that otherwise allow an accurate percep- therefore, despite the constant retinal size, the horizontal tion of an objectʼs size, regardless of its distance (size moon generates a larger percept than the zenithal moon. constancy). However, there are different opinions on One of many problems with this interpretation is, how- how size scaling and size constancy are implemented in ever, that participants tend to report the horizontal moon the human visual system. as both larger and nearer (Hershenson, 1989), a phenome- For instance, Berkeley (1709/1948) suggested that size non commonly referred to as the size–distance paradox. scaling is achieved by taking into account learned proprio- A potential solution of this paradox is to postulate the ceptive and visual cues such as the angle of regard or aerial existence of different levels of distance representations in perspective. It has furthermore been proposed that con- the brain (i.e., registered distance and apparent distance). stant size representations are formed by taking into According to this view, size distance scaling is initially account the relative size of neighboring objects (Baird, based on one form of distance representation (e.g., regis- Wagner, & Fuld, 1990; McCready, 1986; Restle, 1970; Rock tered distance), which makes the moon appear larger. & Ebenholtz, 1959), texture gradients, and invariant prop- This larger appearance then reduces “apparent” distance, erties of the environment (Gibson, 1979; please see Ross which results in the moon appearing closer (Gogel & & Plug, 2002; Ross & Plug, 1998, for more elaborated Mertz, 1989; Rock & Kaufman, 1962). reviews). In addition, perceived size has been linked to Likewise, the neural mechanisms underlying the moon oculomotor processes such as accommodation (accommo- illusion are unclear. Different variants of apparent dis- dative micropsia) and convergence (convergence micro- tance theories consistently suggest that the moon illusion involves brain areas that integrate distance and retinal (angular) object size to generate size–distance invariant 1Research Centre Jülich, 2Leibniz Research Centre for Working object representations. Environment and Human Factors (IfADo), Dortmund, Germany, Thus far, investigating the “natural” moon illusion within 3Heinrich-Heine-Universität Düsseldorf, 4Cologne University an fMRI setting was hindered by the fact that the moon *These authors contributed equally to this work. cannot easily be transferred to an artificial environment. © 2014 Massachusetts Institute of Technology Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26:8, pp. 1871–1882 doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00590 For instance, binocular vision seems to play an important fMRI Measurement and Data Preprocessing role in enhancing the apparent size of the nearby horizon Functional imaging data were acquired by means of a 3-T moon (e.g., Enright, 1989). Presenting the moon illusion TRIO MRI system (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) using on a standard 2-D computer screen may thus obscure T2*-weighted EPI sequence (repetition time = 2.2 sec, important aspects. Nowadays, however, visual stimulus echo time = 30 sec). In Experiment 1, 415 volumes were presentation devices such as magnetic resonance (MR)- acquired, and 605 volumes were obtained in Experiment 2. compatible goggle systems allow 3-D presentation even Each volume consisted of 36 axial slices, allowing for within an fMRI environment, thereby enabling a functional whole-brain coverage (3 mm thickness, distance factor investigation of the moon illusion, albeit an artificial one. 10%, field of view 200 mm, 64 × 64 matrix, in-plane voxel The goal of this study was not only to reveal the neural size 3 × 3 mm2). correlates associated with experiencing this illusion by The fMRI data preprocessing and statistical inference transferring the moon illusion into an fMRI suitable set- were performed using the Statistical Parametric Mapping ting but also to relate the neural correlates underlying the software (Friston et al., 1994). The first four images were moon illusion to those linked to different levels of object excluded from the analysis, as these were acquired within size perception. the time period the MR signal needs to reach a steady To identify the neural network underlying the moon state. Images were spatially realigned to the fifth volume illusion and to delineate different levels of visual pro- to correct for interscan movement. Then the mean EPI cessing represented within such a network, two fMRI image for each participant was computed and spatially experiments were performed: The first experiment di- normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) rectly investigated the neural activity related to the moon template using the “unified segmentation” function in illusion by presenting computer-generated (binocular) SPM5 (Ashburner & Friston, 2005). The data were then 3-D stimuli of a virtual moon at two different positions smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm FWHM. (low or high), either within a natural scene or on a neu- tral background resulting in a two factorial design (see Figure 1A). Brain regions underlying the moon illusion EXPERIMENT 1: MOON ILLUSION were expected to be revealed by an interaction between the factors moon position (low vs. high) and scene (with Stimuli or without). More specifically, the perceived size of the Visual stimuli were presented using an MR-compatible virtual moon was expected to be increased only at the 3-D goggle system (VisuaStim, Resonance Technology, lower position in combination with the presence of a Northridge, CA). The device consists of two thin-film tran- scene. sistor (TFT) displays with a screen diagonal of 37.5° visual A second experiment was performed to test whether angle. The stimuli that covered the whole display were the moon illusion is generated by brain regions that form presented as disparate images on the two TFT displays of size-invariant (linear) object representations by taking the binocular goggles and hence generated a marked into account retinal (angular) size and perceived distance. depth impression. The stimulus background consisted Such brain regions were identified by means of fMRI of either an artificial scene (henceforth “with scene”)or adaptation (Grill-Spector & Malach, 2001). Participants alternatively of a plain blue screen (henceforth “without saw spheres located in different depth planes, with either scene”; see Figure 1A). Orthogonal to the scene variable, constant or variable perceived size (see Figure 1B). The a small moon (0.83° visual angle) appeared at one of the latter was realized by altering the perceived distance be- two possible positions (low or high). Hence, the experi- tween observer and object while simultaneously keeping ment was based on a 2 × 2 factorial design with Scene the objectʼs retinal (angular) size constant. (with vs. without) and Moon Position (low vs. high) as within-subject variables. A behavioral pilot experiment (data not shown) had confirmed that this setting is appro- METHODS priate for reliably inducing the moon illusion. Throughout this fMRI experiment, two white bars were Participants presented continuously on the left and on the right margin Twenty-five healthy participants (12 women) participated of the screen, which served as background for a compari- in a single fMRI session consisting of two separate experi- son disk that was superimposed either on the left or on ments. All participants were paid for their participation the right bar. Its vertical position was set between the and gave informed consent before the

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    12 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us