Sex Pheromone Signal and Stability Covary with Fitness 2 3 Thomas Blankers1*, Rik Lievers*1, Camila Plata1, Michiel Van Wijk1, Dennis Van 4 Veldhuizen1 & Astrid T

Sex Pheromone Signal and Stability Covary with Fitness 2 3 Thomas Blankers1*, Rik Lievers*1, Camila Plata1, Michiel Van Wijk1, Dennis Van 4 Veldhuizen1 & Astrid T

bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.05.429875; this version posted February 6, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC 4.0 International license. 1 Sex pheromone signal and stability covary with fitness 2 3 Thomas Blankers1*, Rik Lievers*1, Camila Plata1, Michiel van Wijk1, Dennis van 4 Veldhuizen1 & Astrid T. Groot1,2. 5 6 1 Institute for Biodiversity and Ecosystem Dynamics, University of Amsterdam, Science Park 7 904, Amsterdam, the Netherlands 8 2 Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology, Hans-Knöll-Strasse 8, Jena, Germany 9 * These authors contributed equally 10 11 Author for correspondence: Thomas Blankers, [email protected] 12 13 14 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.05.429875; this version posted February 6, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC 4.0 International license. 15 ABSTRACT 16 If sexual signals are costly to produce or maintain, covariance between signal expression and 17 fitness is expected. This signal-fitness covariance is important evolutionarily, because it can 18 contribute to the maintenance of genetic variation in signal traits, despite selection from mate 19 preferences. Chemical signals, such as moth sex pheromones, have traditionally been assumed to 20 be stereotypical species-recognition signals, but their relationship with fitness is unclear. Here 21 we test the hypothesis that for chemical signals that are primarily used for conspecific mate 22 finding, there is covariation between signal properties and fitness in the noctuid moth Heliothis 23 subflexa. Additionally, as moth signals are synthesized de novo every night throughout the 24 female's reproductive life, the maintenance of the signal can be costly. Therefore, we also 25 hypothesized that fitness covaries with signal stability (i.e. the lack of intra-individual variation 26 over time). We measured among- and within-individual variation in pheromone amount and 27 composition as well as fecundity, fertility, and fitness in two independent groups of females that 28 differed in the time in between two consecutive pheromone samples. In both groups, we found 29 reproductive success and longevity to be correlated with pheromone amount, composition, and 30 stability, supporting both our hypotheses. This study is the first to report a correlation between 31 fitness and sex pheromone composition in moths, solidifying previous indications of condition- 32 dependent moth pheromones and highlighting how signal-fitness covariance may contribute to 33 heritable variation in chemical signals both among and within individuals. 34 Keywords: sexual communication, fitness, trade-offs, sex pheromone, Lepidoptera 35 36 INTRODUCTION 37 Many sexually reproducing organisms discriminate among potential mates. By selecting a mate, 38 choosing individuals may receive direct benefits, e.g. protection or nutrients, and indirect 39 benefits, e.g. by receiving 'good' genes which result in more viable or sexy offspring (1–5). Mate 40 choice commonly occurs through sexual signals, and variation in reproductive success across 41 individuals producing different sexual signals is the basis of sexual selection (3,6,7). 42 In general, signals under sexual selection are subject to directional selection, as those individuals 43 are chosen that confer the highest direct or indirect benefits. Additionally, in many organisms, bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.05.429875; this version posted February 6, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC 4.0 International license. 44 sexual signals are used to localize potentially suitable, conspecific mates. These so-called species 45 recognition signals are often under stabilizing selection, because variation in these signals 46 renders them less reliable. Both directional and stabilizing selection are expected to erode genetic 47 variation (8,9). However, sexual signals are shown to have high levels of genetic variance 48 (10,11) and many observations indicate that sexual signals evolve rapidly, diverge early on 49 during speciation, and are important barriers to gene flow among closely related species (6,12– 50 15). To understand how sexual signals evolve, it is important to understand how (genetic) 51 variation in these signals is maintained. 52 If signals are costly to produce or maintain, their expression and their composition are expected 53 to be correlated with fitness (5,16). Negative correlations between signal and fitness indicate that 54 signal investment trades off with fitness (one-trait trade-offs sensu (17)). Positive correlations 55 between signal expression and fitness are expected when only high-quality senders are able to 56 bear the cost of the signal (two-trait trade-offs sensu (17) and indicate that the signal is 57 condition-dependent (18). Covariation between sexual signal variation and fitness can maintain 58 genetic variation in sexual signals, even in the face of selection (19–22). This is because fitness 59 is the result of the combined effect of many different traits and thus controlled by many different 60 loci (23). 61 Signal-fitness covariance has been studied mostly in species with acoustic and visual signals, 62 while chemical signals have received much less attention (24,25). This may be because chemical 63 signals, such a sex pheromones, are generally assumed to be biosynthetically cheap (24,26,27) 64 and have traditionally been assumed to be independent of signaler quality (24,28). Specifically, 65 moth sex pheromones have typically been considered as species-recognition signals (29,30) and 66 empirical evidence suggests moth pheromone signal composition is under stabilizing selection 67 (31–35). However, sexual signals probably do not function solely as species recognition or mate 68 choice signals, but rather range along a continuum (20,36,37). This idea is supported by 69 empirical studies that showed a relationship between nutrition and pheromone amount (38), body 70 size and sex pheromone amount (39), and body size and sex pheromone composition (40) in 71 moths, and between nutritional state, age, and parasite load and pheromone composition in 72 beetles (41). However, even though moth sex pheromones have been studied extensively in the 73 past forty to fifty years and the sex pheromone of > 2000 moth species has been identified, very bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.05.429875; this version posted February 6, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC 4.0 International license. 74 little is known about the relationship between signal variation and fitness or about variation 75 within individuals. Moreover, we lack empirical insight into the relationship between sexual 76 signal variation and fitness for chemical mate attraction in general. 77 Here, we tested the hypothesis that even for stereotypical species-recognition signals, there is 78 covariation between sexual signal composition and fitness. Since many sexual signals need to be 79 maintained throughout an individual’s reproductive lifetime, and maintenance likely requires 80 continuous investment, we also hypothesized that the ability of an individual to maintain its 81 signal covaries with fitness as well. We tested these hypotheses for the female sex pheromone in 82 the noctuid moth Heliothis subflexa. Specifically, we examined how sex pheromone signaling 83 activity (calling activity from hereon), and pheromone amount and composition changed over 84 time. 85 In moths, older (virgin) females tend to have reduced mating activity and reduced mating success 86 (42–48), and virgin female moths generally keep investing in signaling (49). Thus, prolonged 87 virginity is expected to trigger physiological responses that modulate resource allocation 88 between somatic maintenance and reproduction. We assessed these trade-offs by comparing a 89 biologically realistic delay to first mating (3 days) with an extreme case of prolonging female 90 virginity (8 days), here referred to as ‘early’ and ‘late’ maters, respectively. We then asked 91 whether a) the composition and amount of the pheromone signal in early and later maters 92 covaried with fitness, and b) the stability (within-individual variation) of the pheromone during 93 prolonged virginity was correlated with fitness. We addressed these questions separately in the 94 early and in the late maters to explore the robustness of our findings to a) variation in age and b) 95 the time span over which intra-individual variation was measured. We expected that high-fitness 96 females produced more pheromone compared to low-fitness females, and that maintaining high 97 pheromone amounts and stable pheromone composition trades off with fitness. 98 99 METHODS 100 Insects 101 The laboratory population of H. subflexa originated from North Carolina State University and 102 has been reared at the University of Amsterdam since 2011, with occasional exchange between bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.05.429875; this version posted February 6, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC 4.0 International license. 103 NCSU, Amsterdam, and the Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology in Jena to maintain 104 genetic diversity. The rearing was kept at 25°C and 60% relative humidity with 14h:10h light- 105 dark cycle.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    25 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us