Preregistration: a Pragmatic Tool to Reduce Bias and Calibrate Confidence in Scientific Research

Preregistration: a Pragmatic Tool to Reduce Bias and Calibrate Confidence in Scientific Research

PERSPECTIVE Preregistration: A pragmatic tool to reduce bias and calibrate confidence in scientific research Tom E. Hardwicke* & Eric-Jan Wagenmakers# Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam Scientific research is performed by fallible humans. Degrees of freedom in the construction and selection of evidence and hypotheses grant scientists considerable latitude to obtain study outcomes that align more with their preferences than is warranted. This creates a risk of bias and can lead to scientists fooling themselves and fooling others. Preregistration involves archiving study information (e.g., hypotheses, methods, and analyses) in a public registry before data are inspected. This offers two potential benefits: (1) reduce bias by ensuring that research decisions are made independently of study outcomes; and (2) calibrate confidence in research by transparently communicating information about a study’s risk of bias. In this article, we briefly review the historical evolution of preregistration in medicine, psychology, and other domains, clarify its pragmatic functions, discuss relevant meta-research, and provide recommendations for scientists and journal editors. * [email protected] ORCID: 0000-0001-9485-4952 # [email protected] ORCID: 0000-0003-1596-1034 “The mind lingers with pleasure upon the facts that fall happily into the embrace of the theory...There springs up...an unconscious pressing of the theory to make it fit the facts, and a pressing of the facts to make them fit the theory.” — Chamberlin (1890/1965)1 “We are human after all.” — Daft Punk (2005)2 1. Introduction Science is a human endeavour and humans are fallible. Scientists’ cognitive limitations and self-interested motivations can infuse bias into the research process, undermining the production of reliable knowledge (Box 1)3–6. The contemporary scientific ecosystem, in which research is funded, conducted, and disseminated, actively perpetuates bias by primarily rewarding scientists based on the nature of their findings rather than the quality of their methods7–9. Biased research is wasteful10, perpetuates falsehoods11, and can lead to applied interventions that are premature, ineffective, or even harmful12,13. There is concern in many fields that scientists are fooling themselves, and fooling each other, at a frequency that is unacceptably high14–18. These problems notwithstanding, the obvious success of science — from vaccines to Mars rovers — is a testament to the potential of scientific methods. A key contributor to this success is the ongoing development of tools that help scientists to learn about the world without fooling themselves19. For example, statistical tools help to differentiate signals from noise, randomisation techniques help to isolate causal mechanisms, and placebos help to control for participant reactivity. There is growing interest in a tool called preregistration which involves researchers archiving study information (e.g., aims/hypotheses, methods, and analyses) in a public registry before data are inspected20,21. Preregistration has two core functions: (1) reduce bias by constraining researcher degrees of freedom in the analysis and explanation of study outcomes; and (2) calibrate confidence in a study’s results and conclusions by transparently communicating information about risk of bias. In this article, we briefly review the historical evolution of preregistration in medicine, psychology, and other domains, clarify its pragmatic functions, discuss relevant meta-research, and provide recommendations for scientists and journal editors. Box 1. Human after all: Cognitive bias and skewed incentives The storybook image of the scientist as an objective, rational, and dispassionate arbiter of truth is apparently pervasive amongst both lay people and scientists themselves22. But despite these pretensions, scientists have egos, career ambitions, and rent to pay; we are human, after all. Any intrinsic motivation toward the pure pursuit of knowledge may be undermined by extrinsic motivations towards producing the most fundable or publishable research findings. Currently, the allocation of funding, awards, and publication prestige predominantly reward scientists based on their research findings being impressive over being right7–9. Typically, this manifests as a preference for novel, positive, and ‘statistically significant’ findings over incremental, negative, or null findings7,23–25. There is additional pressure to produce articles with concise, coherent, and compelling narratives, encouraging selective reporting of research methods and results23,24,26 in order to hide the messy realities of scientific inquiry beneath a veneer of artificial perfection27. Psychological research has documented an array of cognitive biases that can create systematic errors in reasoning and knowledge updating. Even appropriate incentives, expertise, and good intentions may not be sufficient to overcome the influence of cognitive biases and there is some (though limited) evidence of their impact in scientific contexts28–31. Human fallibility highlights the need for intellectual humility32 and transparency to enable our peers to properly evaluate our research33. Relevant examples of cognitive bias include: Confirmation bias. A tendency to preferentially seek out, evaluate, and recall information that supports one’s existing beliefs34–36 (as depicted in the opening quotation from Chamberlin). Motivated reasoning. Generating post-hoc rationalizations that frame previous decisions in a favourable light37. Hindsight bias. A tendency to think past events had a higher likelihood of occurring relative to our actual prior predictions of the same events (“I knew it all along”)38,39. Apophenia. A tendency to identify seemingly meaningful patterns in random data40. Bias blind spot. A lack of awareness about how our own decisions are influenced by bias41. 2. A brief history of preregistration Philosophers and methodologists have long debated the epistemic merits of ‘predesignating’ hypotheses before confronting them with evidence42–45. The practical idea of a study registry was contemplated intermittently in the social and behavioural sciences during the 1960s-1980s46. However, aside from a remarkable forebear of the contemporary Registered Reports format (Box 2) offered at the European Journal of Parapsychology between 1976 and 199247, early preregistration pondering lacked the necessary cultural impetus and technological infrastructure to initiate substantive changes in research practice48. In medicine, concern about publication bias prompted proposals for an international clinical trial registry in the 1980s49. ClincialTrials.gov launched in the year 2000 and subsequently clinical trial registration became a requirement for publication in major medical journals50, as well as a legal and ethical requirement in some jurisdictions51. Preregistration of other research designs, such as observational studies, has been advocated52 and contested53, and is less common54. Despite registration mandates, compliance remains a major problem, with many trials registered retrospectively or engaged in selective reporting55–61. Additionally, registrations typically do not include detailed analysis plans59,62 and often contain insufficient detail55,63–65, leaving scope for bias66. Nevertheless, editors at the BMJ67 recently described preregistration as “The single most valuable tool we have to ensure unbiased reporting of research studies”. As a multidisciplinary ‘crisis of confidence’ in research has unfolded over the last decade14–18, interest in preregistration beyond medicine has increased dramatically20, prompting debate in psychology21,68–72, political science73,74, economics75,76, cognitive modelling77,78, neuroimaging79, secondary data analysis80, qualitative research81, and beyond (Supplementary Information A). Several domain-specific registries have been established (Supplementary Information B). Registry entries72,82,83 and survey evidence84 signal increasing adoption of preregistration; however, prevalence estimates suggest that it remains uncommon overall in psychology and the social sciences (Hardwicke et al., 2021; Hardwicke et al., 2020). The contemporary Registered Reports format (Box 2) was introduced in 201387 and is currently offered by more than 278 journals, including Cortex, Royal Society Open Science, and Nature Human Behaviour88,89. Box 2. Complementary tools and extensions Several tools and extensions may enhance preregistration90,91. As these tools can introduce researcher degrees of freedom and do not protect against selective reporting bias, they should ideally complement rather than replace preregistration. Robustness checks. Whilst preregistration aims to constrain researcher degrees of freedom, robustness checks directly exploit them in order to evaluate their impact. Traditional sensitivity analyses may evaluate a few justifiable options for a single research decision92; however, recent approaches, variously known as “multiverse analysis”93, “vibration of effects”94, “specification curve”95, or “multimodel analysis”96, systematically assess the factorial intersection of multiple choices for multiple decisions, potentially resulting in tens of thousands of unique analysis specifications97–100. This is akin to simultaneously examining multiple cells in the array depicted in Figure 1, rather than a single prespecified cell. Some have argued that systematic robustness checks render preregistration redundant71,101. However, the subjective choice of which specifications to examine

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    48 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us