Response to Selection in Finite Locus Models with Nonadditive Effects

Response to Selection in Finite Locus Models with Nonadditive Effects

Journal of Heredity, 2017, 318–327 doi:10.1093/jhered/esw123 Original Article Advance Access publication January 12, 2017 Original Article Response to Selection in Finite Locus Models with Nonadditive Effects Hadi Esfandyari, Mark Henryon, Peer Berg, Jørn Rind Thomasen, Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jhered/article/108/3/318/2895120 by guest on 23 September 2021 Piter Bijma, and Anders Christian Sørensen From the Center for Quantitative Genetics and Genomics, Department of Molecular Biology and Genetics, Aarhus University DK-8830 Tjele, Denmark (Esfandyari, Berg, Thomasen, and Sørensen); Seges, Danish Pig Research Centre, Copenhagen, Denmark (Henryon); School of Animal Biology, University of Western Australia, Crawley, Australia (Henryon); Nordic Genetic Resource Center, Ås, Norway (Berg); VikingGenetics, Assentoft, Denmark (Thomasen); and Animal Breeding and Genomics Centre, Wageningen University, Wageningen, The Netherlands (Bijma). Address correspondence to H. Esfandyari at the address above, or e-mail: hadi.esfandyari@ mbg.au.dk. Received May 18, 2016; First decision June 7, 2016; Accepted January 2, 2017. Corresponding Editor: C Scott Baker Abstract Under the finite-locus model in the absence of mutation, the additive genetic variation is expected to decrease when directional selection is acting on a population, according to quantitative-genetic theory. However, some theoretical studies of selection suggest that the level of additive variance can be sustained or even increased when nonadditive genetic effects are present. We tested the hypothesis that finite-locus models with both additive and nonadditive genetic effects maintain more additive genetic variance (VA) and realize larger medium- to long-term genetic gains than models with only additive effects when the trait under selection is subject to truncation selection. Four genetic models that included additive, dominance, and additive-by-additive epistatic effects were simulated. The simulated genome for individuals consisted of 25 chromosomes, each with a length of 1 M. One hundred bi-allelic QTL, 4 on each chromosome, were considered. In each generation, 100 sires and 100 dams were mated, producing 5 progeny per mating. The population was selected for a single trait (h2 = 0.1) for 100 discrete generations with selection on phenotype or BLUP-EBV. VA decreased with directional truncation selection even in presence of nonadditive genetic effects. Nonadditive effects influenced long-term response to selection and among genetic models additive gene action had highest response to selection. In addition, in all genetic models, BLUP-EBV resulted in a greater fixation of favorable and unfavorable alleles and higher response than phenotypic selection. In conclusion, for the schemes we simulated, the presence of nonadditive genetic effects had little effect in changes of additive variance and VA decreased by directional selection. Subject area: Quantitative genetics and Mendelian inheritance Keywords: finite locus model, nonadditive effects, selection Dominance and epistatic effects have been detected in many experi- (VA) in populations (Goodnight 1987; Cheverud and Routman mental populations (Carlborg and Haley 2004) and these allelic 1995; Hansen and Wagner 2001; Caballero and Toro 2002; Barton interactions can influence the amount of additive genetic variance and Turelli 2004). It has been argued that epistatic variance may be © The American Genetic Association 2017. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected] 318 Journal of Heredity, 2017, Vol. 108, No. 3 319 “converted” into additive variance by genetic drift when a population population size (Robertson 1960). In the absence of new mutations, passes through a population bottleneck (Goodnight 1995; Cheverud the total response to selection is limited by the initial standing vari- and Routman 1996; Cheverud et al. 1999; López-Fanjul et al. 2002; ation. Selection acts on the variation present in the population, and Barton and Turelli 2004). However, this argument is not restricted will typically act to reduce it; the extent to which it does so depends to genetic drift. Changes in VA occur with variations in the genetic on the allele frequencies, and on the relation between genotype and background, and any process that changes allele frequencies, includ- phenotype (i.e., the “genetic architecture”). Interactions between ing selection, can change VA (Hansen and Wagner 2001). Gene inter- alleles (epistasis) are a key component of this genetic architecture, actions may also affect response to selection through a build-up of and their effect on the response to selection has been controversial. linkage disequilibrium associated with favorable gene combinations, On the one hand, artificially selected populations are usually well since parents transmit not only half of the additive effects to offspring, approximated by the infinitesimal model, which, in its simplest form, but also a quarter of the pairwise epistatic effects (A × A) and smaller assumes infinitely many loci of small and additive effect (Hill et al. fractions of higher-order interactions (Lynch and Walsh 1998). This 2008). This suggests that gene interactions do not play an important suggests that some of the linkage disequilibrium built by epistatic role. On the other hand, biology can hardly be additive, and genomes selection can be converted into response to selection (Griffing 1960). are finite. If the trait depended on a small number of additive alleles, Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jhered/article/108/3/318/2895120 by guest on 23 September 2021 The model most commonly used for genetic evaluation is the these would quickly fix contradicting the robust observation of sus- infinitesimal model. It assumes large numbers of genes affecting traits tained response to selection. One possibility is that epistasis sustains with each gene having a small additive effect (Fisher 1918). In this additive genetic variance for longer: Alleles that were initially del- model, selection does not change the allele frequency significantly at eterious or near-neutral may acquire favorable effects as the genetic any individual locus, nor does it change the genetic variance except background changes, “converting” epistatic variance into additive, for nonpermanent changes caused by gametic phase disequilibrium and so prolonging the response to selection. The question of whether (the Bulmer effect; Bulmer 1971). The assumptions of the infini- selection can generate additive genetic variance is an important one tesimal model are incorrect. In practice, there are individual genes, for a number of reasons, including the more nuanced understanding sometimes with large effects, and many genes showing dominance we are developing of the relationship between molecular variation and epistasis (Mackay, 2001a, 2001b). An alternative to the infinites- (“QTL”) and phenotypic variation, and the results of long-term selec- imal model is a finite-locus model, which can accommodate nonad- tion experiments that can show an approximately linear response to ditive inheritance. Under the finite-locus model, the additive genetic selection for a hundred generations or more (Goodnight 2015). variation is expected to decrease when directional selection is acting Previous studies for investigating effect of selection on additive on a population, according to quantitative-genetic theory (Crow and genetic variance in presence of nonadditive effects have focused on Kimura 1970; Falconer and Mackay 1996). However, some theoreti- phenotypic selection (Fuerst et al. 1997; Carter et al. 2005; Hallander cal studies of selection suggest that the level of additive variance can and Waldmann 2007). An alternative to selection based only on the be sustained or even increased when nonadditive genetic effects are phenotypic record of the individual is selection based on best linear present, in a manner similar to the action of genetic drift (Carter unbiased predictor (BLUP) of breeding value (Henderson 1975), et al. 2005; Fuerst et al. 1997). Experimental evidence for this phe- which uses records on all relatives, in addition to the individual’s own nomenon was found by Martinez et al. (2000), when they selected record, in genetic evaluation. The BLUP theory was first proposed by mice for body fat, and by Sorensen and Hill (1982), who selected Henderson in 1949 to describe the environmental and genetic trend D. melanogaster for abdominal bristle number. Furthermore, estimates in cattle and was subsequently developed (Henderson 1975). Carlborg et al. (2006) showed that epistatic interactions between 4 The main advantage of the BLUP methodology is the high level of loci mediated a considerably higher response to selection for growth accuracy of the breeding (or genetic evaluations) and has the property in chicken than predicted by a single-locus model. In a simulation to account for selection of parents in a breeding population. Hence, it study, Hallander and Waldmann (2007) investigated changes inVA in fairly accounts for the fact that some animals are from better parents the presence of nonadditive effects in a trait subjected to directional than other animals. Many simulation studies have compared selection selection. They showed that by including dominance and epistatic responses based on BLUP with those based on either phenotype or effects, VA was increased during the initial generations of selection. family index. In 2 simulation studies of a pig population, phenotypic

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    10 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us