Genetic and Environmental Influences on Spatial Reasoning: a Meta-Analysis ☆ T of Twin Studies ⁎ Michael J

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Spatial Reasoning: a Meta-Analysis ☆ T of Twin Studies ⁎ Michael J

Intelligence 73 (2019) 65–77 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Intelligence journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/intell Genetic and environmental influences on spatial reasoning: A meta-analysis ☆ T of twin studies ⁎ Michael J. King , David P. Katz, Lee A. Thompson, Brooke N. Macnamara Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH, USA ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: Behavioral genetic approaches, such as comparing monozygotic and dizygotic twins, are often used to evaluate Spatial reasoning the extent to which variations in human abilities are the result of genetic (heritable), shared environmental, and Behavioral genetics non-shared environmental factors. We conducted a meta-analysis on the twin study literature—comparing Meta-analysis monozygotic and dizygotic twins—to provide clarity and a general consensus regarding the extent to which Cognitive development genetic and environmental factors contribute to variation in spatial ability. Consistent with previous work, we Intelligence found that spatial ability is largely heritable (meta-analytic a = .61; 95% CI [.55, .66]), with non-shared en- vironmental factors having a substantial impact (meta-analytic e. = .43; 95% CI [.38, .49]), and shared en- vironmental factors having very little impact (meta-analytic c . = .07; 95% CI [.05, .10]). Moderator analyses were performed to establish if spatial ability type, sex, or age impacted the explanatory power of genetics or environmental factors. These effects did not differ significantly by sex or spatial ability type. However,the influence of shared environments did significantly differ depending on age. This result was drivenbythe youngest age group (ages 4–15) demonstrating relatively high amounts of shared environmental influence (c = .15, 95% CI [.10, .20]) compared with the other age groups (cs = .00–.07). 2 Spatial ability is a category of reasoning skills that refers to the orientation (ηp = 0.42; Gilmartin & Patton, 1984). capacity to think about objects in both two and three dimensions, and Spatial ability is also a vital part of success in many disciplines. to draw conclusions about those objects from limited information. Fields of study such as engineering, chemistry, and physics all use Spatial ability is an important area of study because of its association spatial visualization to help individuals complete tasks and solve pro- with the functioning of other high-level cognitive skills, such as logical blems. For example, chemistry students use 3D modeling kits to create reasoning, memory retrieval, verbal skills, mathematical skills, pro- physical replicas of molecular models and chemical reactions that they blem-solving, and reading (Lohman, 1996; Spearman & Jones, 1950). are retrieving from a memory of a 2D image (Pribyl & Bodner, 1987). For example, in a study investigating relationships between students' Similarly, dentistry students develop spatial mental models of the 3D spatial ability, logical thinking, and mathematical achievement, structure of teeth, which improves their ability to mentally maintain Bektasli (2006) found that logical thinking and mathematical achieve- and manipulate representations of these specific structures (Hegarty, ment were significantly correlated with spatial ability. Spatial ability is Keehner, Khooshabeh, & Montello, 2009). In both of these examples, also associated with mathematical thinking (i.e., the ability to use so- mental rotation is needed to consistently and accurately complete the phisticated strategies effectively on mathematical problems) tasks. (rs = .43–.74; see e.g., Casey, Nuttall, Pezaris, & Benbow, 1995; Kyttälä Tests of spatial ability have also been shown to predict initial per- & Lehto, 2008; Laski, et al., 2013; Mix et al., 2016; Reuhkala, 2001), formance in physics coursework (Φ = .55–.76); Kozhevnikov, Motes, & mathematic fluency (i.e., the ability to answer calculation problems Hegarty, 2007). Here, spatial visualization skills were used to solve under a time limit) (r = .74; Hart, Petrill, Thompson, & Plomin, 2009), kinematics problems by predicting the two-dimensional motion of an and non-numerical math abilities (e.g., the number line task) (r = .60; object, translating from one frame of reference to another, and inter- Tosto et al., 2014). Spatial ability has also been shown to predict geo- preting kinematics graphs (Kozhevnikov et al., 2007). As another ex- graphical skills, specifically on performance in map-use tasks involving ample, Hambrick et al. (2011) displayed that tests of spatial ability route-panning, visual search, symbol identification, and left/right predicted performance among novice geologists on a bedrock-mapping ☆ This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors ⁎ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychological Sciences, 11220 Bellflower Road, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH 44106, United States. E-mail addresses: [email protected] (M.J. King), [email protected] (D.P. Katz), [email protected] (L.A. Thompson), [email protected] (B.N. Macnamara). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2019.01.001 Received 12 June 2018; Received in revised form 12 December 2018; Accepted 3 January 2019 0160-2896/ © 2019 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. M.J. King, et al. Intelligence 73 (2019) 65–77 task (r = .28). Across all of the science, technology, engineering, and ability to find visuo-spatial patterns. Matrix reasoning is typically mathematics (STEM) fields, spatial ability plays a critical role inde- measured using a Raven's Progressive Matrices test, which is also used veloping expertise. As such, it has been suggested that spatial ability be as a non-verbal estimate of fluid intelligence. included in modern talent searches to better identify adolescents with Studies examining the etiology of spatial ability typically do not potential for STEM who are currently being missed (Wai et al., 2009). include measures representing all 3 of these spatial factors; therefore, Importantly, the likelihood of earning an advanced degree in STEM measurement invariance must be considered when comparing results areas has been shown to increase as a function of spatial ability (< 10% across studies. It is also important to consider how genes and the en- of those holding STEM PhDs were below the top quartile in spatial vironment might differentially influence individual variation in eachof ability during adolescence), even while controlling for intelligence (Wai the spatial factors. In the current study, we test type of spatial ability as et al., 2009). a potential moderator, to understand whether the contributions be- The positive associations between spatial abilities and cognitive and tween gene and environment differ between mental rotation, spatial academic skills are well-known. However less well known are the visualization, and matrix reasoning. sources of variation in spatial abilities. Investigating the extent to which genetic and environmental factors contribute to variation in spatial 2. Sex differences ability provides insight into the sources of these individual differences. Twin studies allow us to examine these two contributions. However, the Variability in spatial ability has been evident in research for more twin study literature on spatial reasoning abilities reports highly vari- than a century. Most notably, group differences in males compared to able estimates of genetic and environmental influences on spatial females often acknowledge a male advantage in spatial skills (Battista, abilities. For example, many studies have found additive genetic in- 1990; Casey, Nuttall, & Pezaris, 2001; Geary, Hamson, & Hoard, 2000; fluences accounting for more variability in spatial reasoning ability as Keig et al., 1993; Voyer et al., 1995). Mental rotation ability specifically compared to environmental influences 2(a = .58–.94) (Chow, Epp, has been consistently shown to produce one of the largest and most Lieblich, Barha, & Galea, 2013; Johnson & Bouchard, 2007; Johnson consistent sex differences, in favor of males, in the cognitive literature et al., 2007; Mosing, Madison, Pedersen, Kuja-Halkola, & Ullén, 2014; (Parsons et al., 2004; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Vandenberg and Kuse, Reynolds, Finkel, Gatz, & Pedersen, 2002; Shakeshaft & Plomin, 2015; 1979; Voyer et al., 1995). For example, performance on the Mental Tucker-Drob, Briley, Engelhardt, Mann, & Harden, 2016). There is also Rotation Task has resulted in significant mean sex differences a considerable amount of research that has found additive genetic in- (d = 0.90, p = .04) (Parsons et al., 2004). Additionally, Voyer et al. fluences, while considerable, account for less variability in spatial (1995) found a large effect size for sex differences in mental rotation reasoning ability as compared to environmental influences tasks (d = 0.56, p < .05). Though this difference does not typically (a2 = .12–.32) (Malanchini et al., 2016; McClearn et al., 1997; Mosing appear in matrix reasoning tasks (Eysenck, 1981; Court, 1983; Lynn, et al., 2012; Petrill et al., 1998; Tosto et al., 2014; Tucker-Drob, 1994; Mackintosh, 1996; Jensen, 1998; Lynn, 1998, 1999). Reynolds, Finkel, & Pedersen, 2014). Thus, a meta-analysis is needed to The etiology of sex differences has been a persistent topic of dis- synthesize the twin study findings and examine potential sources of cussion in psychological research. Some of the early findings of the heterogeneity. male advantage were explained in terms of underlying biological (ge- The

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    13 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us