Lower Social Class Does Not (Always) Mean Greater Interdependence: Women in Poverty Have Fewer Social Resources Than Working-Cla

Lower Social Class Does Not (Always) Mean Greater Interdependence: Women in Poverty Have Fewer Social Resources Than Working-Cla

JCCXXX10.1177/0022022114534768Journal of Cross-Cultural PsychologyStephens et al. 534768research-article2014 Article Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 2014, Vol. 45(7) 1061 –1073 Lower Social Class Does © The Author(s) 2014 Reprints and permissions: Not (Always) Mean Greater sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0022022114534768 Interdependence: Women in jccp.sagepub.com Poverty Have Fewer Social Resources Than Working-Class Women Nicole M. Stephens1, Jessica S. Cameron2, and Sarah S. M. Townsend3 Abstract Social resources (i.e., number and nature of relationships with family and friends) are an important, yet largely unrecognized, feature of the sociocultural contexts of social class that influence psychological functioning. To assess the nature and content of social resources, we conducted semistructured interviews with American women living in poverty (n = 21) and working-class (n = 31) contexts. In contrast to previous research, which demonstrates that lower social class contexts foster greater social connection and interdependence than middle- class or upper-class contexts, this study revealed that poverty constitutes a clear cutoff point at which reduced material resources no longer predict higher levels of social connection, but instead social isolation. Our interview data revealed that women in poverty had fewer connections to family and friends, experienced greater difficulty with trust, and reported more challenges involving relationships compared with working-class women. These findings extend psychological theories regarding how social class shapes psychological functioning and have important implications for understanding the maintenance and reproduction of poverty. Keywords social class, poverty, culture, self, inequality, relationships, social integration An emerging body of literature in cultural psychology suggests that social class is a form of cul- ture (D. Cohen, 2009; Fiske & Markus, 2012; Stephens, Markus, & Phillips, 2014). In other words, social class involves ongoing participation in particular sociocultural contexts—socially and historically constructed environments that contain culture-specific ideas, practices, and insti- tutions (Stephens & Townsend, 2013). The material and social conditions of the social class 1Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA 2University of California at Berkeley, USA 3University of Southern California, Los Angeles, USA Corresponding Author: Nicole M. Stephens, Kellogg School of Management, Northwestern University, Jacobs Center, 394, 2001 Sheridan Road, Evanston, IL 60208, USA. Email: [email protected] Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com by guest on July 3, 2014 1062 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 45(7) contexts that people inhabit are important because they afford culture-specific selves and patterns of thinking, feeling, and acting (Markus & Conner, 2013; Stephens, Markus, & Fryberg, 2012); for example, people in different social class contexts provide different answers to fundamental questions such as, “Who am I?” and “How should someone like me act?” Social Class in Social and Cultural Psychology Emerging social psychological theories of social class tend to assume a linear relationship between social class differences and the type of self that one is likely to become. Specifically, increased levels of rank in the social hierarchy and material resources associated with higher social class are thought to afford higher levels of independence and separation from others (cf. Kraus & Stephens, 2012; Kraus, Piff, Mendoza-Denton, Rheinschmidt, & Keltner, 2012; Stephens, Fryberg, & Markus, 2011; Stephens, Markus, & Townsend, 2007). Conversely, decreased levels of rank and material resources associated with lower social class are thought to afford higher levels of interdependence with and connection to others. These social psychologi- cal theories predict that, on average, individuals in working-class contexts (with relatively lower rank and fewer material resources) should be more relationally oriented and socially connected than individuals in middle-class or upper-class contexts. Extending this logic, individuals in pov- erty should be even more relationally oriented than those in working-class contexts. Indeed, the cultural and social psychology literature supports the assumption of a linear rela- tionship between social class and the self. A growing cultural psychology literature on social class reveals that individuals in lower social class contexts tend to prioritize relationships and community over more individually oriented goals (Piff, Stancato, Martinez, Kraus, & Keltner, 2012; Stephens, Fryberg, Markus, Johnson, & Covarrubias, 2012); are more attentive and respon- sive to others’ preferences (Stephens et al., 2007; Stephens et al., 2011); and exhibit more con- textual, rather than dispositional, patterns of attribution (Grossmann & Varnum, 2011; Kraus, Piff, & Keltner, 2009). Dovetailing with this literature, the social psychology literature on power and status reveals that individuals assigned to a high power role or asked to imagine having power in a single situation view themselves as more independent from others and also display behaviors that are often aligned with independent cultural norms (cf. Lee & Tiedens, 2001). Specifically, they show greater confidence, attend less to others and to the social context, are more emotionally expressive, and focus more on pursuing their own goals (Anderson & Berdahl, 2002; Gruenfeld, Inesi, Magee, & Galinsky, 2008; Guinote, 2008). While these studies consistently suggest that rank and resources associated with social class will have a linear relationship with levels of independence or interdependence, we propose that the actual relationship is more nuanced. Many studies in psychology inadvertently include a restricted range of social class, comparing individuals from middle- or upper-class contexts with those from working-class contexts. Moreover, given the field’s reliance on college student sam- ples (Sears, 1986), individuals who lack the material resources to gain admission to an institution of higher education are necessarily undersampled (Carnevale & Rose, 2004; Stevens, 2009). Tellingly, research in fields such as sociology, which incorporate a broader range of social classes (i.e., from poverty to upper-class), points to a different conclusion: Lower social class may foster social isolation (e.g., Briggs, 2005; Massey & Denton, 1993; Rankin & Quane, 2000; Smith, 2010; Steele & Sherman, 1999; Wilson, 1987, 1996, 2009). The Current Research Given this apparent contradiction, the following question emerges: At what point do fewer mate- rial resources and lower rank promote social isolation as opposed to social integration? To gain traction on this question, we conducted a series of in-depth, semistructured interviews with women in poverty—who have not yet been studied in the cultural psychological literature—and Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com by guest on July 3, 2014 Stephens et al. 1063 compared them with women in working-class contexts. We sought to identify and characterize the nature and content of social relationships among individuals in these distinct social class contexts. In doing so, we challenged the assumption in psychological literature that interdepen- dence necessarily decreases as material resources and rank increase. We focused broadly on indicators of social integration (Fothergill et al., 2010) and the mean- ings that people ascribe to their social relationships. First, we used closed-ended survey questions to measure social integration. Specifically, we assessed how attached our participants were to society by considering the number of informal ties (i.e., connections to family and friends) avail- able to them (see reviews by Berkman, Glass, Brissette, & Seeman, 2000; S. Cohen, 2004). As an additional measure of social integration, we also considered participants’ implicit orientation to these social relationships. Second, to probe the meaning and quality of their relationships, we asked participants open-ended interview questions and analyzed the descriptions of their rela- tionships. Specifically, we measured (a) their levels of interdependence as indicated by the extent to which participants both trusted in others and felt that trust was a relevant concept in their social lives, and (b) the extent to which they viewed relationships as involving support as opposed to challenges. To refer to these diverse indicators of social integration and relationship meaning and quality, we use the term social resources. We propose that the association between social class and social resources is not a simple linear relationship, whereby fewer material resources and lower rank necessarily contribute to greater social connection. Instead, we suggest that the circumstances of poverty constitute a critical point at which reduced material resources no longer foster social integration and interdependence with others, but rather promote a state of social isolation. Extending previous cultural psychological research on social class, we provide an in-depth analysis of social resources in the context of pov- erty. The current study seeks to unpack the contradictions of prior research by examining the nature, content, and meaning of social resources for individuals in poverty compared with those who have greater material resources and rank in society—namely, individuals in working-class contexts. We conducted our interviews at participants’ residences or in public locations

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    13 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us