Serotonin Transporter Polyadenylation Polymorphism Modulates the Retention of Fear Extinction Memory

Serotonin Transporter Polyadenylation Polymorphism Modulates the Retention of Fear Extinction Memory

Serotonin transporter polyadenylation polymorphism modulates the retention of fear extinction memory Catherine A. Hartleya,1, Morgan C. McKennab, Rabia Salmana, Andrew Holmesc, B. J. Caseyd, Elizabeth A. Phelpsa,e, and Charles E. Glattb,1 aDepartment of Psychology and eCenter for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003; bDepartment of Psychiatry and dThe Sackler Institute for Developmental Psychobiology, Weill Cornell Medical College, New York, NY 10065; and cLaboratory of Behavioral and Genomic Neuroscience, National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, Bethesda, MD 20852-9411 Edited by Michael Posner, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR, and approved February 21, 2012 (received for review February 3, 2012) Growing evidence suggests serotonin’s role in anxiety and depres- of extinction memory may underlie serotonin’s effects on anxiety sion is mediated by its effects on learned fear associations. Pharma- and depression. cological and genetic manipulations of serotonin signaling in mice Research in animal models provides strong evidence for this alter the retention of fear extinction learning, which is inversely hypothesis. Genetic knockout of the serotonin transporter (5- associated with anxious temperament in mice and humans. Here, HTT) increases anxiety and depression-related behavior in the we test whether genetic variation in serotonin signaling in the form mouse (12). Furthermore, during fear conditioning, 5-HTT knock- of a common human serotonin transporter polyadenylation poly- out mice show normal acquisition and initial extinction learning, morphism (STPP/rs3813034) is associated with spontaneous fear but exhibit a selective deficit in extinction recall (13, 14) accom- recovery after extinction. We show that the risk allele of this poly- panied by abnormal neuronal morphology in regions that support morphism is associated with impaired retention of fear extinction extinction retention (9, 13). These data suggest that 5-HTT down- memory and heightened anxiety and depressive symptoms. These regulation impairs extinction retention, resulting in persistent fear STPP associations in humans mirror the phenotypic effects of sero- memories that may contribute to anxiety and depression. However, tonin transporter knockout in mice, highlighting the STPP as a no study has yet examined whether normal variation in seroto- potential genetic locus underlying interindividual differences in se- nergic function modulates extinction memory in humans. rotonin transporter function in humans. Furthermore, we show The 5-HTT is expressed as two mRNA species that differ in that the serotonin transporter polyadenylation profile associated the use of two polyadenylation signals (15, 16). Polydenylation with the STPP risk allele is altered through the chronic administra- is a posttranscriptional modification of the 3′ end of the tran- tion of fluoxetine, a treatment that also facilitates retention of script that occurs in the majority of protein-coding mRNAs. extinction learning. The propensity to form persistent fear associ- Alternative polyadenylation forms are common in the brain and ations due to poor extinction recall may be an intermediate pheno- can lead to diversity in the regulation of gene expression (17). type mediating the effects of genetic variation in serotonergic The two reported 5-HTT polyadenylation forms differ by a 123- function on anxiety and depression. The consistency and specificity bp element, and the human gene contains a common T/G single of these data across species provide robust support for this hypoth- nucleotide polymorphism (rs3813034) in the more distal of the esis and suggest that the little-studied STPP may be an important two polyadenylation signals. We have termed rs3813034 the se- risk factor for mood and anxiety disorders in humans. rotonin transporter polyadenylation polymorphism (STPP) be- cause it alters the use of the polyadenylation signal in which it erotonin is implicated in the etiology of anxiety and de- occurs, influencing the balance of the two polyadenylation forms Spression; however, its precise role in these disorders is unclear. in the brain (15, 16). G-allele carriers have a reduced fraction of An early “chemical hypothesis” that depression stems from a se- 5-HTT mRNA containing the distal polyadenylation sequence rotonin deficiency was belied by observations that reducing se- (distal polyadenylation fraction; DPF), and also exhibit increased rotonin levels does not induce depression in healthy individuals risk for panic disorder (15). Of functional interest, the distal (1) and that inhibition of serotonin reuptake fails to yield rapid polyadenylation sequence element is positively correlated with antidepressant effects (2). These findings suggest that necessary the steady-state level of 5-HTT mRNA (15) and occurs adjacent intervening processes mediate serotonin’s role in anxiety and to a microRNA (miR-16) binding site (18), suggesting that depression. The “network hypothesis” proposes that serotonin binding of regulatory proteins to the distal sequence element fosters neural plasticity that supports adaptive processing of af- may modulate miR-16 binding (19) and alter 5-HTT translation fective information (3). Under this view, anxiety and depression (18), as has been described in miR-16–mediated regulation of stem from aberrant integration of salient environmental in- cyclooxygenase 2 gene expression. formation due to altered serotonergic function. Consistent with Here, we genotyped participants (SI Appendix,TableS1)in this notion, a prominent theory posits the dysregulation of learned a two-day fear conditioning paradigm (Fig. 1A) for the STPP to fear associations as a central underlying mechanism by which determine whether genetic variation in serotonergic function might serotonin contributes to mood and anxiety disorders (4). influence extinction retention. During fear acquisition, a colored Extinction learning is a primary means of regulating conditioned square (the conditioned stimulus +, or CS+) was paired with fear responses (5). During extinction, fear expression decreases, reflecting new learning that a once-threatening stimulus now sig- NEUROSCIENCE nals safety. The efficacy of extinction at reducing fear depends on Author contributions: C.A.H., B.J.C., E.A.P., and C.E.G. designed research; C.A.H., M.C.M., the ability to retrieve an extinction memory upon subsequent R.S., and C.E.G. performed research; C.A.H. and C.E.G. analyzed data; A.H. contributed previously published data in the 5-HTT knockout mouse; and C.A.H., A.H., B.J.C., E.A.P., encounters with a conditioned stimulus. However, failure to recall and C.E.G. wrote the paper. extinction learning may occur with the passage of time, resulting in The authors declare no conflict of interest. “ ” spontaneous recovery of the original fear memory (6). Research This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. suggests that the degree of extinction retention is a relatively stable 1 To whom correspondence may be addressed. E-mail: [email protected] or ceg2004@med. individual trait with corresponding neurobiological substrates (7– cornell.edu. 9). Recent reports in humans that spontaneous recovery of fear is This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10. associated with heightened anxiety (10, 11) suggest that modulation 1073/pnas.1202044109/-/DCSupplemental. www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1202044109 PNAS | April 3, 2012 | vol. 109 | no. 14 | 5493–5498 Downloaded by guest on September 25, 2021 from late acquisition to late extinction (t = 4.868, df = 109, P = 0.0000038). As a group, participants showed modest spontaneous fear recovery (differential CR from the first block extinction recall minus the final block of extinction) the following day (Fig. 1C) (one-sample t test; t = 1.88, df = 109, P = 0.063). To ascertain whether STPP genotype influenced fear expres- sion, we conducted linear regressions with each participant’s number of putative “risk” (G) alleles as an independent variable, and fear acquisition, extinction, and spontaneous recovery measures as dependent variables. We made the a priori prediction that the STPP G allele, which has been associated with a lower 5-HTT DPF and increased risk of panic disorder (15), would be associated with impaired extinction retention. We found that the number of G alleles showed a dose-dependent linear relationship with the magnitude of spontaneous fear recovery (β = 0.191, P = 0.046) (Fig. 2A, see SI Appendix, Fig. S1 for scatterplot). This association was unaffected by participants’ age, sex, and race (SI Appendix, Tables S2 and S3). Demonstrating the selectivity of this effect, there was no relationship between the number of G alleles Fig. 1. Fear conditioning paradigm and summary conditioned response data. and either fear acquisition (β = −0.077, P = 0.422) or extinction (A) Experimental design. (B) Summary graphs illustrating group conditioned (β = −0.105, P = 0.266) (SI Appendix, Fig. S2). This STPP asso- response (mean differential SCR to the CS+ versus the CS-) during late acqui- ciation is consistent with the selective effect of 5-HTT knockout in sition (final two blocks) and late extinction (final two blocks). Participants mice on extinction retention (13, 14) (Fig. 2B). acquired a conditioned fear response that was significantly decreased via For comparison purposes, we also genotyped samples at the extinction learning. (C) Conditioned responses increased (P = 0.063) from the fi fi highly studied 5-HTTLPR, which has been inconsistently asso-

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    6 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us