Journal of Vision (2013) 13(3):32, 1–16 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/3/32 1 Positional priming of pop-out is nested in visuospatial context Department of Psychology, # Ahu Gokce Ludwig-Maximilians-Universitat,¨ Munchen,¨ Germany $ Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universitat,¨ Munchen,¨ Germany Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck College, # Hermann J. Muller¨ University of London, UK $ Department of Psychology, # Thomas Geyer Ludwig-Maximilians-Universitat,¨ Munchen,¨ Germany $ The present study investigated facilitatory and inhibitory decades. Shore and Klein (2000) argued that memory in positional priming using a variant of Maljkovic and search is organized around three different time scales, Nakayama’s (1996) priming of pop-out task. Here, the ranging from milliseconds and seconds through min- singleton target and the distractors could be presented in utes up to hours and days. Conceptually, these effects different visuospatial contexts—but identical screen have been attributed to visual short-term memory locations—across trials, permitting positional priming (vSTM) operating within (e.g., Soto, Hodsoll, Rotsh- based on individual locations to be disentangled from tein, & Humphreys, 2008) and across (Chun & priming based on interitem configural relations. The Nakayama, 2000) search trials. The third form, of results revealed both significant facilitatory priming, i.e., longer-lasting memory effects, has been attributed to faster reaction times (RTs) to target presented at previous configural learning, such as contextual cueing (e.g., target relative to previously empty locations, and Rosenbaum & Jiang, 2013). The present study is inhibitory priming, i.e., slower RTs to target at previous distractor relative to previously empty locations. concerned with one of these mechanisms: cross-trial However, both effects were contingent on repetitions priming of item locations (Maljkovic & Nakayama, versus changes of stimulus arrangement: While 1996). Specifically, we ask whether positional priming facilitation of target locations was dependent on the in visual pop-out search is influenced by factors related repetition of the exact item configuration (e.g., T-type to the visuospatial arrangement of the search items followed by T-type stimulus arrangement), the inhibitory and, if so, whether these factors differ between priming effect was more ‘‘tolerant,’’ being influenced by of target and distractor locations. repetitions versus changes of the item’s visuospatial In visual pop-out search, observers are required to category (T-type followed by Z-type pattern; cf. Garner & detect a singleton feature target (e.g., the only red vs. Clement, 1963). The results suggest that facilitatory and green colored target, with color being the selection- inhibitory priming are distinct phenomena (Finke et al., relevant attribute) and report another target feature 2009) and that both effects are sensitive to subtle (e.g., target cut-off side left vs. right; cut-off ‘‘orienta- information about the arrangement of the display items tion’’ would be the response-defining attribute). Inter- (Geyer, Zehetleitner, & Muller,¨ 2010). The results are trial priming refers to the fact that reaction times (RTs) discussed with respect to the stage(s) of visual pop-out are faster for repeated relative to nonrepeated item search that are influenced by positional priming. attributes (see, e.g., Fecteau & Munoz, 2003; Found & Mu¨ller, 1996; Goolsby & Suzuki, 2001; Kristja´nsson & Introduction Campana, 2010; Kristja´nsson & Driver, 2008; Lamy, Antebi, Aviani, & Carmel, 2008; Maljkovic & Na- kayama, 1994, 1996; Mu¨ller, Heller, & Ziegler, 1995; Trial-based memory guided attention Olivers & Meeter, 2006). Such priming effects have been reported for several stimulus attributes, such as The role of memory in visual search has been of features (which includes target and distractor features; great interest to researchers over the last one to two e.g., Lamy, Yashar, & Ruderman, 2010), dimensions Citation: Gokce, A., Muller,¨ H. J., & Geyer, T. (2013). Positional priming of pop-out is nested in visuospatial context. Journal of Vision, 13(3):32, 1–16, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/3/32, doi:10.1167/13.3.32. doi: 10.1167/13.3.32 Received January 31, 2013; published November 26, 2013 ISSN 1534-7362 Ó 2013 ARVO Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 10/01/2021 Journal of Vision (2013) 13(3):32, 1–16 Gokce, Mu¨ller, & Geyer 2 (e.g., Found & Mu¨ller, 1996; To¨llner, Rangelov, & distractor inhibition, respectively. Further work (Malj- Mu¨ller, 2012), and locations (e.g., Geyer et al., 2010). kovic & Nakayama, 1996; experiment 3) indicated that Additionally, priming can also manifest for the search target locations are maintained in positional vSTM objects themselves (e.g., Huang, Holcombe, & Pashler, within an object-centered reference frame, in addition to 2004; Yashar & Lamy, 2011). Of particular relevance in location-specific representations. The crucial finding was the present context is Huang et al.’s (2004) report that that positional facilitation was still evident even when priming from the target’s selection-relevant feature only the target’s ‘‘relative’’ position was repeated across (size) was contingent on repetitions versus changes of trials. In this experiment, the three search items were the target’s response-relevant feature (orientation). presented in a ‘‘row’’ arrangement, with the target This led to the proposal that priming effects in visual appearing at the left, middle, or right position of the row. search reflect the operation of an episodic memory There were two conditions: ‘‘absolute-same’’ and ‘‘rela- mechanism that stores information about selection- tive-same.’’ In the absolute-same condition, the target relevant, response-relevant, and irrelevant target fea- was positioned at identical locations across trials, in tures (see also Hillstrom, 2000). At the core of this terms of both its exact (i.e., absolute) X-Y screen account is the idea that when processing the current coordinates and its relative row position. In the relative- display, observers match the target to other recently same condition, the target’s location was the same in the experienced items, particularly the target on the row, but this time the row of items was presented in a immediately preceding trial. A full match or full different display quadrant compared to the previous nonmatch can facilitate processing, whereas a partial trial. Both conditions yielded significant priming, sug- match can hinder search performance. Object-specific gesting that positional facilitation minimally requires effects are usually considered as evidence for a ‘‘late’’ that the target is presented at the relative-same location priming system, which influences processes that occur within a given visuospatial configuration. Note, though, after the selection of the target by focal attention (see, that target location priming was larger in the absolute- e.g., Olivers & Meeter, 2006, for a discussion). The same condition, indicating that at least parts of the alternative view is that priming facilitates the selection priming effects are due to location-specific, that is, of the target by focal attention (e.g., Kristja´nsson & retinotopic and/or spatiotopic, representations. Nakayama, 2003; Mu¨ller et al., 1995; To¨llner, Gra- The finding of configuration-centered facilitation was mann, Mu¨ller, Kiss, & Eimer, 2008). In recent years, a further elaborated by Geyer et al. (2010), who showed consensus has emerged that priming can aid processes that distractor locations, too, are represented in of response selection and target selection, too (Lamy et positional vSTM within an object-centered reference al., 2010; To¨llner, Gramann, Mu¨ller, Kiss, & Eimer, frame. The critical manipulation was the number of 2008; To¨llner et al., 2012; Yashar & Lamy, 2011). distractors on a given trial: In the majority of trials (91%), observers were presented with search displays that contained three-item, equilateral-triangle configu- Positional priming of pop-out rations. However, in the remaining 9% of the trials, the search displays contained only two items: one target and In Maljkovic and Nakayama’s study (1996), the one distractor (the color of the target was fixed in this search displays contained three diamond-shaped items experiment). Thus, on trials following a two-item (one target, two distractors) presented in a virtual display, the target could appear either on a previously (equilateral) triangle arrangement. The target was a color visible or empty distractor location within the triangle singleton: It was either red amongst green distractors or frame, the latter being a location where observers would green amongst red distractors. All stimuli had a notch on have expected a distractor in the majority of trials. The either the right or the left side. Observers’ task was to result was that of inhibitory priming arising from empty respond to the orientation (side) of the target notch. distractor locations (note that the inhibition effect was Maljkovic and Nakayama (1996) analyzed RTs as a comparable between conditions of occupied and empty function of the target location on the current trial relative distractor locations). The finding of inhibition arising to the previous trial(s). There were three basic intertrial from empty distractor locations led Geyer et al. (2010; transitions (amongst others). The current target (trial n) see also Geyer, Gokce, & Mu¨ller, 2011) to propose that could appear
Details
-
File Typepdf
-
Upload Time-
-
Content LanguagesEnglish
-
Upload UserAnonymous/Not logged-in
-
File Pages16 Page
-
File Size-