Hemigrapsus Oregonensis: Implications for Reproductive Success

Hemigrapsus Oregonensis: Implications for Reproductive Success

UC Davis UC Davis Previously Published Works Title Temporal variation in cannibalistic infanticide by the shore crab Hemigrapsus oregonensis: Implications for reproductive success Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/3fk7x0hq Journal Marine Ecology, 36(3) ISSN 0173-9565 Authors Miller, SH Morgan, SG Publication Date 2015 DOI 10.1111/maec.12172 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Marine Ecology. ISSN 0173-9565 SHORT COMMUNICATION Temporal variation in cannibalistic infanticide by the shore crab Hemigrapsus oregonensis: implications for reproductive success Seth H. Miller1 & Steven G. Morgan1,2 1 Bodega Marine Laboratory, University of California Davis, Bodega Bay, CA, USA 2 Department of Environmental Science and Policy, University of California, Davis, CA, USA Keywords Abstract Feeding behavior; Hemigrapsus oregonensis; larval release; reproductive output; resource Larvae of benthic marine organisms are released amid high densities of suspen- limitation. sion feeding and predatory adults and are highly subject to being consumed, even by conspecifics or their own parent. During laboratory feeding trials con- Correspondence ducted in June 2006, female shore crabs (Hemigrapsus oregonensis) from Stege Seth H. Miller, Bodega Marine Laboratory Marsh in San Francisco Bay (37°54.5300 N, 122°19.7340 W) that released their University of California Davis, 2099 Westside larvae during the previous 24 h ate fewer conspecific larvae than females that Road, Bodega Bay, CA 94923, USA. had not recently released larvae, though the behavior was not repeated during E-mail: [email protected] similar trials in 2007. Additionally, the number of larvae eaten increased with Accepted: 15 March 2014 increasing starvation time, and hungrier females showed a trend toward eating more larvae from a different species (Carcinus maenas) than larvae of conspe- doi: 10.1111/maec.12172 cifics. Thus, suppression of suspension feeding may reduce conspecific preda- tion of newly released larvae, but this response partially depends on hunger level. This is the first time crabs have been shown to suppress feeding to reduce cannibalism of larvae, and this behavior could affect reproductive success and population dynamics. 1997; Gibson 2003; Morgan & Anastasia 2008; Miller & Introduction Morgan 2013). In addition to larval defenses, adults of Tiny larvae of benthic marine invertebrates are released many species release larvae or gametes synchronously amid a ‘wall of mouths’ (Emery 1973), navigating a when predation is minimized (Johannes 1978; Morgan gauntlet of planktonic predators before settling in a suit- 1995a,b; Hovel & Morgan 1997; Rasmuson & Morgan able adult habitat. In response, many larvae and adults 2013) or predators are swamped (Harrison et al. 1984). have evolved antipredatory defenses. Some larvae possess Adults also may migrate to safer locations to release chemical defenses that decrease their palatability to pre- larvae or gametes (Johannes 1978; Warner 1988; Carr dators (Lindquist & Hay 1996 for review; Lopanik et al. et al. 2004). 2006). Other larvae, particularly those that remain near- Although it has received limited attention, suspension shore or in estuaries throughout their development, have feeding adults cannibalize their own offspring or consume evolved morphological defenses, such as spines or large their neighbors’ offspring during periods of peak release overall size (Wilson 1929; Morgan 1989, 1990), or exhibit of eggs or larvae (Crisp & Patel 1960; Egloff 1966; Young defensive behaviors, such as sinking or fleeing when con- 1988; Pechenik et al. 2004; Alfaro 2006). Facultative sus- fronted by a predator (Morgan 1987, 1995a,b). Many lar- pension feeders, such as many crab species, also exhibit vae avoid high densities of planktivorous fishes nearshore cannibalism. Later stage crab larvae consume earlier stage by occurring in seaward flowing currents that expedite larvae, postlarval crabs consume larvae in resource-lim- transport to safer waters offshore (e.g. Hovel & Morgan ited environments (Anger 1995; McConaugha 2002), and Marine Ecology (2014) 1–6 ª 2014 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 1 Cannibalistic infanticide Miller & Morgan juvenile crabs frequently cannibalize new settlers or youn- those larvae (n = 6 in 2006, n = 4 in 2007) or (ii) an ger juveniles (Moksnes et al. 1997; Heck et al. 2001; unrelated conspecific female that had released larvae Moksnes 2004; Gimenez 2010). However, the extent of within 24 h (n = 12 in 2006, n = 4 in 2007), to test for cannibalism by adult crabs on newly hatched larvae is lar- the effect of relatedness on cannibalism. In a second gely unknown (Srikrishnadhas & Ramamoorthi 1976). experiment, larvae were placed with an unrelated conspe- Unlike obligate suspension feeders, facultative suspen- cific female that released larvae within 24 h (n = 18 in sion feeders have the option of switching to a different 2006, n = 8 in 2007) or (ii) a conspecific female that had feeding mode during peak release of eggs or larvae. For not recently released larvae (n = 11 in 2006, n = 13 in example, many adult hermit and brachyuran crabs are 2007), to test for the effect of recentness of larval release capable of switching between suspension feeding and on cannibalism. their primary feeding modes when food is scarce (Schem- Feeding trials were conducted in a temperature-con- bri 1982; Depledge 1989; Whitman et al. 2001), and trolled room at 11 °C with a natural diel cycle to approx- copepods decrease cannibalism on conspecific larvae imate conditions experienced by natural populations when alternative food sources are abundant (Daan et al. releasing larvae on nocturnal ebb tides. Females were 1988). Additionally, some species decrease or stop sus- removed the next morning after 16 h, and the number of pension feeding near the time of larval release, perhaps larvae remaining in each container was recorded. The low reducing cannibalism of larvae (Thorson 1966; Brun numbers of larvae in our trials allowed us to record con- 1972). However, hungry crabs may suspension feed dur- servative estimates of cannibalism because crabs would be ing peak larval release, given that many species forage less exposed to hundreds of thousands of larvae during a nat- selectively when food is scarce (Kislalioglu & Gibson ural spawning event. The duration of the trials was cho- 1976; Pastorok 1980; Williamson 1980). sen to mimic the duration of a natural spawning event, Reproductive success and metapopulation dynamics during which numerous females in the population release should be influenced by the ability of suspension feeders larvae over the course of the night. All experimental to detect and avoid consuming their own offspring or methods were identical in both years. their neighbor’s offspring. We determined whether female We added a new experiment in 2007 to determine crabs ate newly hatched larvae of (i) their own, (ii) a whether females eat more larvae of a different species conspecific female, and (iii) a different species, as well as than of a congener and whether the results depended on whether (iv) hunger level or (v) how recently larvae were hunger level. Seventeen H. oregonensis females were held released affected these results. individually in bowls and were each fed 12 newly hatched C. maenas larvae (larvae of these two species are approxi- mately the same size). Females had not released larvae Material and Methods within 24 h to eliminate any potential effect of recent lar- We collected ovigerous and nonovigerous females of val release on suspension feeding. We conducted two tri- the yellow shore crab Hemigrapsus oregonensis from a als to determine the effect of hunger level on single population in the lower intertidal zone of Stege cannibalism. In the first trial, females were starved for Marsh in San Francisco Bay, California (37°54.5300 N, 1 day. Immediately after the first trial concluded, these 122°19.7340 W) in 2006 and 2007 at the height of their same females were starved for 22 more days and then the reproductive period in June. We also collected ovigerous experiment was repeated. Using the same crabs in both females of the European green crab Carcinus maenas from trials allowed us to eliminate any potential effect of indi- Bodega Harbor, California (38°19.0190 N, 123°02.3860 W) vidual variation on the results. All statistical comparisons in 2007. This species was introduced to San Francisco were made using arcsine-transformed data to meet the Bay in approximately 1989 and rapidly expanded its assumptions of analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the range to cover much of the West Coast (Grosholz et al. JMP 9 software package (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA). 2000). Crabs were transported to the laboratory and held individually in mesh containers (17 9 10 cm) in simu- Results lated diel (14 h light :10 h dark) and tidal cycles (timed patterns of high and low tide to ambient tidal cycles). Hemigrapsus oregonensis females that released larvae Upon hatching, all larvae were separated from the female within the previous 24 h cannibalized 32% of the larvae crab and held in labeled containers identifying their presented over both years. They ate similar percentages of parent. their own larvae and larvae of a conspecific female in Twelve newly hatched H. oregonensis larvae were added both years (ANOVA, F1,24 = 0.0022, P = 0.9633, Fig. 1), to replicate bowls (15 cm diameter 9 5 cm tall) with and there was no interaction between year and relation- clean seawater containing (i) the female

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    7 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us