Perception of Coda Voicing: Glottalisation, Vowel Duration, and Silence

Perception of Coda Voicing: Glottalisation, Vowel Duration, and Silence

PERCEPTION OF CODA VOICING: GLOTTALISATION, VOWEL DURATION, AND SILENCE Joshua Penney, Felicity Cox, and Anita Szakay Centre for Language Sciences, Department of Linguistics, Macquarie University, Australia [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected] ABSTRACT Recent studies have analysed whether listeners are sensitive to glottalisation in perception. In an eye Recent research from Australian English has shown tracking study, [3] found that American English that glottalisation of vowels preceding coda stops listeners were slower to identify words with voiced results in increased perception of coda voicelessness. coda stops when glottalisation was present, though However, the addition of glottalisation results in a they reacted similarly for words with voiceless codas shorter portion of the vowel being modally voiced, regardless of whether glottalisation was present or raising the question of whether listeners may parse absent. This suggests that listeners are sensitive to glottalisation as belonging to the coda rather than the glottalisation and associate it with voiceless, but not preceding vowel. If so, listeners would perceive a voiced, coda contexts. shorter preceding vowel duration therefore [13] conducted a perceptual task with AusE increasing the perception of coda voicelessness. listeners in which preceding vowel duration and This study thus compared listeners' coda voicing glottalisation were co-varied. They found that responses for words containing glottalised vowels listeners identified a higher proportion of voiceless with words containing vowels in which glottalisation stops when glottalisation was present, suggesting was replaced with silence. The results suggest that that glottalisation has the effect of promoting the both glottalisation and shorter vowel duration/longer perception of coda voicelessness. This effect was coda closure duration result in increased voiceless found to be stronger for inherently short vowels (e.g. percepts, but that listeners respond differently to /ɐ/ STRUT) than for inherently long vowels (e.g. /ɐː/ these two conditions. The findings indicate that START). Inherently short vowels have been shown to listeners are sensitive to glottalisation and utilise this display smaller durational differences across coda as a cue to coda voicing rather than simply voicing contexts than inherently long vowels in perceiving shorter modally voiced vowels. production [4, 12]. Therefore, the vowel duration cue to coda voicing may not be as strong in the Keywords: Glottalisation; coda stop voicing; vowel context of inherently short vowels and, accordingly, duration; closure duration. glottalisation may be a stronger cue in this context [14]. Importantly, the effect of glottalisation 1. INTRODUCTION promoting the perception of coda voicelessness was found even when listeners were presented with a Glottalisation occurs in conjunction with voiceless preceding vowel with an extended duration, which coda stops in many varieties of English [5, 6, 8, 15, would otherwise produce the percept of coda voicing 18, 19], including Australian English (AusE), where [2, 11, 17]. it has been shown to be present 55% of the time in As glottalisation necessarily results in non-modal voiceless coda stop contexts compared to 6% in phonation affecting the vowel preceding the coda voiced coda contexts [12]. Glottalisation may be stop, it may be possible that listeners in [13] did not realised by the insertion of a glottal stop prior to an parse the glottalised portion of the vowel as oral stop, though in AusE it is most often manifested belonging to the vowel. That is, listeners may have as irregular, laryngealised phonation (i.e. creaky used only the modally voiced vocalic portion in their voice) on the end of the vowel preceding a coda perception of stimulus voicing. In other words, stop. Some researchers hypothesise that listeners would have perceived a shorter vowel glottalisation is employed in association with duration when glottalisation was present compared voiceless coda stops in order to enhance the to when it was absent. As shorter preceding vowels perception of voicelessness [9, 15], as glottalisation produce the percept of voiceless coda stops [2, 11, appears to be more common when voiceless coda 17], an increase in the proportion of voiceless stops precede sonorants, an environment in which responses would be expected. In addition, if the anticipatory coarticulatory voicing is likely [8, 16]. glottalisation were interpreted as part of the coda The inclusion of glottalisation may minimise the stop closure, this too would be consistent with likelihood of anticipatory voicing. increased perception of coda voicelessness because 1863 increased closure duration is associated with manipulated to fall from 265 to 203Hz across each voiceless coda stops [7, 10]. of the vowels. We then manipulated the duration of Thus, the aim of this study is to investigate the vowels. For each of the four tokens we created a whether listeners are perceptually sensitive to continuum of nine equally spaced vowel duration glottalisation itself or whether they parse steps. The minimum and maximum durations were glottalisation as part of the closure and, determined by production values for the relevant consequently, respond to the shorter (modally vowels reported in [12] for young female AusE voiced) vowels when perceiving coda voicing. We speakers. The mean minus two standard deviations will examine whether listeners react in the same way preceding a voiceless coda was the minimum to glottalised stimuli as they do to stimuli in which duration, and the mean plus two standard deviations the glottalised portion is replaced with silence, preceding a voiced coda was the maximum duration. thereby reducing vowel duration and increasing closure duration. If listeners do parse glottalisation Figure 1: Spectrograms of ninth-step (i.e. longest as belonging to the coda and hence are sensitive to vowel duration) ‘bud’ stimuli. Upper panel shows shorter preceding vowel duration rather than control condition; middle panel shows glottalised glottalisation, we would hypothesise that the condition; lower panel shows silence condition. inclusion of both glottalisation and of silence would 5000 provide the same results. On the other hand, if 4000 listeners are perceptually sensitive to the glottalised 3000 portion of the vowel, we would expect increased 2000 perception of coda voicelessness when glottalisation Frequency (Hz) is present compared to silence. 1000 5000 Time (s) 2. METHOD 4000 2.1. Participants 3000 2000 Frequency (Hz) We enlisted 51 participants (female: 46; male: 5) to 1000 take part in this task. All were undergraduate 5000 students who received course credit for participating. Time (s) Participants were aged between 18 and 23 years 4000 (mean: 19.5) and were native AusE speakers, who 3000 were born in and completed all of their schooling in 2000 Frequency (Hz) Australia. No participant reported any speech or 1000 hearing issues. 0 0 0.732 Time (s) 2.2. Stimuli A second set of stimuli was then created for the The stimuli were created from natural speech tokens glottalised condition. This set was identical to the of the words bead, bid, bard, and bud produced by a first set of stimuli, with the exception that the final non-rhotic female native AusE speaker aged 25. portion of the vowel in each token was replaced with Recordings were made in a sound treated studio at glottalisation taken from the sustained vowels Macquarie University with an AKG C535 EB produced with laryngealised phonation, resulting in microphone and a Presonus StudioLive 16.2.4 AT a drop in F0. The final 35% of the vowel was mixer recorded to an iMac at a sampling rate of replaced with glottalisation for inherently short 44.1kHz. The speaker produced the target words vowels, and the final 25% was replaced for with modal phonation, and also produced sustained inherently long vowels, as reported for production in realisations of each of the relevant vowels with [12]. laryngealised phonation from which glottalised Finally, a third set of stimuli was produced for stimuli were created. the silence condition. This set was identical to the Cues to coda voicing were removed: release glottalised set of stimuli, except that the glottalised bursts were replaced with a perceptually ambiguous portion of the vowel (final 35% for inherently short burst taken from a low amplitude voiced coda stop vowels; final 25% for inherently long vowels) was from an unstressed syllable; F1 formant transitions replaced with silence. This also resulted in a at the end of the vowels were removed; intensity truncated F0 contour. contours were manipulated to ensure uniformity; Figure 1 illustrates the ninth step of the bud closure periods were replaced with silence; F0 was 1864 continuum in each of the three conditions (control, a cue to coda stop voicing: when vowel duration is glottalised, silence). The modally voiced portion of short (i.e. in the lower steps of the continuum), the vowel in the glottalised condition is the same listeners produce a high proportion of voiceless coda duration as the vowel in the silence conditions. Note stop responses. As vowel duration increases, so too that the overall stimulus duration remains the same does the proportion of responses for voiced coda in all three conditions. stops [13, 14]. In the glottalised condition the results also mirror what has been previously reported: 2.3. Procedure namely, that the presence of glottalisation at the

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    5 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us