Reproductive Aging and Mating: the Ticking of the Biological Clock in Female Cockroaches

Reproductive Aging and Mating: the Ticking of the Biological Clock in Female Cockroaches

Reproductive aging and mating: The ticking of the biological clock in female cockroaches Patricia J. Moore* and Allen J. Moore School of Biological Sciences, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9PT, United Kingdom Edited by David B. Wake, University of California, Berkeley, CA, and approved June 5, 2001 (received for review March 30, 2001) Females are expected to have different mating preferences be- reproductive state? Few empirical studies have addressed cause of the variation in costs and benefits of mate choice both these questions. Lea et al. (15) present evidence that the between females and within individual females over a lifetime. consistency of mate preference in midwife toads, presumably Workers have begun to look for, and find, the expected variation reflecting a high motivation to mate, is greatest in ovulating among females in expressed mating preferences. However, vari- females. Kodric-Brown and Nicoletto (16) find that older ation within females caused by changes in intrinsic influences has female guppies are less choosy than when they are younger not been examined in detail. Here we show that reproductive even if still virgin. Likewise, Gray (17) demonstrated that older aging caused by delayed mating resulted in reduced choosiness by female house crickets show no significant preference for the female Nauphoeta cinerea, a cockroach that has reproductive calls of attractive males compared with young females. cycles and gives live birth. Male willingness to mate was unaf- An essential factor in considering the effect of reproductive fected by variation in female age. Females who were beyond the state on the expression of female mate choice is to show that in optimal mating age, 6 days postadult molt, required considerably fact there is variation in the costs associated with mate choice less courtship than their younger counterparts. Females who were that are correlated with changes in reproductive state. None of older when they mated had fewer offspring per clutch and fewer these previous studies (15–17) addressed whether there is an clutches than females who mated young. Thus, reduced choosiness associated cost to maintaining mate choice. In fact, Lea et al. was correlated with a permanent reduction in fertility. There was explicitly state that there is no additional cost to maintaining no difference in overall senescence among females, and thus the choosiness for ovulating females of the Mallorcan midwife toads, reduction in clutch size did not result in the expected increased because they can hold eggs for about a week in their oviducts and lifespan. We suggest that reproductive aging in N. cinerea, similar thus can afford to be discriminating. to aging in general, occurs because the maintenance of oocytes is The study we report here addresses the question of how female costly, and selection is relaxed after the optimal mating period. Our results further suggest that selection for continued choosiness is mating preferences vary as the cost of choice changes. We show also relaxed and supports direct selection on female choosiness that in the cockroach Nauphoeta cinerea the choosiness of and a cost to choosiness. females, defined as the courtship effort females require from males before mating, changed as a function of female age aging ͉ female mate choice ͉ fitness ͉ mate quality ͉ sexual selection because of age-related changes in the reproductive system. Older females were less choosy than younger females. We also showed that the decrease in choosiness was correlated with a reduction exual selection theory predicts that mate choice, usually by in the reproductive potential of older females, providing the first Sfemales, evolves because of variation in benefits provided by evidence of varying costs of mating preferences reflecting in- mating with particular mates. Much of the recent research on trinsic changes in a female. sexual selection therefore has been directed at detecting the benefit provided by males to females (reviewed in refs. 1 and 2). N. cinerea Life History Considerably less research effort has addressed variation in N. cinerea is ovoviviparous; although no nutrients are passed female preferences (3–7). During female mate choice, however, from the mother to the developing young, females carry the females must tradeoff the benefits gained by being choosy against the costs of stronger preferences (2). This cost͞benefit developing embryos in a brood pouch until the offspring emerge tradeoff should result in variation in the expression of female as first instar nymphs (18). Females may also provide maternal mate preferences both between females and by an individual care in the form of protection to the offspring during their first female over her lifetime. instar (ref. 19; unpublished data). Thus, there is a relatively large Variation in female mate preferences can result from extrinsic maternal investment in N. cinerea. Once a female has mated, she or intrinsic factors. Examples of external conditions that may is unreceptive to males until the birth of her first clutch (20, 21). cause females to be less choosy when making mating decisions Even then, females may not remate. Maternal care further limits include environmental conditions that increase the energy out- the number of offspring a female can produce (21, 22). In put required to search for and compare different males (8) or contrast, males provide no paternal care and make only genetic high predation risk (9–11). Seasonal constraints on reproduction contributions to their offspring (22, 23). Thus, compared with can also influence female choosiness (12). female N. cinerea, males have a relatively unlimited fertilization Intrinsic factors should also influence the expression of capacity (20–23). This asymmetry in parental investment is female mate choice (13, 14). Parker’s ESS model of mate expected in species in which sexual selection, and particularly EVOLUTION choice (13) suggests that the level of choosiness is a function mate choice, operate (24). of female reproductive quality. Females with a reproductive quality above a certain threshold should only accept mates of similarly high quality. However, females with a reproductive This paper was submitted directly (Track II) to the PNAS office. quality below this threshold should be less choosy, accepting *To whom reprint requests should be addressed: 3.614 Stopford Building, School of Biological Sciences, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9PT, United lower quality males as mates. Parker’s model considers repro- Kingdom. E-mail: [email protected]. ductive quality as an intrinsic value of the female: what The publication costs of this article were defrayed in part by page charge payment. This happens if female reproductive quality changes over time? Is article must therefore be hereby marked “advertisement” in accordance with 18 U.S.C. there phenotypic plasticity in female choosiness based on §1734 solely to indicate this fact. www.pnas.org͞cgi͞doi͞10.1073͞pnas.161154598 PNAS ͉ July 31, 2001 ͉ vol. 98 ͉ no. 16 ͉ 9171–9176 Downloaded by guest on September 29, 2021 Sexual selection in N. cinerea occurs as a result of both Statistical Analyses. Standard parametric (ANOVA) and non- male–male competition and female mate choice (reviewed in ref. parametric (goodness-of-fit) tests of significance were used, 25). Preferences by females are reflected in their willingness or because our data were either normally distributed or met unwillingness to mate (25, 26), and mate choice can be inde- distribution requirements after transformation. Within pendent of male-male competition (27, 28). Further, female ANOVA, we tested a priori predictions that change is associated behavior toward single males is no different from their behavior with age by using contrast analysis (30). We tested four specific when multiple males are present, indicating that N. cinerea contrasts based on the pattern of reproductive maturation and females use a threshold mating preference (22). Female choosi- receptivity in this species, in which an increasing number of ness during courtship has been shown to reflect mating prefer- females are receptive up to 6 days postreproductive molt, but all ences and result in mate choice in N. cinerea [refs. 22, 28, and 29; females are receptive after 9 days as adults (21). Thus, in addition our definitions follow Jennions and Petrie (2) where ‘‘choice’’ to the overall ANOVA, we tested for a linear change with age, refers to the outcome of the propensity to mate, and ‘‘prefer- a deviation from linearity (quadratic change with age), signifi- ence’’ and ‘‘choosiness’’ refer to the amount of time spent cant differences between young and old ages (3, 6, and 9 differ assessing mates]. Females require significantly less courtship from 12, 15, and 18), and significant differences among three effort from preferred males regardless of the number of males groups, young, middle and old ages (3 and 6 differ from 9 and present (22, 28, 29). 12, which differ from 15 and 18). All statistical tests were performed by using SYSTAT 9.0. Materials and Methods Animal Husbandry. Late instar nymphs were isolated from mass Results colonies, separated according to sex into 17 ϫ 12 ϫ 6.5-cm Effects of Age on Mating Behavior. Only 6 of 20 females presented plastic containers, and provided with ad libitum rat chow and with males at 3 days postadult emergence mated on the first day. water. All animals were kept in incubators at 27°C on a 16͞8 Of the 14 females that did not mate on day 3, 10 mated on day light͞dark cycle. Adult females and males were isolated on the 4, one on day 5, and the remaining two females mated on the day of adult emergence, and individuals were placed into 11 ϫ sixth day postadult emergence. Of the females presented to 11 ϫ 3-cm clear plastic boxes and provided with food and water. males for the first time 6 days postemergence, 17 of 20 mated on the first day.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    6 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us