Conspicuous Visual Signals Do Not Coevolve with Increased Body Size in Marine Sea Slugs

Conspicuous Visual Signals Do Not Coevolve with Increased Body Size in Marine Sea Slugs

doi: 10.1111/jeb.12348 Conspicuous visual signals do not coevolve with increased body size in marine sea slugs K. L. CHENEY*, F. CORTESI*†,M.J.HOW‡,N.G.WILSON§,S.P.BLOMBERG*, A. E. WINTERS*, S. UMANZOR€ ¶ & N. J. MARSHALL‡ *School of Biological Sciences, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Qld, Australia †Zoological Institute, The University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland ‡Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Qld, Australia §The Australian Museum, Sydney, NSW, Australia ¶Department of Biology, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, NM, USA Keywords: Abstract animal patterns; Many taxa use conspicuous colouration to attract mates, signal chemical aposematism; defences (aposematism) or for thermoregulation. Conspicuousness is a key image statistics; feature of aposematic signals, and experimental evidence suggests that pre- nudibranchs; dators avoid conspicuous prey more readily when they exhibit larger body spectral contrast; size and/or pattern elements. Aposematic prey species may therefore evolve visual signalling. a larger body size due to predatory selection pressures, or alternatively, larger prey species may be more likely to evolve aposematic colouration. Therefore, a positive correlation between conspicuousness and body size should exist. Here, we investigated whether there was a phylogenetic correlation between the conspicuousness of animal patterns and body size using an intriguing, understudied model system to examine questions on the evolution of animal signals, namely nudibranchs (opisthobranch mol- luscs). We also used new ways to compare animal patterns quantitatively with their background habitat in terms of intensity variance and spatial frequency power spectra. In studies of aposematism, conspicuousness is usually quantified using the spectral contrast of animal colour patches against its background; however, other components of visual signals, such as pattern, luminance and spectral sensitivities of potential observers, are largely ignored. Contrary to our prediction, we found that the conspicuous- ness of body patterns in over 70 nudibranch species decreased as body size increased, indicating that crypsis was not limited to a smaller body size. Therefore, alternative selective pressures on body size and development of colour patterns, other than those inflicted by visual hunting predators, may act more strongly on the evolution of aposematism in nudibranch molluscs. aposematism, is found in a wide range of organisms Introduction including insects, snakes, molluscs, fish and amphibians Animals that contain toxic or unpalatable chemicals (Poulton, 1890; Cott, 1940; Ruxton et al., 2004). How often use conspicuous colouration and distinct body such warning or aposematic colouration evolves has patterning to communicate unprofitability to visual puzzled scientists for decades and has been limited by a hunting predators. Such warning colouration, or lack of comparative studies investigating real prey species. Conspicuousness is a key feature of warning sig- nals, as conspicuous signals are more likely to be detected Correspondence: Karen L. Cheney, School of Biological Sciences, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Qld 4072, Australia and learned by predators, and memorized for longer Tel.: +61 7 3365 7386; fax: +61 7 3365 1655; (Rothschild, 1984; Roper, 1994; Lindstrom et al., 2001; e-mail: [email protected] Aronsson & Gamberale-Stille, 2008). A conspicuous ª 2014 THE AUTHORS. J. EVOL. BIOL. 27 (2014) 676–687 676 JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª 2014 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY Conspicuousness and body size in nudibranchs 677 signal must differ from its background in colour, Materials and methods pattern and/or luminance from the perspective of the intended receiver (Endler, 1978, 1991). High contrast Study species against the background increases signal efficiency and initial wariness by predators (Roper & Cook, 1989; Nudibranchs exhibit tremendous species-level diversity Lindstrom et al., 2001; Ruxton et al., 2004), and the with over 3000 species worldwide. Our sampling cov- speed and strength of avoidance learning (Gittleman & ered representative species from infraorder Doridacea Harvey, 1980). However, no simple method exists for (families: Aegiridae, Chromodoridae, Dendrodorididae, quantifying the conspicuousness of an object against Discodoridae, Dorididae, Phyllidiidae and Polyceridae) its background (but see Endler, 2012). Furthermore, and infraorder Aeolidida (families: Glaucidae, Facelini- the relative importance of each component and how dae and Flabellinidae) (Table S1). Most nudibranchs they interact is often unclear (but see Osorio et al., contain secondary metabolites, including isocyanides, 1999; Aronsson & Gamberale-Stille, 2008). diterpenes and sesquiterpenes (Faulkner & Ghiselin, Experimental evidence suggests that an increase in 1983; Cimino et al., 1985; Avila, 1995), which protect body size and/or in pattern element size within the the animals from predatory attacks (Avila, 1995; Mollo visual display strengthens the avoidance response of et al., 2005). These chemicals are often localized in warning colouration by predators (Gamberale & Tull- selected parts of the body (Avila & Paul, 1997; Somerville berg, 1996b, 1998; Lindstrom et al., 1999; Nilsson & et al., 2006; Wagele et al., 2006) and can be diet-derived Forsman, 2003). Domestic chicks have been shown to or produced de novo (Cimino et al., 1983; Cimino & have an unlearned aversion to larger-sized insect prey Sodano, 1993; Fontana et al., 1994). Nudibranchs also (Gamberale & Tullberg, 1996a, 1998), and larger pat- range in their visual displays and include those that are tern elements in artificial prey caused blue tits to learn highly cryptic against their background habitat to those signals more rapidly and provided enhanced avoidance that exhibit bold and distinct body colouration, which of unpalatable prey (Lindstrom et al., 1999). Predator are used as aposematic signals. Although information selective pressures may therefore cause species that on the identity of potential nudibranch predators is lim- have acquired aposematic colouration to increase over- ited, predators are thought to include fish (e.g. puffer- all body size and/or increase body pattern elements rel- fish, triggerfish and wrasse) and other invertebrates such ative to body size (e.g. widening of stripes or size of as crabs, sea spiders and other opisthobranchs. How- dots). Alternatively, species with larger body size may ever, fish predators are considered to be the main selec- be more likely to evolve conspicuous colouration. If tive pressure that drives the evolution of conspicuous predator selective pressures influence the evolution of colours and patterns due to their ability to detect colour aposematic displays, we would expect to find an evolu- (Siebeck et al., 2008) and their di- or trichromatic visual tionary correlation between conspicuousness and body system (Marshall et al., 2006). We measured nudi- size of aposematic species. Indeed, a comparative analy- branch body lengths from live individuals that were sis of poison frogs (Dendrobatidae) (Hagman & Fors- collected for spectral reflectance measurements and man, 2003) indicated that body size was correlated used nudibranch identification books with detailed with an increase in conspicuous colouration, in terms body length information (Cobb & Willian, 2006; Debe- of brightness quantified by human assessment and lius & Kuiter, 2007; Coleman, 2008). We used mean computer analysis of digital colour photographs. body length from a minimum of eight individual mea- However, Nilsson & Forsman (2003) failed to find such surements for each species; measurements for juveniles a correlation in moths, but lifestyle was thought to were omitted. In this study, average body size ranged confound the results as a shift from lone behaviour to from 1.0 to 10.0 cm (Table S1). gregariousness was also partnered with a decrease in body size. Phylogenetic reconstruction In this study, we examined this hypothesis using an intriguing, understudied model system, namely nudi- We used a Bayesian inference approach to estimate the branchs (opisthobranch molluscs, commonly known as phylogenetic relationships between 76 nudibranch spe- sea slugs). To do this, we quantified the conspicuous- cies for which we were able to collect pattern and/or col- ness of animal body colouration using new ways of our data. Phylogenetic relationships were reconstructed quantifying the spatial frequency of body patterns using published COI and 16S gene sequences from Gen- (intensity variance and power spectrum analysis) and Bank (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/genbank/) for 49 compared this to spectral contrast measurements, which species. We also sequenced both genes for an additional is frequently the sole measure of conspicuousness in 16 species, COI for an additional eight species and 16S studies of animal colour patterns. We then used for an additional three species (Accession Numbers Bayesian phylogenetic regression analysis to assess how listed in Table S1). Three pleurobranch species: Pleuro- these measures of conspicuousness related to animal branchea meckeli, Bathyberthella antarctica and Thompsonia body size. antarctica were used as outgroups to root the trees. ª 2014 THE AUTHORS. J. EVOL. BIOL. 27 (2014) 676–687 JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª 2014 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY 678 K. L. CHENEY ET AL. DNA was extracted using a Qiagen DNAeasy blood (i) First- and second-order image statistics

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    12 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us