Comparative Primate Neurobiology and the Evolution of Brain Language Systems

Comparative Primate Neurobiology and the Evolution of Brain Language Systems

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Comparative primate neurobiology and the evolution of brain language systems 1,2,3,4,5 James K Rilling Human brain specializations supporting language can be our capacity for language. As discussed below, compara- identified by comparing human with non-human primate tive studies of primate neurobiology have helped to brains. Comparisons with chimpanzees are critical in this identify some of these specializations. Comparisons with endeavor. Human brains are much larger than non-human our closest living relative, the chimpanzee, have been primate brains, but human language capabilities cannot be crucial in this endeavor, for we cannot conclude that a trait entirely explained by brain size. Human brain specializations has uniquely evolved in humans unless we also demon- that potentially support our capacity for language include firstly, strate its absence in modern chimpanzees [4]. wider cortical minicolumns in both Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas compared with great apes; secondly, leftward Brain size asymmetries in Broca’s area volume and Wernicke’s area The human brain averages 1330 cc in size, a value that far minicolumn width that are not found in great apes; and thirdly, exceeds that for the brain of any other living primate arcuate fasciculus projections beyond Wernicke’s area to a species [5]. Rhesus macaque brains average only 88 cc [6]. region of expanded association cortex in the middle and inferior Chimpanzee brains average 405 cc and gorilla brains temporal cortex involved in processing word meaning. average 500 cc [5]. These numbers suggest the possibility Addresses that our unique capacity for language is simply a product 1 Department of Anthropology, Emory University, 1557 Dickey Drive, of our large brain size. However, evidence from high- Atlanta, GA 30322, United States functioning human microcephalics, some of whom have 2 Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University, brain volumes within the great ape range, suggests other- Atlanta, GA 30322, United States 3 wise since their linguistic abilities can eclipse those of Center for Behavioral Neuroscience, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, United States chimpanzees. Thus, there are apt to be qualitative differ- 4 Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta, ences between human and non-human primate brains GA 30322, United States 5 that support our capacity for language [7]. Center for Translational Social Neuroscience, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, United States Language circuitry Corresponding author: Rilling, James K. ([email protected]) In 1970, Geschwind outlined a model of the functional neuroanatomy of human language [8 ]. This model is a useful starting point for comparative analysis, however it Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 28:10–14 is important to note that it does not include features that This review comes from a themed issue on Communication and we now know to be important, such as the involvement of language the thalamus, basal ganglia and cortical areas beyond Edited by Michael Brainard and Tecumseh Fitch Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas in language [9 ,10 ,11,12]. In this model, Wernicke’s area, the posterior portion of the left superior temporal gyrus, is essential for speech comprehension, and Broca’s area, a region in the left Available online XXX inferior frontal cortex, is responsible for speech pro- 0959-4388/$ – see front matter, # 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights duction. These two areas are connected by a large white reserved. matter fiber tract known as the arcuate fasciculus. Perhaps http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2014.04.002 other primate species lack human language skills because they lack homologues of human Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas [13]. However, when defined based on cytoarchi- Introduction tecture and shared non-linguistic functional properties, Language is among the most distinctive attributes of such homologues have been identified in non-human Homo sapiens. Other primate species communicate, but primate brains [14]. This leads to the question of whether none does so by combining thousands of symbols accord- there might be microstructural differences within these ing to a defined set of rules to generate phrases with a areas between humans and non-human primates. nearly infinite variety of meanings [1,2]. Although chim- panzees raised in human linguistic environments develop Broca’s area limited symbolic abilities, none has eclipsed the language Not only is Broca’s area involved in human speech, it also competence of a 2 and 1/2-year old human child [3]. Thus, mediates aspects of communication in non-human there must be human brain specializations that support primates [15]. Mirror neurons are found in macaque area Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 28:10–14 www.sciencedirect.com Comparative primate neurobiology and language evolution Rilling 11 F5, part of which is homologous to the posterior part of greater integration of information by providing more human Broca’s area (area 44). Mirror neurons ostensibly space for connections. Finally, Broca’s area is larger in allow monkeys to improve their understanding of actions the left cerebral hemisphere of the human brain and this they observe in others by mapping those observed actions may be related to the strong tendency for many aspects of onto their own motor system and simulating the action in language function to be lateralized to the left hemisphere. their own brain. Mirror neurons are well known for their Importantly, this Broca’s area asymmetry is not present in response to reaching and grasping movements of both self chimpanzees [29]. and others, but they also fire when producing or observing communicative mouth movements [16]. Macaque mon- Wernicke’s area keys communicate extensively with orofacial expressions, Whereas production of human and macaque monkey voca- so mirror neurons are likely vital to macaque communi- lizations depend on different neural substrates, compre- cation. hension of species-specific vocalizations seems to depend on similar neural substrates in humans and macaque mon- There is evidence that mirror neuron regions are also keys [30]. Human speech comprehension depends on the involved in orofacial communication in humans [17], how- left posterior superior temporal gyrus (i.e. Wernicke’s area) ever human Broca’s area is also responsible for the gram- [8 ]. Similarly, the left superior temporal gyrus is respon- matical aspects of speech [18 ,19]. Perhaps, just as Broca’s sible for discriminating species-specific vocalizations, but area is involved in human speech production, Broca’s area not other types of auditory stimuli, in Japanese macaques homologue in non-human primates is involved in the [31]. Single cell electrophysiology similarly implicates the production of non-human primate vocalizations. However, superior temporal gyrus in processing macaque species- this does not seem to be the case, since lesioning the specific calls. Monkey auditory cortex consists of three monkey homologue of Broca’s area does not impair voca- longitudinal cytoarchitectonic streams in the superior lization [20,21]. Monkey calls instead depend upon the temporal lobe: a core region, a surrounding belt region, limbic system and brainstem. This neurological difference and an adjacent parabelt region [32]. Single cell electro- reflects differences in the nature of human speech and non- physiology studies have revealed that unlike core areas that human primate vocalizations. In contrast to human speech, respond best to pure tones, lateral belt areas respond best to monkey calls are largely involuntary expressions of complex sounds, including species-specific vocalizations emotional arousal [22], implying that they may not be [33]. Further evidence for similarity between humans and under voluntary cortical control. On the other hand, there macaques in the neural substrates for processing species- is some evidence that captive chimpanzees will exhibit specific vocalizations comes from a comparative PET neu- volitional calls [23], and that Broca’s homologue is involved roimaging study. In a small sample of macaque monkeys, in their production [24]. This raises the possibility that blood flow responses were more pronounced for species- Broca’s area was recruited for volitional vocal, in addition to specific calls compared with non-biological sounds within orofacial communication, before the divergence of humans cortical area Tpt as well as the dorsal frontal operculum, and chimpanzees some 5–7 million year ago. presumed homologues of Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas, respectively [34 ]. However, it should be noted that Despite these similarities, there are obvious functional another monkey PET study found that the anterior tip differences between human and chimpanzee Broca’s of the left superior temporal gyrus, rather than Wernicke’s areas, not just in the motor aspects of speech but also area homologue, was specialized for processing species- in the involvement of human Broca’s area with syntax specific calls [35]. The difference could be attributable to [11,25]. These functional differences are likely to be the former study measuring blood flow with a temporal supported by underlying anatomical differences. It is resolution of 60 s and the latter measuring glucose metab- now known that Broca’s area has wider cortical minicol- olism occurring over a period of about 25 min [35]. umns in humans than in great apes [26 ], whereas mini- column width does not differ between humans

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    5 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us