For Review Only 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 3

For Review Only 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 3

Behavioral Ecology Bitter taste enhances predatory biases against aggregations of prey with warning coloration Journal:For Behavioral Review Ecology Only Manuscript ID: BEHECO-2012-0319.R2 Manuscript Type: Research Article aposematism, warning colouration, aggregation, bitter taste, Gallus gallus Keywords: domesticus, innate bias Page 1 of 27 Behavioral Ecology 1 2 3 1 Title: Toxin-incited biases against aggregations of prey with warning colouration 4 5 2 6 7 8 3 Authors: Hannah M. Rowland a, Graeme D. Ruxton b, John Skelhorn c 9 10 4 11 12 13 5 a. Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge, CB2 3EJ, UK 14 15 6 b. School of Biology, Dyers Brae, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Fife, KY16 9TH, UK 16 17 18 7 c. Centre for ResearchFor in Animal Review Behaviour, College of LifeOnly and Environmental Sciences, 19 20 8 University of Exeter, Washington Singer Laboratories, Perry Road, Exeter EX4 4QG, UK 21 22 23 9 24 25 10 Corresponding Author: Hannah M. Rowland. 26 27 28 11 Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge, CB2 3EJ, UK. 29 12 [email protected] 30 31 32 13 Tel: +44 (0) 1223 331 759 33 14 Fax: +44 (0) 1223 336 676 34 35 36 15 37 38 16 RUNNING HEADER: Bitter taste and prey selection 39 40 41 17 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 1 Behavioral Ecology Page 2 of 27 1 2 3 18 TITLE: Bitter taste enhances predatory biases against aggregations of prey with warning 4 5 19 coloration 6 7 8 20 9 10 11 21 ABSTRACT 12 13 14 22 Aposematic prey that possess chemical defences advertise these to potential predators using 15 16 23 conspicuous warning coloration. Aposematism is often associated with group living, which is 17 18 24 hypothesised to enhanceFor the protection Review of these species. PredatorsOnly exhibit unlearned biases 19 20 21 25 against foods with warning coloration, and the presentation of a novel sound or bitter-tasting 22 23 26 toxin augments these biases. Whether these non-visual signal components also cause naïve 24 25 27 predators to more strongly avoid aggregated prey, and whether biases against aggregations are 26 27 28 restricted to situations where aggregated prey possess visual signals typically associated with 28 29 29 aposematism, is unknown. We conducted an experiment in which naïve domestic chicks ( Gallus 30 31 30 gallus domesticus 32 ) acted as predators and used artificially-colored pastry prey. The experiment had a 33 34 31 2 X 2 design in which naïve birds were offered a drop of either water or bitter-tasting 35 36 32 chloroquine solution before being given the choice between solitary and aggregated prey that 37 38 33 were either both red, a typical aposematic color, or both green (usually associated with crypsis 39 40 34 and palatability). We found that birds were warier of red-aggregated prey and attacked 41 42 35 significantly more solitary prey before aggregated prey compared to green. After sampling bitter- 43 44 45 36 tasting chloroquine solution the birds showed a bias in their attack decisions, attacking 46 47 37 significantly fewer aggregated prey in total compared with those who had sampled water, but 48 49 38 only when prey were red. Thus, exposure to a bitter tasting toxin affected predatory preferences. 50 51 39 We discuss our findings in relation to the mechanisms of bias, the benefits of group living and 52 53 40 the evolution of warning coloration and aggregation. 54 55 41 56 57 58 59 60 2 Page 3 of 27 Behavioral Ecology 1 2 3 42 KEY WORDS: aposematism; warning coloration; aggregation; chemical defence; bitter 4 5 43 taste; domestic chick; Gallus gallus domesticus ; evolution; innate bias 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 For Review Only 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 3 Behavioral Ecology Page 4 of 27 1 2 3 44 INTRODUCTION 4 5 45 Aposematism, the combination of a repellent physical or chemical defence, such as a toxin, with 6 7 46 conspicuous coloration (Cott 1940; Poulton 1890), is taxonomically and geographically 8 9 47 10 widespread (including birds, Dumbacher et al. 1993; marine animals, Edmunds 1991; insects, 11 12 48 Schmidt 2008; and amphibia, Summers & and Clough 2001). Aposematism has been particularly 13 14 49 well studied in the insects: the Monarch Butterfly ( Danaus plexippus L. ) sequesters toxic 15 16 50 cardenolides from its host plant and signals its chemical defence using a highly conspicuous 17 18 51 orange-and-black patternFor (Brower Review et al. 1968); the Seven SpotOnly Ladybird ( Coccinella septempunctata ) 19 20 52 synthesizes the toxin coccinelline, and advertises using black spots on a red background 21 22 23 53 (Marples, Brakefield & Cowie 1989); and the yellow-and-black stripes of the Common Wasp 24 25 54 (Vespula vulgaris ) are an indicator of its venomous sting (Schmidt 2008). These species have in 26 27 55 common a tendency to live, migrate or hibernate in groups, which is hypothesised to enhance 28 29 56 their conspicuousness and the power of their advertisements (Sillen-Tullberg & Leimar 1988). 30 31 32 57 Understanding how prey have evolved aposematic colouration and aggregative behavior 33 34 58 that increases the likelihood of detection by predators (Ruxton & Sherratt 2006) is an intriguing 35 36 59 question for evolutionary biologists, because attacks are likely to be costly, even if prey possess 37 38 39 60 defences that increase the likelihood of survival (Higginson et al. 2011). The most widely 40 41 61 accepted answer to this question is that naive predators are instinctively averse to the colors and 42 43 62 patterns most commonly associated with toxicity (Mastrota & Mench 1995; Roper & Cook 1989; 44 45 63 Schuler & Roper 1992; Schuler & Hesse 1985; Sillén-Tullberg 1985), and aggregation of 46 47 64 aposematic prey generates unlearned aversions (Gamberale & Tullberg 1996a). Conspicuous 48 49 65 coloration also facilitates faster avoidance learning by predators (Gittleman & Harvey 1980; 50 51 52 66 Guilford 1992; Roper & Wistow 1986). Similarly, aggregation enhances the speed at which 53 54 67 predators learn to avoid aposematic prey (Gagliardo & Guilford 1993; Riipi et al. 2001; Sillén- 55 56 68 Tullberg 1990; Sillén-Tullberg & Leimar 1988; Tullberg, Leimar & Gamberale-Stille 2000). 57 58 59 60 4 Page 5 of 27 Behavioral Ecology 1 2 3 69 Aposematic prey rarely rely on visual signals only , and make use of additional non-visual 4 5 70 signals of their unpalatability by using for example, sounds (Brown, Boettner & Yack 2007; 6 7 71 Haskell 1966), odours (Moore, Vance Brown & Rothschild 1990; Rothschild, Moore & Vance 8 9 72 10 Brown 1984) and the secretion of bitter-tasting compounds (De Jong et al. 1991). These are 11 12 73 thought to act as ‘go-slow’ signals that cause predators to reduce their attack rates on prey that 13 14 74 are more likely to be defended (Guilford 1994). In line with this theory, it has been found 15 16 75 consistently that the presentation of a novel sound, odour or bitter-tasting compound causes 17 18 76 naïve predators to increaseFor their Reviewbias against novel foods orOnly foods with visual traits typically 19 20 77 associated with aposematism, such as conspicuousness or a red or yellow color (Jetz, Rowe & 21 22 23 78 Guilford 2001; Lindström, Rowe & Guilford 2001; Marples & Roper 1996; Rowe & Guilford 24 25 79 1996, 1999a, b; Rowe & Skelhorn 2005; Siddall & Marples 2008, 2011; Skelhorn, Griksaitis & 26 27 80 Rowe 2008). It is not currently known whether these additional signal components also cause 28 29 81 naïve predators to bias their foraging preferences against aggregated prey, and, if so, whether 30 31 82 biases against aggregations are restricted to situations where aggregated prey possess visual 32 33 83 signals typically associated with aposematism. To answer this question, we examined the foraging 34 35 36 84 behavior of naïve domestic chicks ( Gallus gallus domesticus ), which served as visually hunting 37 38 85 predators. We produced two ‘species’ of prey which differed in color signal. Prey were either red 39 40 86 (a color typically associated with insect warning patterns) or were green (a color associated with 41 42 87 palatable cryptic prey). We gave separate groups of predators a choice between aggregated and 43 44 88 solitary prey that were either all red or all green. Chicks are known to use taste cues in 45 46 89 conjunction with visual signals to make foraging decisions (Rowe & Skelhorn 2005; Skelhorn, 47 48 49 90 Griksaitis & Rowe 2008). We therefore used a well-established system of presenting a taste cue 50 51 91 prior to a prey preference test, to determine whether sampling a bitter-tasting toxin prior to 52 53 92 meeting the prey caused birds to bias their attacks away from aggregated prey and towards 54 55 93 solitary prey; and whether any bias to avoid aggregations was present both when prey were red 56 57 94 and when they were green (see Rowe & Skelhorn 2005; Skelhorn, Griksaitis & Rowe 2008; 58 59 60 5 Behavioral Ecology Page 6 of 27 1 2 3 95 Skelhorn 2011). This simulates a real ecological scenario that can occur in many predator-prey 4 5 96 interactions . Within a given prey species, both chemical defence and colouration commonly vary 6 7 97 ontogenetically, with aposematism being more common in later life-history stages and crucially 8 9 98 10 often occurring later in development than the expression of significant chemical defences (e.g.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    28 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us