Vocal Repertoire of the Mexican Chickadee I. Calls

Vocal Repertoire of the Mexican Chickadee I. Calls

J. Field Ornithol., 61(4):380-387 VOCAL REPERTOIRE OF THE MEXICAN CHICKADEE I. CALLS MILLICENT SIGLER FIeKEN Departmentof BiologicalSciences Universityof Wisconsin-Milwaukee Milwaukee, Wisconsin53201 USA Abstract.--I describeacoustic structure and use of calls (excludingsong and song-like vocalizations)of Mexican Chickadees(Parus sclateri). Calls are comparedwith thoseof other North Americangray-backed chickadees. The generalcall repertoireof the Mexican Chickadee is similar to other chickadeeshowever, the "chick-a-dee" call of the Mexican Chickadeeis very differentin acousticstructure from calls of other membersof its species group. Other differencesin acousticstructure may be due to the linkage, rather than independentevolution of certain calls. REPERTORIO VOCAL DE PARUS SCLATERI I. LLAMADAS Sinopsis.--Describola estructuraac6stica y uso de llamadas(excluyendo las cancionesy vocalizacionesparecidas a cantos)de Parussclateri. Las 11amadasson comparadascon las producidaspor otra avesde Norteam6ricadel mismog6nero. Encuentro que el repertorio en generaldel ave estudiadaes similar a la de otroscong6neres. Sin embargo,la 11amada "chic-a-di" de la especiemexicana es diferenteen estructuraac6stica a la 11amadade otros miembrosdel grupode especies.Otras diferenciasen la estructuraac6stica podrian deberse a la uni6n, en vez de la evoluci6nindependiente, de ciertas11amadas. The Mexican Chickadee(Parus sclateri) is poorlyknown comparedto the other, well-studiedgray-backed chickadees of North America. Dixon and Martin (1979) performedthe first sonagraphicanalysis of several vocalizationsof this species,and noted marked differencesfrom other chickadees.Here a more detailedsonagraphic analysis and descriptionof use of calls are provided.Songs and song-likevocalizations are treated elsewhere(Ficken 1990b). I comparevocalizations of the Mexican Chick- adeewith other closelyrelated chickadees for which sonagramsare avail- able:Black-capped Chickadee, P. atricapillus(Ficken et al. 1978), Carolina Chickadee, P. carolinensis(Smith 1972), and Mountain Chickadee, P. gambeli (Dixon et al. 1970, Gaddis 1985). STUDY AREA AND METHODS The main study site was Rustler Park in the Chiricahua Mountains (CochiseCo.), Arizona, the site of Dixon and Martin's (1979) study. Three visits were made: 5 and 6 Oct. 1985, 15-17 and 19 May 1986, and 5-10 Oct. 1986. Recordingbegan shortly after dawn and continued until the birds showeda marked decreasein vocalizingaround noon. In October,I followedflocks, but attemptedto recordthe vocalizationsof a singlefocal bird beforemoving on to another in order to obtain some samplesof known individuals. Three to five different flockswere studied eachfall (theywere sufficientlydistant from eachother that it wasunlikely that individualsmoved between them). In May recordingwas concen- 380 Vol.61, No. 4 MexicanChickadee Calls [381 trated near the nest of one pair, but the territories of at least five other pairs were visited.Apart from the focal pair, however,vocalizations were infrequent exceptduring territorial boundaryencounters. I used a Sony Walkman Professionalcassette tape recorder,with an ElectrovoiceSoundspot microphone in 1985, and a Nakamichi CM-100 cardioidmicrophone in 1986. In addition to recordingvocalizations, as much data as possiblewere includedon their contexts.Vocalizations were analyzedwith a Kay 7800 Digital Sona-Graph(150 Hz filter bandwidth). RESULTS Only one nest was observed(young were presenton the day it was found--15 May). As this speciesis sexuallymonomorphic and birdswere unmarked, the role of the sexesin care of the young couldnot be deter- mined unequivocally. During the non-breedingseason, mixed speciesflocks consisted of 30- 100 individuals including four to six chickadeesand numerousother species.The flocksranged over a rather large area. The chickadeescalled frequently(mainly "Chick-a-dee"calls); other speciesexcept for Pygmy Nuthatchesø(Sittapygmaea) were much lessvocal. In many casestwo chickadeesforaged within 1 to 2 m of eachother, with a third much more distant(y 10 m). Other chickadeeswere at least20 m farther away. Very little chasingor supplantingwas observed,much lessthan in fall flocks of Black-cappedChickadees (pers. obs.). I usethe call terminologyof Ficken et al. (1978) for the Black-capped Chickadee,which is basedlargely on that of Smith (1972) for the very similar repertoireof the Carolina Chickadee.The useof the sameterms doesnot imply homologynecessarily. The entire vocalrepertoire of the Mexican Chickadeemay not have been sampled,particularly somerare calls that may occuronly in specificcontexts. It is likely, however,that the major call categorieswere recorded. "Chick-a-dee"calls.--The "Chick-a-dee" call soundsquite different from thoseof other gray-backedchickadees. A detailed analysisof this call complexwill be presentedelsewhere (Ficken and Hailman, in prep.), but a brief descriptionis providedfor comparisonswith other calls in the species'repertoire. Three note-typesin this complex call are relatively common(A, C and D in Fig. l a and b). In over 1000 calls only one or two exampleswere found of a note-typethat is similar to the B note- type of the Black-cappedChickadee (Fig. lb). The sameletters are used for designationof notetypes as for the Black-cappedChickadee (Ficken et al. 1978), but at this preliminary stageof analysis,homology is not implied. The A note-typesare quite variablein pitch and duration.They have a zig-zag pattern on a sonagram,showing rapid frequencymodulation which õivesthem abuzzy quality very differentfrom callsof otherchick- adees.The B and C are more similar to thoseof the Black-cappedChick- adee.The D note-typeshows some similarities with comparablenotes of 382] M.S. Ficken J.Field Ornithol. Autumn 1990 8- C • Time (sec) I I 0,5 sec FIGURE1. a. call in "Chick-a-dee"complex showing A and D note-types.b. call in "Chick- a-dee"complex showing B and C note-types.c. "Hiss" (a very shortone). d. "Hiss" (a long one, showinga slight but interruptedbanding pattern). e. "Tseet." congeners,but has a much reducedbanded pattern, and thesenotes sound much harsher and noisier. As is the casewith many other chickadees,the "Chick-a-dee" call is complexwith manyvariations. Note-types occur in differentcombinations and the number of repetitionsof note-typesis also variable.This com- binatorialsystem has been analyzed in detail in the Black-cappedChick- adee (Hailman et al. 1985). "Hiss."--The "Hiss" is one of the mostunusual callsof this chickadee, both in acousticstructure and usage.The call is highly variable (two extreme examplesare in Fig. l c and d). Coefficientsof variation were very large for both lowest frequencyand duration (Table 1). For an indicationof variability,I comparethe acousticallyrather similarD note to "Hisses" from the same bird. Coefficients of variation were much less for the lowestfrequency and durationof the D notethan for the "Hiss." The "Hiss" sounds like a sudden exhalation of air. Well-defined fre- Vol.61, No. 4 MexicanChickadee Calls [383 TABLE1. Characteristicsof "Hisses"compared with the D notefrom the sameindividual. Highest Lowest frequency frequency Duration Note type (kHz) (kHz) (s) Hiss • 7.90 2.79 0.10 n 6 9 9 C.V. 5.4 49.1 70 D note • 6.79 3.38 0.24 n 11 12 12 C.V. 8.8 8.8 30.2 quencybands are infrequentand whenthey occur they are broken,unlike the moreregular banding patterns typical of D notesof othergray-backed chickadees."Hisses" are similar to the D note-typein this species,but differ in their greatervariability and in usuallybeing uttered singlyand neverin conjunctionwith othernote-types of the "Chick-a-dee"complex. Occasionallytwo or more "Hisses"may be given,but then they usually exhibit variable inter-call intervalsrather than the more regular intervals between D notes in a series. "Hisses"are givenin a wide varietyof situations,always when a bird is perched.They are not confinedto socialcontexts, as they are sometimes given by lone birds that are temporarilyseparated from other group membersduring the fall. They are more frequent when individualsare in closeproximity in flocks.Hisses often occurred during boutsof "Gar- gles"in territorial encounters(Ficken 1990b), the apparentrecipient of the "Gargles" givesthe "Hisses." Many were recordednear the nest hole,just beforea bird enteredthe cavity. Becauseof the varied usage, it is difficult to ascribea messageto this call. "Hisses" often occurred, however, in situationswhen there was some hesitancyto locomote,or where the caller may have beenslightly fearful. "Hisses"are widespreadin parids.Thielcke (1968) reportedthem in 13 species.However, in all other parids for which the call is reported, the usageis very differentfrom that in the Mexican Chickadee.In other species,"Hisses" are usually given within the nest cavityas the parent or nestlingssway back and forth (Thielcke1968). The acousticquality of the call and the nature of the associatedmovement gave rise to the hypothesisthat it is Batesianmimicry of a snake(Pickens 1928, Sibley 1955). "Tseets."--Theseare very short,low amplitudenotes (Fig. le), that are uncommon(n = 6, but others may not have been detected).The samplesize is small, and usageis uncertain,but they seemassociated with agonisticsituations in whichthe callermay be somewhatfearful, as seemsto be the casein the very similarcall of the Black-cappedChickadee (Ficken et al. 1978). "Twitter."--The calls consistof a variable number of very rapidly utteredtransients that covera wide frequencyrange and appearas nearly 384] M.S. Ficken J.Field Ornithol.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    8 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us