Evaluating Landmine-Detection Rats in Operational Conditions

Evaluating Landmine-Detection Rats in Operational Conditions

Journal of Conventional Weapons Destruction Volume 18 Issue 3 The Journal of ERW and Mine Action Article 17 November 2014 Evaluating Landmine-detection Rats in Operational Conditions Amanda Mahoney APOPO Timothy Edwards APOPO Kate Lalonde APOPO Christophe Cox APOPO Bart Weetjens APOPO See next page for additional authors Follow this and additional works at: https://commons.lib.jmu.edu/cisr-journal Part of the Other Public Affairs, Public Policy and Public Administration Commons, and the Peace and Conflict Studies Commons Recommended Citation Mahoney, Amanda; Edwards, Timothy; Lalonde, Kate; Cox, Christophe; Weetjens, Bart; Gilbert, Tekimiti; Tewelde, Tess; and Poling, Alan (2014) "Evaluating Landmine-detection Rats in Operational Conditions," The Journal of ERW and Mine Action : Vol. 18 : Iss. 3 , Article 17. Available at: https://commons.lib.jmu.edu/cisr-journal/vol18/iss3/17 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Center for International Stabilization and Recovery at JMU Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Journal of Conventional Weapons Destruction by an authorized editor of JMU Scholarly Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Evaluating Landmine-detection Rats in Operational Conditions Authors Amanda Mahoney, Timothy Edwards, Kate Lalonde, Christophe Cox, Bart Weetjens, Tekimiti Gilbert, Tess Tewelde, and Alan Poling This article is available in Journal of Conventional Weapons Destruction: https://commons.lib.jmu.edu/cisr-journal/ vol18/iss3/17 Mahoney et al.: Evaluating Landmine-detection Rats in Operational Conditions Evaluating Landmine-detection Rats in Operational Conditions Researchers evaluate the accuracy of pouched rats’ ability to detect landmines under operational search conditions. Results indicate the ineffectiveness of one training method for maintaining quality operational performance and suggest further examination. by Amanda Mahoney, Timothy L. Edwards, Kate Lalonde, Christophe Cox, Bart Weetjens, Tekimiti Gilbert, Tess Tewelde and Alan Poling [ APOPO and Western Michigan University ] he uncertainty of specific landmine locations presents a implemented in which food was not presented. Each rat emitted fewer unique challenge to using animals for landmine detection, identification responses thereafter. While the rats’ accuracy fell substan- T preventing rewards from being arranged under operation- tially, false alarms did not significantly increase. On average, the decline al search conditions. A common strategy is to create a training field in accuracy was evident within three days of the onset of extinction. near the operational site, so animals receive regular refresher training. Furthermore, when the reinforcement condition was reinstated, rats However, the animals may discriminate between the operational condi- took an average of four days to recover accuracy to baseline levels. These tion and the training condition based upon contextual discrepancies. results show that mine-detection rats’ performance deteriorates quickly when extinction is arranged. Overview Consistent with the International Mine Action Standards (IMAS) Phase 1 of the present experiment evaluated the accuracy of five rats’ 94.01 (2008), APOPO’s mine-detection rats are given opportunities for abilities to detect landmines in a no-reward condition followed by a reinforcement at a training site located close to the actual minefield.6 reward condition on a training field (i.e., non-hoed ground). Phase 2 The training site contains inactive landmines planted at known loca- evaluated the same conditions when the no-reward condition was con- tions and resembles the operational site as closely as possible. At the ducted in an area made to simulate an operational minefield (i.e., hoed training site, rats receive differential reinforcement for correct indica- ground). When the sites were identical, the rats’ accuracy was similar tion responses, and groups of rats rotate between the operational and across conditions. When the no-reward session ground was hoed in training sites so that there are no disruptions in landmine clearance. Phase 2, the rats’ accuracy fell relative to their performance in the base- This arrangement provides differential, albeit intermittent, reinforce- line condition and reward session.1 These results indicate that conduct- ment for correct indication responses and should be sufficient for main- ing training with reinforcement in areas that differ substantially from taining accurate detection so long as operational search areas is an ineffective method for maintaining good • Training and operational sites closely resemble one another, pre- operational performance. Alternative reinforcement methods, such as venting the rats from discriminating settings where reinforce- creating reinforcement opportunities within a minefield using TNT ment is and is not available contamination, should be examined. • Exposure to the former setting is limited in time so that the del- Anti-Persoonsmijnen Ontmijnende Product Ontwikkeling (APOPO) eterious effects of extinction are not manifested.8,9 Previous find- uses pouched rats trained with operant conditioning; the organization’s ings suggest the reinforcement for correct indication responses operational experience and published data suggest these rats can de- does sufficiently maintain accurate in the operational settings tect landmines and other explosive remnants of war (ERW) successful- under which the rats’ performance has been evaluated.3,4 ly.1,2,3,4 The rats work in extinction when they search for mines, because However, if the training field does not sufficiently resemble the op- landmine locations are unknown. Therefore, whether an indication re- erational site, accurate operational performance will likely be unsus- sponse is correct (i.e., near a mine or other ERW) or incorrect is unclear, tainable. Basic research shows that animals exposed to arrangements in and such responses are never reinforced (i.e., rewarded with food) in the which periods of extinction alternate with periods of reinforcer avail- field.5 Studies have shown that operant behavior weakens and becomes ability, each correlated with a salient exteroceptive stimulus, respond more variable when extinction is arranged, and Mahoney et al. recently rarely during the signaled extinction period.10,11 Since creating a train- examined whether similar results would be obtained with pouched rats ing site that closely resembles a particular demining site is sometimes exposed to extinction on our training minefield.6 Every day during the impossible, ascertaining whether a similar relation occurs under field baseline condition, five rats separately searched one 100-sq-m box that conditions is important. The present study was intended to determine contained a single mine under conditions where an indication response whether the reinforcement of correct identification responses in set- within 1 m of a mine was reinforced with food, while all other responses tings that did not closely resemble settings in which extinction was in had no programmed consequences. Rats detected, on average, 97.8% of effect was sufficient to maintain accurate landmine detection. For com- the mines and made very few errors. An extinction condition was then parison, we also determined detection accuracy when the extinction Published by JMU Scholarly Commons, 2014 1 18.3 | fall 2014 | the journal of ERW and mine action | research and development 59 Journal of Conventional Weapons Destruction, Vol. 18, Iss. 3 [2014], Art. 17 The rat’s location is easily calculated in x and y coordinates. All photos courtesy of APOPO. setting and reinforcement setting were the same. In some applications, Materials included clickers that sounded before providing food (ba- APOPO’s rats searched for landmines (and other explosives) in areas nanas served as the reinforcer), timers, data sheets and mine detection that had first been cleared of vegetation by a brush-cutting machine or training box materials. Measuring tape stretched along the side of the rototilled by large armored machines that sometimes fail to detonate box, and rope stretched across the box to guide the rat as it walked inside every piece of unexploded ordnance. Because training fields cannot be the box. The rats, attached to the rope via a harness and leash, walked rototilled in ways that mimic minefields, grass-covered, pre-prepared back and forth inside the box along the rope. Two measuring tapes at- boxes were hoed by hand to uncover raw soil and used as the two set- tached to the rat’s harness at the zero mark, and each trainer held one tings of interest. measuring tape. Thus, the exact location of the rat’s indications could be specified using x and y coordinates revealed by the measuring tape in one Setting, Subjects and Materials trainer’s hand and the measuring tape value at the trainer’s feet. After APOPO conducted the experiment in Morogoro, Tanzania, on the the rat traversed the rope in one direction, trainers took a 0.5-m step for- APOPO training field. There, mines were buried within permanent box- ward, and the rat traversed the box in the opposite direction. es ranging in size from 100 to 400 sq m. Some boxes received markings Data were recorded on graph paper that depicted the box measure- to indicate the location of the landmines while some did not; however, ments, with each test box divided on the datasheet into 0.5-m by 0.5-m the locations of all landmines were recorded in a database. Boxes with- cells; cells corresponding

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    8 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us