Modern Psychological Studies Volume 15 Number 1 Article 5 2009 Would New Yorkers help a lost child? 1976 v 2008? Amanda Verdi Fordham University Follow this and additional works at: https://scholar.utc.edu/mps Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Verdi, Amanda (2009) "Would New Yorkers help a lost child? 1976 v 2008?," Modern Psychological Studies: Vol. 15 : No. 1 , Article 5. Available at: https://scholar.utc.edu/mps/vol15/iss1/5 This articles is brought to you for free and open access by the Journals, Magazines, and Newsletters at UTC Scholar. It has been accepted for inclusion in Modern Psychological Studies by an authorized editor of UTC Scholar. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Amanda Vardi Would New Yorkers help Fordham University a lost child: 1976 v 2008? To test the notion of urban "stimulus overload" (Milgram, 1970), this study replicates a 1977 "lost child" experiment, with a child (age 9 or 10) asking 146 New Yorkers for help. As expected: (a) The rate of New Yorkers who helped a lost child rose significantly, from 46% in 1977 to 61.6% in 2008. (b) When debriefed and told that the lost child was actually part of an experiment, only 11% of New Yorkers expressed a negative reaction, compared with 55% who reacted positively. In fact, the more helpful one's behavior, the more positive their later reaction to debriefing (r = +.67, p < .001). The implications of these findings are discussed, regarding the future methods and findings of urban psychology research. Throughout history, the city has often been the following two questions: how does the city associated with a negative attitude; researchers refer impact the individual and why do people live in to this as the anti-urban bias (Steiner, 1977; cities. The theories that focus on what the Fowkes, 1988; White, 1962; Fischer, 1984). consequences of urbanism are include the following: Fischer (1984) reveals that this negative image of the adaptational (internal changes), situational city is present in the Bible and in American literature. (immediate behavior), selection or compositional Though this anti-urban bias does indeed exist, a pro- (who chooses to live in cities), S-O-R (city life , and urban bias has been identified as well; some cultures subcultural. associate the city with positive images and it is thus Milgram pioneered the use of empirical methods clear that though biases do exist, there is to study the individual in the city (Milgram, 1970). ambivalence toward the view of cities (Takooshian, Ultimately, it was Milgram who launched the field of 1977; Steiner, 1976; Fowkes, 1988). urban psychology, and he did so via a distinctive Furthermore, theorists and researchers in the field style of experimentation; as Blass (2005, p. 18) of urban psychology have focused on whether and says, "in most of his urban research the outcome how the size of the community affects people's lives. measure was discrete and dichotomous . [thus] Essentially, the field of urban psychology focuses on the findings lent them a quality of absoluteness, 31 clarity, and finality that made their implications great majority of sensory inputs that are not related directly discernible to both lay and professional to their own survival needs; thus, Cacciola's readers." research supports the conventional image of the Milgram tested the adaptation theory of urban urbanite who "does not want to get involved." behavior—that the city produces internal, long term Solomon's research (1980) proved that anonymity changes in individuals, due to urban "stimulus reduces help and that the findings are pertinent to overload" (Milgram, 1970). When urbanites are urban life inasmuch as big cities encourage constantly bombarded by stimuli, they adapt by anonymity as a response to sensory overload. He filtering out "unimportant" inputs or by labeling some also found that anonymous city dwellers may further as "high" or "low" priority. He asserts that all of this attempt to adapt to overload by ignoring low priority causes a behavior change in the individual; in the inputs, such as low status victims, and ambiguous city, incivility increases and we are likely to bump requests for help. into others without apologizing for it, trust is This field experiment replicates the 1977 lost child diminished and we tend to think people in the city experiment. In addition to testing helpful behavior are unfriendly, bystander behavior decreases our among urbanites, it goes further to debrief them, then helping behavior, and role behavior increases—we assess their frank feelings about their experience. have more formal, superficial relationships. He also This research tested two hypotheses: (a) New reveals that anonymity increases when living in the Yorkers would be more helpful in 2008 than in the city; essentially, there is a sense of freedom in not 1970s. This is based on the common but untested being known, but also isolation. wisdom that since the 1970s, traumata like terrorism Milgram's theory was supported by much and the 9-11-01 tragedy have made New Yorkers research regarding the "Lost Child" (Milgram, 1992; more concerned about the welfare of one another. Takooshian, Haber, & Lucido, 1977; Cacciola, (b) When debriefed, those who helped the child 1980). Essentially, researchers wanted to find out would express a positive rather than negative feeling how and to what extent helping behavior is impacted about field experiments. This too is based on the by the city. Takooshian, Haber, & Lucido (1977) anecdotal experiences ofpast field researchers (like found that in the cities, 46% offered to help a lost Philip Spencer and Heather Hoerner, 2005). child while in the towns, 72% did so. They also Before collecting field data, 34 students at the found that differences existed qualitatively; in the start of a course in urban psychology were asked to cities, 52 of the 69 who refused did so abruptly. pen two predictions: (a) First, what percent of New They also focused on Chicago in particular to point Yorkers would aid an upset nine-year-old who out that Chicagoans were not only the most helpful; asked them "I'm lost. Can you help me phone my they also offered help in an unusual way, by calling house?" Their predictions ranged widely, from 20% the police. In Chicago, 35% of the helpers ignored up to 90%, with a mean of 62%. (b) Then after the nearby phone and, instead, flagged down a viewing a six-minute videotape of the lost child patrol car or went looking for a patrolman. experiment, these 34 students were asked what Psychologist Stanley Milgram and other urban words they would use to predict pedestrians' likely theorists have referred to such behavior as an feelings when debriefed that the child was not really "institutionalized response." Essentially, city dwellers lost. Aside from a few positive words, such as learn to refer responsibilities such as picking up litter, curious, interested, and amused, the clear majority intervening in crime, or other social problems to the of predictions were negative words, such as authorities. Cacciola's research (1980) was also annoyed, embarrassed, angry, deceived, indifferent, interesting in that it revealed that city-dwellers regretful, and 'those damn psychologists!" Of filtered out the lost child to the point where they course such predictions are no substitute for stepped over the child or walked around a wide experimental findings. circle so as not to step on the sprawled child. Essentially, the Manhattanites learned to filter out the 32 Method and to indicate if the reaction to being debriefed was a positive, negative, or a neutral/mixed one, as well Procedure as indicate comments given as approached by the A team of 7 female researchers from Fordham child or as debriefed. The researchers also University at Lincoln Center participated in recorded if the reaction to being debriefed was a collecting experimental data for a 2008 replication of positive, negative, or a mixed/neutral one. If the this study in New York City; they worked under the participants simply kept walking, the recorder would supervision of Dr. Harold Takooshian.1 The label the act as "ignored or neutral" and another participants included 4 children2 who were relatives recorder, standing somewhat nearby, would catch of students in an Urban Psychology course at the participant and offer them a debrief card. If the Fordham University. The 4 children would run up to participant was willing to answer a few questions the subjects and exclaim the following: "excuse me, I am same questions would be asked of them as was of lost; can you please help me phone my mom." the helpful participants. Some variations of this did exist, where some of the The researchers analyzed the data using SP S S. children would say, "excuse me, I am lost. I was They labeled the helping behavior as 0=not helpful; separated from my class during a field trip; can you 1=mixed/ignore; 2=helpful. They labeled reactions please help me phone my teacher." It is important to to the debriefing as 0=negative; 1=mixed/ignore; indicate that the children were not asking for money; 2=positive. They obtained a crosstabulation of the in fact, they had $12 in a pouch that had their name four children by rate helping as well as of helpful and emergency information on it. The children behavior by positive reaction to debriefing. The included Andrew, age 9; Ryan, age 10; Joshua, age researchers also obtained the Pearson Correlation 10; and Olivia, age 9. Coefficient in order to determine what type of The researchers went to various locations in association existed between positive helping Manhattan, New York where there would be a large behavior and a positive reaction to being debriefed.
Details
-
File Typepdf
-
Upload Time-
-
Content LanguagesEnglish
-
Upload UserAnonymous/Not logged-in
-
File Pages9 Page
-
File Size-