Pain Processing in Multisensory Environments

Pain Processing in Multisensory Environments

Review article e-Neuroforum 2010 · 1:23–28 Marion Höfle1 · M. Hauck2 · A.K. Engel1 · D. Senkowski1 DOI 10.1007/s13295-010-0004-z 1 Department of Neurophysiology and Pathophysiology, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg 2 Department of Neurology, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg © Springer-Verlag 2010 Pain processing in multisensory environments Introduction It is well known that different environ- influences between visual and pain stim- mental and cognitive factors can modu- uli. These studies support the notion that Imagine yourself cutting peppers. While late activity in the so-called pain matrix, visual inputs can strongly affect the pro- you watch the pepper getting smaller, i.e., the network of brain regions involved cessing of painful stimuli. Finally, we give your mind might wander and the knife in pain processing [28]. Moreover, there is an outlook for future research on pain pro- accidentally carves the skin of your finger elaborate knowledge of the neurophysio- cessing in multisensory environments. and you feel a sharp pain. Painful events in logical mechanisms underlying pain pro- real-world situations do not occur in iso- cessing, as well as of the principle mecha- Dimensions of pain perception lation but often comprise input from ad- nisms of multisensory integration of non- ditional sensory modalities, i.e., the visual painful information (. Box 1). Surpris- The application of different pain models percept of the knife penetrating the finger. ingly, however, only few studies have sys- (. Box 2) revealed that pain is a complex The successful integration and recall of tematically addressed how stimuli from experience that can be described along painful stimuli in combination with high- other sensory modalities affect nocicep- two major dimensions, which themselves ly informative input of other sensory mo- tive processing. Thus, it is not known comprise different components. On the dalities is often crucial for our wellbeing whether such influences may follow the one hand, there is the sensory-discrimina- and survival. It enables us to detect and general principles of multisensory inte- tive dimension of pain, which comprises correctly respond to potentially harmful gration. quality, intensity, and spatio-temporal as- stimuli. To understand pain processing in In this article, we will first introduce pects of the nociceptive sensation. For in- naturalistic multisensory environments, it the different dimensions of pain and de- stance, in the finger-cutting example de- is therefore important to study how senso- scribe factors that have been shown to scribed above, the intensity of the pain ry inputs of other modalities influence the modulate pain perception and pain pro- sensation may crucially depend on the processing of pain. cessing. Then, we provide an overview of force and the edge of the knife and the pain recent studies focusing on cross-sensory can be well localized to the respective fin- Box 1: Principles governing multisensory processing Studies using non-painful stimuli showed that a large number of multisensory processes follow specific principles that predict the strength of multisen- sory interactions. The principles, which have been originally derived from studies on single neuron activity in the superior colliculus (SC) of cats [27], but which can be also applied to multisensory processing in humans describe the impact of: (1) the relative temporal alignment of sensory inputs, (2) the spatial configuration of stimuli, and (3) the intensity of sensory inputs on multisensory integrative processing. A first principle of multisensory processing considers the relative timing of sensory inputs. Studies on the effects of timing on multisensory interactions in the SC show that there is a relatively flexible time interval of 100–200 ms in which sensory inputs delivered to one modality can influence responses to stimuli delivered to another modality. Stimuli that are presented in close temporal synchrony are more likely to be integrated than inputs that are pre- sented with a large temporal asynchrony. A second principle deals with the influence of the spatial configuration of sensory inputs. Enhanced responses in SC [18] are observed for multisensory stimuli with combinations of inputs that originate from the same location in space compared to combinations of inputs that originate from different locations in space. A third principle covers the effects of stimulus intensities on multisensory processes. This principle, which is frequently labeled “principle of inverse effectiveness”, describes the observation that the strength of a multisensory integration effect is in many cases inversely related to the magnitude of the response to the combined unimodal inputs. Thus, the combination of stimuli that by themselves elicit weak responses often leads to stronger multisensory interactions than the combination of inputs that by themselves elicits strong responses. Together, these principles provide an important framework to inform and drive current research on multisensory processing. e-Neuroforum 2 · 2010 | 23 Review article Box 2: Human experimental pain models There are different methods to induce pain under controlled settings. The most frequently used pain models in electrophysiological studies are laser and electric stimulation (for a review see [5, 26]). Laser stimulation consists of a brief radiant heat pulse delivered by a laser beam, typically applied to the dor- sum of the hand. Electric stimulation is either applied via a surface electrode at the wrist or via a thin electrode that is fed through a small hole in the epi- dermal skin of a fingertip. Usually, the individual pain threshold is determined beforehand and for the experimental procedure, stimuli comprising 1.5- to 2-fold individual pain threshold intensity are used. On the perceptual level these stimuli are comparable to a weak pinprick or pulling a single hair follicle. ger. On the other hand, the affective-moti- perimental manipulations and that func- view see [30, 33]). Emotional pictures de- vational dimension of pain comprises un- tional neuroimaging studies provide an picting scenes or faces are commonly used pleasantness and disturbing character as excellent tool for the study of the anatom- for mood induction during pain process- well as distress caused by the pain sensa- ical structures of pain processing and pain ing (e.g., [15, 16]). Moreover, odors [30, 31], tion [17, 21]. Interestingly, the unpleasant- modulations. emotional statements [34], and hypnotic ness and disturbing character of the pain The temporal dynamics of pain pro- suggestions [14, 23] are capable of induc- sensation can vary depending on contex- cessing have been examined in electro- ing different emotional states during pain tual factors (e.g., if friends are around) and encephalography (EEG) and magneto- processing. Most of the studies found that prior learning experience (e.g., if one al- encephalography (MEG) studies, which positive mood tends to reduce the percep- ready knows how to cope with the respec- allow the non-invasive measurement tion of painful events and that negative tive painful situation). Thus, different as- of neuronal activity with a high tempo- mood tends to enhance pain perception pects of painful events can have different ral resolution. Pain-evoked potentials or [33]. Another factor that can modulate impacts on the two main dimensions of fields in the EEG and MEG, respective- pain processing is attentional distraction. pain. ly, which can be measured by the repeat- Painful events are perceived as less intense The anatomical structures underlying the ed presentation of identical pain stimu- when individuals are distracted from the dimensions of pain processing in humans li and subsequent averaging of response event (for a review see [30]). Thus, atten- have primarily been examined in studies signals across trials, typically occur be- tional and affective factors have a strong using functional magnetic resonance im- tween 100 and 350 ms after the onset of influence on pain perception. Given that aging (fMRI) and positron emission to- painful events [4, 11]. Short latency de- inputs from other sensory modalities of- mography (PET). A large number of fM- flections (<200 ms) in the evoked poten- ten enhance attention to the sensory as- RI and PET studies showed that the two tials or fields are thought to reflect paral- pect of the painful input and that in par- dimensions of pain are linked to process- lel activity in primary and secondary so- ticular the visual perception of painful ing in different anatomical structures of matosensory areas, whereas longer laten- events can evoke negative affective states, the pain matrix (for a review see [22]). The cy deflections (around 200–350 ms) seem it is likely that contextually relevant inputs sensory-discriminative dimension of pain to primarily reflect pain processing in IC from other sensory modalities affect pain has often been associated with processing and ACC [11]. In addition, oscillatory re- processing. in primary and secondary somatosensory sponses have more recently been shown to cortices (SI and SII, respectively), where- reflect the modulation of pain within the Pain processing in the as the affective-motivational dimension pain matrix [13, 12]. In particular, oscilla- context of visual input has frequently been linked to processing tory responses in the gamma band (activ- in anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), insu- ity >30 Hz) seem to be a neuronal

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    6 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us