Author's Personal Copy

Author's Personal Copy

Author's personal copy Author's personal copy Author's personal copy Author's personal copy 50 O. Keynan, R. Yosef / Behavioural Processes 85 (2010) 47–51 Fig. 3. The chances (%) of a meal worm to be impaled and cached for a series of 1–10 worms given for males (N = 9) and females (N = 4) during the non-breeding season (A), during courtship (N = 17 pairs, B), during incubation (N = 13 pairs, C) and during the rearing of nestlings (N = 18 pairs, D). sexes fed the nestlings with most of the meal worms—males usually Although we did not find evidence that male shrikes left impaled fed the females that then fed the nestlings (males—R2 = 0.26, N = 18, prey uneaten as a signal for quality, the impaling of mice near df = 9, f = 3.03, p = 0.12; females—R2 = 0.001, N = 18, df = 9, f = 0.007, the territory borders implies that it certainly served a communi- p = 0.98, Fig. 3D) the same trend continued during the fledgling cation purpose. Shrikes left the mouse uneaten for a relatively long period and both sexes engaged in feeding the young. time, which suggests that it served territorial demarcation with conspecifics, as reported by Sloane (1991) for Loggerhead shrikes 4. Discussion and Antczak et al. (2005) for Great Grey Shrikes. Impaling behavior changed with the onset of the breeding sea- Shrikes in our study did not leave uneaten prey in conspicuous son when females joined male territories. Males used the mouse for larders and did not possess any preferred cache sites. It appears that courtship feeding (Lack, 1940) and used the impaling of the mouse Shrikes in the Arava cannot afford leaving a prey as advertisement as a ritual for pair bonding. Male Shrikes offer gifts to females as for male quality as described for conspecifics in Sede Boker (Yosef part of courtship or precopulatory display (Lefrank and Worfolk, and Pinshow, 1989). This is probably due to the scarcity of prey 1997; Yosef, 2008); the size of the gift is considered a signal of male in the hyper-arid environment of the Arava valley. The amount of quality (Tryjanowski and Hromada, 2005). Females gain direct fit- rainfall has a direct effect on food availability and on the breed- ness benefits during this courtship feeding since they learn of the ing success of birds (Lack, 1948; Rodriguez and Bustamante, 2003; males’ hunting capabilities, and the feeding of high quality food can Morrison and Bolger, 2002) and is also true for shrikes at our study increase reproductive success (Yosef and Pinshow, 1989). site (Keynan and Yosef, 2010). It seems that the fact that no impaled During incubation, when females were dependent on their prey was left uneaten is due to the same reasons. Valera et al. (2001) males for food, only males cached and fed their mates after they ate found that the Lesser Grey Shrike (L. minor) hoards only when their the mouse head. However, when there were nestlings, both sexes immediate food needs are satisfied. Impaled food is also a subject to impaled mice and fed them at the nest. During the entire breed- kleptoparasitism; on two occasions we observed Arabian Babblers ing season, impaling served only for dismemberment of large prey, (Turdoides squamiceps) feed on impaled prey. and we found no evidence for the use of this behavior for any addi- Although it appears that caches do not serve as signals for male tional purpose. These findings are consistent with Antczak et al. quality in our population, our findings confirm the prediction that (2005) for the spatio-temporal changes in impaling by Great Grey the impaling pattern would change temporally between the sexes, Shrike in Poland. and will serve for communications during the non-breeding and The correlation between the number of worms given to the courtship periods. shrike, and the chance a meal worm would be impaled, showed The impaling behavior of female shrikes during the non- that shrikes cached food only after they were satiated. This also breeding season has not been reported to date. Southern Grey suggests that in the hyper-arid environment, abundance of food Shrikes are monomorphic (Infante and Peris, 2004) and we found no is the main factor that limits caching of food. It seems that food differences in impaling behavior between male and female shrikes scarcity in our region is not conducive to caching because shrikes during the non-breeding season. Females defended their territories rarely catch large prey items, or many small ones in large num- and impaled mice on the borders of the territories, similar to the bers, and are unable to cross the “impaling threshold” (Valera et al., behavior of males. 2001) in the extreme desert conditions of our study area. Author's personal copy O. Keynan, R. Yosef / Behavioural Processes 85 (2010) 47–51 51 The correlation between the amount of food and hoarding ended female mate choice. Proceedings of the Forty-fifth Annual Meeting of the Zoo- earlier for males than females during the breeding season, since logical Society of Israel. Isr. J. Ecol. Evol. Keynan, O., Yosef, R., 2010. Annual precipitation affects reproduction of the Southern males fed the females during this period. A similar pattern was Grey Shrike (Lanius meridionalis). Wilson J. Ornithol. 122, 334–339. also observed for Bull Headed Shrikes (L. bucephalus; Yamagishi Lack, D., 1940. Courtship feeding in birds. Auk 57, 169–178. and Saito, 1985). Lack, D., 1948. The significance of clutch size. Part 3. Some interspecific comparisons. Ibis 90, 25–45. In conclusion, although food scarcity in our hyper-arid study site Lefrank, N., Worfolk, T., 1997. Guide to the Shrikes of the World. Yale University did not allow shrikes to create conspicuous caches of uneaten food, Press, p. 192. impaling did serve for communication and territorial demarcation Morrison, S.A., Bolger, D.T., 2002. Variation in Sparrow’s reproductive success with during different parts of the annual cycle. Our findings support our rainfall: food and predator-mediated processes. Oecologia 133, 315–324. Nikolov, B.P., Kodzhabashev, N.D., Popov, V.V., 2002. Diet composition and spatial hypothesis that the behavioral patterns of impaling changes tem- patterns of food caching in wintering Great Grey Shrike (Lanius excubitor)in porally and is sex-dependent, and that caching is correlated to the Bulgaria. Biol. Lett. 41 (2), 119–133. amount of available food. Probst, R., Wegleitner, S., Schmid, R., 2003. Relationship of vertebrate prey size to transport mode and distance in the Northern Shrike. Wilson Bull. 115 (2), 201–204. Acknowledgements Rodriguez, C., Bustamante, J., 2003. The effect of weather on Lesser Kestrel breeding success: can climate change explain historical population declines? J.Anim. Ecol. 72, 793–810. We acknowledge the help of Amotz Zahavi from the Faculty Sangster, G., Knox, A.G., Helbig, A.J., Parkin, D.T., 2002. Taxonomic recommendations of Life Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Israel; Ido Izhaki and Moshe for European birds. Ibis 144, 153–159. Inbar from the Faculty of Life Sciences and Science Education, Haifa Sloane, S., 1991. The Shrikes display advertising. Nat. Hist. 91 (6), 32–38. Smith, S.M., 1973. Food manipulation by young Passerines and the possible evolu- University-Oranim, Israel; Roni Ostreicher and the staff of Hazeva tionary history of impaling by Shrikes. Wilson Bull. 85 (3), 318–322. Field Study Center; and Harel Ben Shachar of the Israel Nature Tryjanwski, P., Karg, M.K., Karg, J., 2003. Diet composition and prey choice by Reserves and Parks Authority. the Red-backed Shrike Lanius collurio in western Polland. Belg. J. Zool. 133, 157–162. Tryjanowski, P., Hromada, M., 2005. Do males of the Great Grey Shrike,Lanius excu- References bitor, trade food for extrapair copulations? Anim. Behav. 69, 529–533. UNESCO, 1977. Map of world distribution of arid lands. MAB, Technician Note 7. Anava, A., Kam, M., Shkolnik, A., Degen, A.A., 2000. Seasonal field metabolic rate and UNESCO, Paris. dietary intake in Arabian Babblers (Turdodies squamiceps) inhabiting extreme Valera, F., Kirstin, A., Hoi, H., 2001. Why does the Lesser Grey Shrike (Lanius minor) desert. Funct. Ecol. 14, 607–613. seldom store food? Determainants of impaling in an uncommon storing species. Antczak, M., Hromada, M., tryjanowski, P., 2005. Spatio temporal changes in Great Behavior 138, 1421–1436. Grey Shrike Lanius excubitor behavior: from food caching to communication Yamagishi, S., Saito, M., 1985. Function of courtship feeding in the Bull-headed Shrike signs. Ardea 93 (1), 101–107. Lanius bucephlus. J. Ethnopharmacol. 3, 113–121. Applegate, R.D., 1977. Possible ecological role of food caches of Loggerhead Shrikes. Yosef, R., Grubb Jr., T.C., 1992. Territory size influences nutritional condition in Auk 94, 192–199. non-breeding Loggerhead Shrikes: a ptilochronology approach. Conserv. Biol. Burton, K.M., 1999. Use of barbed wire by Loggerhead Shrikes (Lanius ludovicianus) 6, 447–449. to manipulate nest materials. Am. Midl. Nat. 142, 198–199. Yosef, R., Pinshow, B., 1988a. Nestling transfer in the Northern Shrike (Lanius excu- Cade, T.J., 1995. Shrikes as predators. Proc. West. Found. Verteb. Zool. 6, 1–5. bitor). Auk 105, 580–581. Carlson, A., 1983. Maximizing energy delivery to dependent young: a field Yosef, R., Pinshow, B., 1988b. Polyginy in Northern Shrike (Lanius excubitor) in Israel. experiment with Red Backed Shrikes (Lanius collurio). J. Anim. Ecol. 52 (3), Auk 105, 581–582. 697–704. Yosef, R., Pinshow, B., 1989. Cache size in Shrikes influences female mate choice and Fuisz, T.I., Yosef, R., 2001. Effect of habitat quality on the hunting behavior of breeding reproductive success. Auk 106, 418–421. Great Grey Shrikes (Lanius excubitor). Acta Zool. Acad. Sci. H 47, 311–320.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    5 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us