A 15-Year Longitudinal Study

A 15-Year Longitudinal Study

ORIGINAL ARTICLE Children’s Self-Reported Psychotic Symptoms and Adult Schizophreniform Disorder A 15-Year Longitudinal Study Richie Poulton, PhD; Avshalom Caspi, PhD; Terrie E. Moffitt, PhD; Mary Cannon, MD; Robin Murray, MD; HonaLee Harrington, BS Background: Childhood risk factors for the develop- diagnosis at age 26 years (odds ratio, 16.4; 95% confi- ment of adult schizophrenia have proved to have only dence interval, 3.9-67.8). In terms of attributable risk, modest and nonspecific effects, and most seem unre- 42% of the age-26 schizophreniform cases in the lated to the adult phenotype. We report the first direct cohort had reported 1 or more psychotic symptoms at examination of the longitudinal relationship between psy- age 11 years. Age-11 psychotic symptoms did not pre- chotic symptoms in childhood and adulthood. dict mania or depression at age 26 years, suggesting specificity of prediction to schizophreniform disorder. Methods: We analyzed prospective data from a birth co- The link between child and adult psychotic symptoms hort (N=761), in which children were asked about de- was not simply the result of general childhood psycho- lusional beliefs and hallucinatory experiences at age 11 pathology. years, and then followed up to age 26 years. Structured diagnostic interviews were employed at both ages and self- Conclusion: These findings provide the first evidence report of schizophreniform symptoms was augmented by for continuity of psychotic symptoms from childhood to other data sources at age 26 years. adulthood. Results: Self-reported psychotic symptoms at age 11 years predicted a very high risk of a schizophreniform Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:1053-1058 HE SEARCH for childhood ining available archival information, which risk factors has played a was usually collected for other purposes central role in the quest to (eg, birth records, home movies, school rec- uncover the etiologies of ords), and there are few sources of de- schizophrenia and related tailed psychological (as distinct from so- Tdisorders. Studies examining such risk fac- ciobehavioral) childhood information tors have mainly used retrospective case- available. It is important to determine control, prospective high-risk, or popu- whether continuity of symptoms over the lation-based cohort designs. Retrospective life course can be identified in psychosis, studies are prone to selection and recall as indeed it has been in depression.15 Sec- bias,1 and prospective high-risk studies2-5 ond, studies to date have addressed what tend to evaluate relatively small samples percentage of schizophrenic individuals of children, resulting in low statistical had a risk factor in their histories, but do power. Over the past decade, the “com- not address what percentage of children ing of age” of many birth cohort studies with the risk factor go on to develop From the Dunedin has seen the population-based cohort de- schizophrenia. With a notable excep- Multidisciplinary Health and sign gain prominence as the method of tion,13 the effect sizes of the childhood risk Development Research Unit, choice for investigating childhood risk fac- factors for schizophrenia identified so far University of Otago, Dunedin, tors for schizophrenia. Risk factors iden- are relatively small (odds ratios [ORs] of New Zealand (Dr Poulton); tified to date include obstetric complica- about 2),16 with positive predictive val- Department of Psychology, tions,6,7 place and time of birth,8 delayed ues for sociobehavioral risk factors of be- University of Wisconsin, motor and language development,9 cog- tween 3% and 5%.12,17 Third, specificity for Madison (Drs Caspi and Moffitt nitive risk factors,9-11 and social- schizophrenia has not been established. and Ms Harrington); and the 9,10,12-14 Institute of Psychiatry, behavioral risk factors. Many of the childhood risk factors iden- University of London, London, However, the extant evidence is char- tified for schizophrenia are also associ- England (Drs Caspi, Moffitt, acterized by 3 limitations. First, most re- ated with affective psychosis and affec- Cannon, and Murray). searchers have been constrained to exam- tive disorders, albeit to a lesser degree.18-20 (REPRINTED) ARCH GEN PSYCHIATRY/ VOL 57, NOV 2000 WWW.ARCHGENPSYCHIATRY.COM 1053 ©2000 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ on 09/26/2021 PARTICIPANTS AND METHODS on you?”; (4) “Have you heard voices other people can’t hear?”; and (5) “Has something ever gotten inside your body or has your body changed in some strange way?” The fifth SAMPLE question was not endorsed by any study member. The items were scored by the psychiatrist (0, no; 1, yes, likely; and 2, Participants were members of the Dunedin Multidisci- yes, definitely). plinary Health and Development Study, a longitudinal in- The responses to these 4 questions were added. The ma- vestigation of the health, development, and behavior of a jority of study members obtained a score of 0 (n=673), 13% birth cohort born between April 1, 1972, and March 31, obtained a score of 1 (n=103), and the remainder obtained 1973, in Dunedin, New Zealand.25 When the cohort was a score of 2 (n=11), 3 (n=1) or 4 (n=1). Given the re- first traced for follow-up at age 3 years, 91% of the eligible stricted range of scores the following groups of cases and con- births participated in the assessment, providing a base trols were created: group 1 (no symptoms [controls]) and a sample of 1037 (52% were boys). Cohort members repre- score of 0 [n=673; 50.2% were male]); group 2 (weak symp- sent the full range of socioeconomic status in the general toms and a score of 1 [n=103; 66.0% were male]); and group population of New Zealand’s South Island, and 85% iden- 3 (strong symptoms and a score of $2 [n=13; 61.5% were tify themselves as exclusively New Zealand European. The male]). At minimum, study members could enter the strong cohort was assessed at ages 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13, 15, 18, 21, symptom group by obtaining a score of 1 (yes, likely) for 2 and 26 years. We report data from the age-11 (1983- symptoms, or by obtaining a score of 2 (yes, definitely) for 1984) and age-26 assessments (1998-1999). 1 symptom. Two psychiatrists (M.C. and R.M.) and 2 clini- Analyses linking age-11 symptoms and age-26 schizo- cal psychologists (R.P. and T.E.M.) reviewed the psychia- phreniform outcomes required present psychiatric inter- trists’ notes and concurred that the symptoms seemed genu- view data at both ages. Psychiatric interviews were com- ine in content. The 3 age-11 risk groups (strong symptoms, plete at age 11 years for the 789 cohort members seen at the weak symptoms, and controls) did not differ by parental 28 Dunedin Unit (one quarter of the cohort was assessed at socioeconomic status (F2,784=1.1, P=.33) or on the school, and unfortunately did not see the psychiatrist). Psy- Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised Full- chiatric interviews assessing schizophreniform symptoms Scale IQ test (F2, 786=1.67, P=.18). were complete at age 26 years for 972 of the 1019 cohort members still living (95% were followed up). Age-26 out- PSYCHIATRIC STATUS AT AGE 26 YEARS comes were compared between those interviewed at both ages 11 and 26 years and included in this report (N=761) and At age 26 years, symptoms were ascertained using the DIS,29 those who had to be omitted because they were inter- administered by health professionals (blind to previous di- viewed at age 26 years but not at 11 years (n=211). Those agnosis) with a master’s degree at minimum. The reporting included vs omitted did not differ significantly on their rate period was 12 months before the interview. Diagnostic pro- 2 of psychiatric diagnosis in general (x 1=0.02, P=.89) nor on cedures, reliability, and validity of the DSM diagnoses made 2 30 prevalence of schizophreniform diagnosis (x 1=1.7, P=.19). in this sample have been reported previously. The 12- The prevalence of age-26 schizophreniform diagnosis was month prevalence of mental disorder in the Dunedin sample 3.7% in the full cohort and a comparable 3.3% in the 761 closely matches the 12-month prevalence for young adult cohort members analyzed here. Thus, study members whose subjects in the US National Comorbidity Survey.31 The pri- results are described in Tables 1 and 2 represent the full mary outcome for this study was schizophreniform disor- birth cohort with regard to adult psychiatric outcome. der (n=36 [3.7%], 25 of whom had been interviewed by the psychiatrist at age 11 years). This diagnosis incorporated sev- AGE-11 PREDICTORS eral changes to the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria,32 which were made to enhance diagnostic validity, as detailed below. At age 11 years, study members were, for the first time in the Dunedin Study, administered the Diagnostic Interview SCHIZOPHRENIFORM DIAGNOSIS Schedule for Children (DISC-C)24 for DSM-III26 by a child psychiatrist.27 The schizophrenia section of the DISC-C in- Criterion A terview asked 5 questions of study members: (1) “Some people believe in mind reading or being psychic. Have other Structured psychiatric interviews, such as the DIS, have been people ever read your mind?”; (2) “Have you ever had mes- criticized for identifying unexpectedly large numbers of cases sages sent just to you through television or radio?”; (3) “Have who endorse psychotic-type experiences and beliefs, some you ever thought that people are following you or spying of whom have clinical psychosis, but many of whom do A compelling childhood risk factor for adult schizo- ences at age 11 years predicted schizophrenic outcomes phrenia would have the following characteristics: it would 15 years later.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    6 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us