Overview of Propensity Score Analysis

Overview of Propensity Score Analysis

1 Overview of Propensity Score Analysis Learning Objectives zz Describe the advantages of propensity score methods for reducing bias in treatment effect estimates from observational studies zz Present Rubin’s causal model and its assumptions zz Enumerate and overview the steps of propensity score analysis zz Describe the characteristics of data from complex surveys and their relevance to propensity score analysis zz Enumerate resources for learning the R programming language and software zz Identify major resources available in the R software for propensity score analysis 1.1. Introduction The objective of this chapter is to provide the common theoretical foundation for all propensity score methods and provide a brief description of each method. It will also introduce the R software, point the readers toward resources for learning the R language, and briefly introduce packages available in R relevant to propensity score analysis. Draft ProofPropensity score- Doanalysis methodsnot aim copy, to reduce bias inpost, treatment effect or estimates distribute obtained from observational studies, which are studies estimating treatment effects with research designs that do not have random assignment of participants to condi- tions. The term observational studies as used here includes both studies where there is 1 Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 2 Practical Propensity Score Methods Using R no random assignment but there is manipulation of conditions and studies that lack both random assignment and manipulation of conditions. Research designs to estimate treatment effects that do not have random assignment to conditions are also referred as quasi-experimental or nonexperimental designs. In this book, the terms observa- tional study, quasi-experimental design, and nonexperimental design will used equivalently. Biased treatment effect estimates may occur due to nonrandom differences between treated and untreated groups with respect to covariates related to the outcome. Pro- pensity scores are probabilities of treatment assignment that, once estimated, can be used in several methods to reduce selection bias. These propensity score methods include many variations of weighting, matching, and stratification. Propensity score methods achieve removal of bias by balancing covariate distributions between treated and untreated groups. Propensity score analysis methods have become a common choice for estimating treatment effects with nonexperimental data in the social sciences (Thoemmes & Kim, 2011). The use of propensity scores to reduce selection bias in nonexperimental studies was proposed by Rosenbaum and Rubin (1983b) and was connected to earlier work by Rubin (1973) on matching methods for selecting a untreated group that was similar to the treated group with respect to covariates. Propensity scores solve a difficult prob- lem with multivariate matching: If there are many covariates, it is difficult to find an appropriate match for each treatment participant with respect to all covariates. With propensity scores, each individual has a unique score that summarizes the relation- ship between covariates and the treatment assignment. Rosenbaum and Rubin (1983b) have shown that adjustment for the propensity score is sufficient to remove all bias related to covariates. Propensity score matching, stratification, and weighting have several advantages over conditioning on covariates. First, they separate the process of reduction of selec- tion bias from the analysis of outcomes. Rubin (2005, 2007) refers to the reduction of selection bias with propensity score methods as the “design” stage of study. This design stage consists of the determination of matched observations, strata, or weights that achieve balance of covariate distributions between treated and untreated groups and should be performed independently and without any knowledge of the outcomes. Second, matching, stratification, and weighting allow for smaller outcome models where fewer parameters are estimated, because covariates are not included in the model unless they are of theoretical interest. Third, because the process of balancing covariates between treated and untreated groups is done independently of the out- come, no assumptions are made about the functional form of the relationship between covariates and the outcome. Draft Proof1.2. Rubin’s - Do Causal not Model copy, post, or distribute Rubin (1974) proposed a framework to understand the problem of causal inference, which has been referred to in the literature as the potential outcomes framework, counterfactual Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. Chapter 1 Overview of Propensity Score Analysis 3 framework, or Rubin’s causal model (Holland, 1986; Shadish, 2010). In this book, the latter term will be used. Rubin’s causal model has been very influential across a variety of fields concerned with causal inference, such as statistics, economics, education, psy- chology, sociology, and epidemiology. Rubin’s causal model provides the theoretical justification for estimation of treatment effects based on weighting (see Chapter 3), stratification (see Chapter 4), and matching (see Chapter 5). 1.2.1. Potential Outcomes In Rubin’s causal model, all individuals in the population have potential outcomes associated with the presence of treatment and potential outcomes in the absence 1 of treatment. More specifically, each individual i has a potential outcome Yi asso- ciated with participating in the treatment condition ()Zi = 1 and a potential out- 0 come Yi if not participating ()Zi = 0. Therefore, the treatment effect for each 10 individual is τ ii=YY − i . However, the outcomes of the participants are only observed in the presence of the treatment condition; conversely, the outcomes of nonpartici- pants are only observed in the absence of the treatment. This idea is illustrated in Figure 1.1. FIGURE 1.1 l Potential Outcomes of Participants and Nonparticipants of a Treatment Potential Outcome Potential Outcome if participant if nonparticipant participants 1 0 = Yi | Zi = 1 Yi | Zi 1 1 0 = Nonparticipants Yi | Zi = 0 YZii| 0 1.2.2. Types of Treatment Effects 1 0 |)Z = 0 In Figure 1.1, the outcomes (Yi |)Zi = 1 and (Yi i are observed, while the outcomes 1 = and 0 are missing. Based on this framework, dif- (Yi |)Zi 0 (Yi |)Zi = 1 ferent types of treatment effect can be defined: (1) The average treatment effect (ATE) is the difference between the outcomes of the individuals in the treated and 10 untreated conditions: ATEE=()YEi − ()Yi . (2) The average treatment effect on the treated (ATT) is the difference between the expected value of the observed outcomes of treated individuals and the expected value of the potential outcomes of the treated Draft Proof - Do10 not copy, post, or distribute individuals: ATTE=(|YZi ii = 11)(−=EY |)Zi . Therefore, this effect only refers to the population of participants. (3) The average treatment effect on the untreated (ATC) is the difference between the expected value of the outcomes of the untreated Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 4 Practical Propensity Score Methods Using R individuals and the expected value of the potential outcomes of the untreated 10 individuals: ATCE=(|YZi ii = 00)(−=EY |)Zi . The choice between the type of treat- ment effects should depend on the research question and related literature and also whether assumptions are met for the treatment effect of interest. In experimental designs, the ATE is equal to the ATT and ATC because random assignment of participants to con- 1 1 ditions implies that they are exchangeable and therefore EY(|i Zi = 1) = EY(|i Zi = 0) 0 0 and EY(|i Zi = 1) = EY(|i Zi = 0). In nonexperimental designs, the ATE, ATT, and ATC could differ substantially. 1.2.3. Assumptions Estimating unbiased treatment effects requires the assumption of strong ignorabil- ity of treatment assignment, which consists of assuming that the treatment assign- ment is independent of the potential outcome distributions, given observed covariates X: (,YY01)|⊥ ZX(Rosenbaum & Rubin, 1983b). Obtaining adequate balance of covari- ate distributions between treated and untreated groups after matching, stratifica- tion, and weighting is evidence that strong ignorability of treatment assignment has been achieved given the observed covariates. This assumption also requires that for every value of the covariates X, the probability of treatment assignment is neither 0 nor 1: 01<<pZ(|i X).1 Estimation of treatment effects under Rubin’s causal model also requires the stable unit treatment value assumption (SUTVA), which states that there is a unique t value Yi corresponding to unit i and treatment t (Rosenbaum & Rubin, 1983b). This one-to-one correspondence between potential outcome and treatment version has a couple of implications: First, the distribution of potential outcomes for one individual is independent of the potential treatment status of another individual. Second, there are no unrepresented versions of the treatment (Rubin, 1986). It is common that implementations of propensity score methods either assume full treatment compliance (i.e., adherence) or estimate the effect of offering

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    18 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us