Embodied Geometry: Signs and Gestures Used in the Deaf Mathematics Classroom – the Case of Symmetry

Embodied Geometry: Signs and Gestures Used in the Deaf Mathematics Classroom – the Case of Symmetry

Embodied Geometry: signs and gestures used in the deaf mathematics classroom – the case of symmetry Christina M. Krause University of Duisburg-Essen Cite as Krause, C.M. (2018). Embodied Geometry: Signs and gestures used in the deaf mathematics classroom – the case of symmetry. In R. Hunter, M. Civil, B. Herbel-Eisenmann, N. Planas, D. Wagner (Eds.), Mathematical discourse that breaks barriers and creates space for marginalized learners (pp. 171-193). Rotterdam: Sense. 1 Embodied Geometry (Krause, 2018) Embodied Geometry: signs and gestures used in the deaf mathematics classroom – the case of symmetry By identifying the differences and similarities in the practices of those whose knowledge of the world is mediated through different sensory channels, we might not only become better able to respond to their particular needs, but also build more robust understandings of the relationships between experience and cognition more generally. (Healy, 2015, p. 289) Research in the field of deaf studies shows that learning mathematics appears to be more difficult for deaf children than it is for those who can hear. Deaf students’ basic math skills are found to lag several years on average behind those of hearing learners of same age (e.g. Kelly, Lang, & Pagliaro, 2003; Nunes, 2004; Pagliaro, 2006; Traxler, 2000). This is partly explained by a lack of informal mathematical knowledge typically gained by hearing children implicitly through everyday interactions in early childhood (Nunes & Moreno, 1998). Furthermore, deaf students struggle with reading, understanding and processing word problems (Hyde, Zevenbergen, & Power, 2003) since they have not had sensory access to the language in which the problems are written so that this language is a foreign language for them. These previous studies mainly compare the learning products of deaf students with those of hearing students but rarely focus on the learning processes. If we assume that mathematical knowledge becomes shaped by processes of meaning making and that mathematical thinking is influenced by our interaction with the world, I suggest that it might be naïve to assume that the learning product will be the same for deaf students, considering the circumstances of learning are not. One major difference between social learning processes in regular and deaf classrooms concerns the modality of language, with spoken language being used in the first case and sign language in the latter, each with its specific characteristics. Building on a Vygotskian approach, Healy (2015) claimed that the sensory channels by which we perceive information deeply influence the structure and process of thinking so that the substitution of the ear by the eye when interacting with others in the mathematics classroom may also influence what kind of mathematical knowledge is constructed. So, “rather than seeing difference as equated to a state of deficiency, difference can be treated as just that, difference” (Healy, 2015, p. 291). Taking this difference into account may help us get a better understanding of learning processes also more generally. This chapter points out some of the differences in the way learning is experienced when sound is no longer a primary sensory channel and discusses possible consequences for the learning of mathematics in the deaf classroom. In doing so, it aims at drawing attention to the importance of taking a more thorough look at the specific situation that is faced by deaf learners (and their teachers), focusing on the influence of sign language as an important component of the process of mathematical meaning making in social interaction. To approach this goal, theory gained from non-mathematics-specific studies on sign language will be introduced, namely the consideration of iconicity as one feature of sign languages that may influence conceptualization (Grote, 2013). In this chapter, I present a case study from a fifth grade geometry classroom with all students as well as the teacher being deaf and communicating in German sign Language. I will draw on three examples to reconstruct how two ‘mathematical signs’1 develop to their use in the classroom together with the mathematical ideas they refer to and how this forms processes of iconization, that is processes in which iconic relationships between the signs and their respective referenced idea become established. This investigation is especially important considering that there are rarely conventions about ‘mathematical signs’ to refer to a mathematical idea. They are often more or less idiosyncratic to the teacher, especially when it comes to more abstract concepts in upper grades. It is therefore important to shed light on the signs that are used, ‘where they come from’, and what might be implied by the iconicity between these signs and the mathematical idea. Based on the analyses, I will discuss possible theoretical implications for the learning of mathematics within a theoretical framework that sees social, semiotic, and individual approaches to learning as being deeply intertwined, described in the next section. Learning mathematics between the social, the semiotic, and the individual In the mathematics classroom, mathematical ideas and objects are mainly encountered and discussed in interaction among students and the teacher. Learning mathematics can therefore be considered a social phenomenon in which individuals co-construct mathematical meaning. But what influences this construction? One aspect may concern the semiotic nature of the social 1 It should be noted that the term ‘sign’ can be used generically, in the semiotic sense, and specifically to refer to morphological units of signed languages which are roughly the equivalents of words or short phrases. While the meaning in any particular case should also be clear from the context I point out that it is only referred to ‘signs’ in a semiotic sense in the section dealing with theoretical assumptions on learning mathematics, “Learning mathematics between the social, the semiotic, and the individual”. 3 Embodied Geometry (Krause, 2018) learning process, such that mathematical objects cannot be accessed directly but only mediated through (semiotic) signs (Seeger, 2006). These signs may be of spoken, written or gestural form, or may be multimodal in their nature and processed through different sensory channels (Arzarello, 2006). When interacting by means of these (semiotic) signs, they need to be interpreted in order to react to them, which makes interaction a constant process of interpreting and responding to each other based on this interpretation (Bikner-Ahsbahs, 2006; Krause, 2016). Gestures form a specific kind of these signs – “idiosyncratic spontaneous movement[s] of the hands and arms accompanying speech” (McNeill, 1992, p. 37) that are not performing any physical action such as writing, scratching and so on. On the social side, they are part of the orchestration of (semiotic) signs that form the multimodal utterances that shape social interaction and can play an active part in epistemic processes (Dreyfus, Sabena, Kidron, & Arzarello, 2014; Krause, 2016). On the individual side, they are an embodied means of expression in which thinking can become manifest (Goldin-Meadow, 2003). Furthermore, they are considered to contribute to the formation of “the embodied mind” (Varela, Thompson & Rosch 1991), under the assumption that [m]eaning is in many ways socially constructed, but, it is not arbitrary. It is subject to constraints which arise from biological embodied processes that take place in the ongoing interaction between mutually constituted sensemakers and the medium in which they exist. (Núñez, Edwards, & Matos, 1999, p. 53, italics in the original) In recent years, increasing attention has been turned toward the theory of embodied cognition, thus valuing the role of the body in mathematical thinking and learning (see Edwards, Ferrara, & Moore-Russo, 2014 for a comprehensive overview and ‘emerging perspectives’). Following this approach, our (mathematical) thinking is crucially influenced by our physical being in the world, in which bodily experience is considered a core source for all conceptual understanding (Lakoff & Núñez, 2000; Nemirovsky, 2003) and this thinking, vice versa, becomes embodied (Arzarello 2006; Edwards, 2009; Goldin-Meadow, 2003). Embodied signs may thus be seen as a meeting point for social and individual learning, being shaped by and themselves shaping mathematical thought and social interaction. For deaf learners, the embodied approach and the role of bodily means of expression takes on even greater significance than for hearing learners. On the one hand, the learning process within social interaction is highly shaped by visual signs since the language used is a visual one. Mathematics literally needs to become visual in the deaf mathematics classroom. On the other hand, the bodily experiences that shape conceptual thinking are different for deaf learners. This has already been considered by Healy and colleagues (Healy, 2015; Healy, Ramos, Fernandes, & Botelho Peixoto, 2016), who understand the sensory channels as instrument in the sense of Vygotsky. According to Vygotsky “the eye, like the ear, is an instrument that can be substituted by another” (Vygotsky, 1997, p. 83, in Healy, 2015, p. 291), but this substitution also involves a change in the process and structure of thinking (Vygotsky, 1981, in Healy, 2015). Healy and colleagues claim that To better understand the deaf mathematics learners, we need to better understand what it means

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    24 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us