Variation of Telencephalon Morphology of the Threespine Stickleback (Gasterosteus Aculeatus) in Relation to Inferred Ecology

Variation of Telencephalon Morphology of the Threespine Stickleback (Gasterosteus Aculeatus) in Relation to Inferred Ecology

doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2010.01987.x Variation of telencephalon morphology of the threespine stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) in relation to inferred ecology P. J. PARK & M. A. BELL Department of Ecology and Evolution, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA Keywords: Abstract ecomorphology; We tested the hypothesis that increased telencephalon size has evolved in fish; threespine stickleback fish (Gasterosteus aculeatus) from structurally complex forebrain; habitats using field-caught samples from one sea-run (ancestral) and 18 habitat complexity; ecologically diverse freshwater (descendant) populations. Freshwater habitats morphometrics; ranged from shallow, structurally complex lakes with benthic-foraging neuroanatomy; stickleback (benthics), to deeper, structurally simple lakes in which stickleback spatial learning; depend more heavily on plankton for prey (generalists). Contrary to our telencephalon. expectations, benthics had smaller telencephala than generalists, but the shape of the telencephalon of the sea-run and benthic populations were more convex laterally. Convex telencephalon shape may indicate enlargement of the dorsolateral region, which is homologous with the tetrapod hippocampus. Telencephalon morphology is also sexually dimorphic, with larger, less convex telencephala in males. Freshwater stickleback from structurally complex habitats have retained the ancestral telencephalon morphology, but popula- tions that feed more in open habitats on plankton have evolved larger, laterally concave telencephala. Burns & Rodd, 2008; Burns et al., 2009; Gonda et al., Introduction 2009) have considered differences in telencephalon Understanding evolution of the vertebrate telencephalon morphology among ecologically contrasting conspecific is crucial to link behavioural ecology with neurobiology. populations of fishes. We used field-caught samples from Because of the high metabolic cost of brain tissue, brain Alaskan populations of threespine stickleback (Gasteros- morphology should reflect functional importance for teus aculeatus) to study the relevance of the CFH to complex behaviours (Dukas, 1999). The Clever Foraging telencephalon morphology on a well-known adaptive Hypothesis (CFH) states that active predators, unlike radiation in fishes. grazers, have evolved larger brains because greater The CFH emphasizes the importance of foraging ecology neurobiological capability is needed to search ‘strategi- in driving neuroanatomical change, and its predictions cally’ for their food (Parker & Gibson, 1977; Striedter, have been confirmed in mammals (see Jolicoeur et al., 2005). The CFH has been proposed to explain the 1984; Gittleman, 1986; Hutcheon et al., 2002; Striedter, evolution of complex brain morphology and behaviour 2005) and birds (see Healy et al., 2005). In bats, insecti- (Parker & Gibson, 1977; Striedter, 2005). In fishes, the vores rely predominantly on echolocation to catch insect relationship between brain size and diet or habitat has prey whereas frugivores, which have larger brains, must been studied in sharks (Yopak et al., 2007) and cichlids locate and time the ripeness of fruits (Eisenberg & Wilson, (Huber et al., 1997; Pollen et al., 2007; Gonzalez-Voyer 1978; Jolicoeur et al., 1984; Hutcheon et al., 2002). In et al., 2009) and was reviewed by Kotrschal et al. (1998) primates (Clutton-Brock & Harvey, 1980) and small and Ito et al. (2007). However, only a few studies (see mammals (Mace et al., 1981), insectivores and frugivores have larger relative brain size than grazers. In the Carniv- Correspondence: Peter J. Park, Department of Ecology and Evolution, ora, larger brain size is associated with species that have Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY 11794-5245, USA. Tel.: (631) 632 8574; fax: (631) 632 7626; complicated foraging strategies (Gittleman, 1986). Fur- e-mail: [email protected] thermore, unlike simpler learning strategies that lack ª 2010 THE AUTHORS. J. EVOL. BIOL. 23 (2010) 1261–1277 JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2010 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY 1261 1262 P. J. PARK AND M. A. BELL neuroanatomical correlates, spatial learning during forag- (a) (b) ing is associated with a larger hippocampus in the forebrain of tetrapods (O’Keefe & Nadel, 1978; Sherry, 1998; Healy et al., 2005). The gross morphology of teleost brain lobes provides quantifiable measures of how a species has adapted to its environment (Davis & Miller, 1967; Kotrschal et al., 1998). It is involved in numerous functions, including classical conditioning (Flood et al., 1976; Overmier & Hollis, 1983), processing sensory information (Davis (c) & Kassel, 1983; Hofmann, 2001), aggression (Davis & Kassel, 1983), solving complex social tasks (Huber et al., 1997; Kotrschal et al., 1998; Hofmann, 2001), mating and reproduction (Demski & Beaver, 2001), schooling (Davis & Kassel, 1983; Shinozuka & Watanabe, 2004), avoid- ance learning (Portavella et al., 2003, 2004; Vargas et al., 2009) and spatial learning (Vargas et al., 2000, 2009; Broglio et al., 2003). Teleosts that forage in structurally Fig. 1 The threespine stickleback brain. (a) Dorsal view (anterior complex Hawaiian reefs have larger telencephala than is up). From anterior to posterior, olfactory nerves, olfactory bulbs sedentary species (Bauchot et al., 1977), supporting the (olf), forebrain (fb), pineal gland (pg), optic lobes (opt), cerebellum CFH. The telencephalon is also larger in African cichlid (cb) and hindbrain (hb). Ventral structures (pituitary gland and species that forage in complex, benthic microhabitats hypothalamus) are not shown. (b) Forty-lm Nissl-stained cross- section of telencephalon at level of a section (S) in A (dorsal is up). than in those from pelagic microhabitats (van Staaden The three major teleost pallial subregions are the dorsomedial (Dm), et al., 1994 ⁄ 95; Huber et al., 1997; Pollen et al., 2007). dorsodorsal (Dd) and dorsolateral (Dl). Endorhinal fissure (ef) and The threespine stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus)isan ventral edge of sulcus limitans telencephali (slt) are shown. (c) excellent species in which to test the CFH. Like other Dorsal view (anterior is left) of the telencephalon showing fixed teleosts, its brain consists of a pair of olfactory bulbs; a (1, 14) and semi-landmarks (circles) used in this study. pair of forebrain, optic and hypothalamus lobes; and a single cerebellum (Nieuwenhuys, 1959, 1962; Segaar & have a deep head and body, smaller eyes and fewer, Nieuwenhuys, 1963; Ekstro¨ m & van Veen, 1983, 1984; shorter gill rakers (reviewed in Bell & Foster, 1994; Ekstro¨ m et al., 1986, 1990; Fig. 1a,b). The teleost fore- McPhail, 1994; Walker, 1997). brain is divided into the telencephalon and diencepha- According to the CFH, foraging on benthos should be lon. The telencephalon can be further subdivided into associated with larger telencephalon size among three- three pallial regions dorsally and two subpallial regions spine stickleback populations. However, unlike shallow ventrally. Pallial regions are generally considered homol- lakes that include exclusively benthic habitat, deep lakes ogous across teleost species, but determining homologies include both littoral habitat near shore and limnetic among nuclei within each region remains challenging habitat off shore. Therefore, like benthics, limnetics may (Nieuwenhuys, 1998b). rely on spatial information within benthic microhabitats, There are three major forms of threespine stickleback, especially during the breeding season, when mating marine, sea-run and freshwater, and the sea-run form occurs near shore. We sampled lake populations from the has founded innumerable freshwater populations (e.g. Matanuska-Susitna Valley (n = 15) and Kenai Peninsula Bell & Foster, 1994; Bell, 1995). Sea-run threespine (n = 3), Alaska. Each lake sample was collected once stickleback populations are distributed worldwide in within daylight hours during the breeding season (late temperate coastal environments and typically exhibit May – mid-June). Other sample properties are summa- limited variation for armour traits (Colosimo et al., 2005) rized in Table 2. All populations were classified as benthic and body shape (Walker & Bell, 2000). They feed on or relatively limnetic based on a series of indirect plankton at sea (Mackney & Hughes, 1995) and have measurements, and telencephalon size and shape were trophic morphology resembling limnetics (McPhail, analysed. However, stomach content analysis (M. Travis, 1994). Adaptation to a wide range of freshwater habitats unpublished data) revealed that many putatively lim- has generated highly divergent morphological pheno- netic populations, based on indirect evidence, consume types among lake populations (reviewed in Bell & Foster, both plankton and benthic prey. Therefore, we refer to 1994; Reimchen, 1994; McPhail, 1994; Walker, 1997). them as ‘generalists.’ Based on existing information on Threespine stickleback from shallow, densely vegetated teleost fishes and without prior knowledge of ecotypic lakes (i.e. benthics) forage mostly on benthic inverte- telencephalon differences in stickleback, we hypothe- brates (Schluter & McPhail, 1992; Hart & Gill, 1994). In sized that benthics rely relatively more on spatial infor- contrast, plankton specialists (i.e. limnetics) eat plankton mation, and therefore should have larger relative in open water. Compared to limnetics, benthics generally telencephalon sizes than generalists. ª 2010 THE AUTHORS. J. EVOL. BIOL. 23 (2010) 1261–1277 JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2010 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    17 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us