Pride, Pity, Anger, Guilt: Thought-Affect Sequences in the Classroom

Pride, Pity, Anger, Guilt: Thought-Affect Sequences in the Classroom

DOCONENT NESONE ED 210,614 CG 015 '639 &UTHOR Weiner, Be"nard , TITLE Pride, Pity, Aver, Guilt: Thought-Affect Sequences in the Classrcom. PDXS AGEJJC! Spencer Foufdation, Chicago, Ill. PUEDATE Apr 81 , 47p.: Paper presented At the Annual Meeting of the NOTE . Western Psycho;ogical Association (61st, Los Angeles, CA, April 9-12, 1981): . e , . EDFS PRICE . MF01)PCO2 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS *Affective Behavior: 'Attribution Theory: Cognitive Processes: *Emotional Response: Interpersonal . Relationship: Literature Reviews:.*Iccus cf Ccntrol: Motivation: *personality: sychological Patterns: -Self Concept: Sttte of thert Reviews /rENTIFIERS *Emotions ., ABSTRACT A set of prevalent tmotions, including pity, anger, guilt, pride (self-esteem) , gratitude, and resignation, .shares a common characteristic, i.e., causal attributions appear tc to suffiacient antecedents for their elicitation. Research in thefield of emotions has shown that the underlying properties cr dimensions of attributions are the significantdeterminatts of these affective reactions. This relationshivis pertinent to a number of issues in the 'study of eaotion. The literature suggests that &Motion takes a Of formats, 'but all varieties rely on the verbal reports and judgments of the participants. Future resrch should include a systematic study of human emotions prevalent in everyday life, partly guided by the phenomenological method, and directed ty the-relief, that cognitions are sufficient antegtedents for feeling states. (JAC) S *#,10011****1141110110#111#10111#**111#10011***1011*****011011*41111********2430**************** * Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made, **. from the original documeft. ****************4***********4,***************************************** 3 4k O PRIDE, PITY, ANGER, tUILT: THOUGHT-AFFECT. SEQUENCES IN THE'CLASSROOM 4 Bernard Weiner University of California, Los Angeles 4 U.S. DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION "PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS NATIONAL INSTITUTE OF EDUCATION MATERIAL HAS,BEEN GRANTED BY EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC) _ The document has been reprodOc.ed as received from the -person or oroeruzeion rt MoornRbng chores, have been made to improve redroducten qualm. TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES Points of WWI or opinions stated in the docu *. INFORMATION CENTER(ERIC)' matt do not nwsesenty represent official NIE posibon or pace 0 Paper presented at-the Anuual Meeting of the Western Psychological Assocation, CD Los Angeles, CA, April 1981. '41 a 1 S I Pride, Pity, Anger, Guilt: Thought-Affect Sequences _in the Classroom Bernard'Weiner University of California, Los Angeles 1. A set of prevalent emotions, knOuding pity, anger, guilt,pride,(self esteem), gratitude, and resignation (hopelessness) share a common characteristic: Causal attributions appear to be sufficient antecedentsfor their elicitation. Furthermore, the underlying properties or dimensions of attributions are the siinificant determinants of these affective reactions. The evidence supporting these assertions regarding cognitionemotion linkages is examined in this paper. First, causal attributions and their underlying propereiei are discus4ed. The relations between the dimensions of causality and the emotions indicated above then are presented. This is followed by a brief consideration of the pertinence of this approach to a t number of issues within the field of emotion. It will be evident to the readers, as itis to this writer, that a myriad of problems remain tobe resolved giventhis attributional analysts of emotional states. No attempt is made to hidethese difficulties. On the other handOt 'is hoped that it will be. evidentto the readers, as it is to the writer, that this attributional analysisfacilitates the undetltanding of some important;;`. emotional rperiences. Causal Attributions A The guiding principle.of attribution theory isthat individuals search # .6 for understanding, seeking to discover why an eventhas occurred (Heidel. 1958); Kelley, 1967; Weiner, 19,0a). Attrib40.onal searchcan be considered one -NA '.. 3 . instance of the more general class of exploratory activities, and attribution 4 theory therefore falls within the broad studyof cognitive functionalism. It is now recognized that thissearch,is most evident when an outcome is unexpected (e.g., failur*%wen success is anticipated) and when desiresave / not been fulfilled g., achievement goals are not reached; there is fl interpersonal-rejection;tee Folkes, 1: ; Lau & Rup sell, 1979; Wong & Weiner,'1981). / As intimated above, causal search is not contined'to any single motivational domain. Individuals desire to.know; for'example, why their . team has been defeated (an achievement concern; Lau & Russell, 1979),- why is P'ess they have h.en refused fora date (aeaffiliative concern; Folkes,,,A ), and why thVave lost an election (a power concern! Kingdon,'1967). The number --4/, of perceived causes is virtually infinite, although the vast majority of answers tot the above questions are selected from a rather circumscribed array. In achievement' situations, success andifailure typically are ascri)edto ability (including both aptitude and learnedskills),Isomeaspect of-motivation (such as aheit- or long-term effortlexpenditure, attention), others {friends, family),:physiological faCiors (e.g.; mood/Aatyrity, health); the difficulty or the ease o,f the task, and luck (see Cooper & Burger, Note 1). Ia an Sffil4ative-context"acceftanCe or rejection of a dating request often is ascribed to prior behaviors(e.g., making a good impression, being too torwaid), physical appearance, and the desites or "state.of the potential date Pi A/elk (wanting to goout, having a boyfrieid or prior engagement!. see Folkes, ).' . And given a poritIcAl contest, eiection'or defeat tends to be attributed to party identification, the personality characteristics of the Candidates, and their stances OnVsues(Kingdon, 1967)- 3 w AO . Inasmuch as the potential list of causes is considerable within any motivational domain, 'and because tie specific causesdifferabetween domains, it is essential to create a clkssification scheme or'a taxonomyof causes. In so doing, the underlying properties of the causes areascertained and their similarities and differences cat be determined.,C uses that ,denote ively differ (e.g., intelligence as acause of achievement succesl, physicsbeauty as a cause of affiliative svecess and personality,as a cause li of pol ical success) may be connotatively quitesimilarl(eN., intelligence, beauty and personality, among other similarities, all refer torelatively enduring personal properties). The discovery of these bases for comparison, which are referred tohere as caulardimensions; is anindispensabld requirement for the construction of a relatively, generalattributional theory of emotion. Causal pimensions Two methods of arriving'at new knowledge, that,Isomewhat wantonly label dialectic and demonstrative (following Rychlak,1968), have been used to . determine the basic dimensions of causality. The dialectic approach has I ,. involved a logical ,grouping of causes, discovery of an apparentcontradiction in reasoning, and the emergence of a newdimension of causality to resolve the uncovered inconsistency. This logical and introspective examinationwithin . ' c the attributional domain initiated with adifferentiation betwen causes located within the person, such as intelligence,physical beauty and personality, and causes outside of the person, such as thedifficulty of a task, the prior engagement of a desired partnerand the popularity of one's opponent. The internal-external distinction is primarily associated withRotter's (1966) . t discussion of locus of control. However,this causal dimension has been f's 5 4 captured with various other labels, such as person-environment or disposition-situation, and is evident, in contrasts between origin-pawn . (deCharms, 1968), intrinsic' - extrinsic motivation (Deci, 1975), and freedom - constraint (Brehm, 1966; Steiner, 1970). Within the achievement domain, causes such as aptitude, effort, and health, are commonlyconsidered \ internal causes, whereas task difficulty, help from others, and luck are among the perceived environmental determinants of anoutcome. Within the I affiliative domain, causes such as physical beauty and "charm" are Internal, whereas the availability of the desired dating partner is an external determinant of acceptance or rejection. The placement of a cause within adimisionit not necessarily ariant over time or between people. For example, rathet than being anexternal cause,41uck'can be considered an attribute of a person CHeis lucky,"). .Given the focus of this paper, the relative placement ofa cause within a dimension is not important. Rather, what is important isthat locu4 for example, is - perceived as a basic property of causes. A shortcoming of this one-ditenSional taxonomy becameevid ent when it f was d sCoveredthat disparate respos,es are displayed given causes with an iderre-) callocusclassification. For example, in achievement-related contexts the perception that anindividual has failed because of a lack of effort gives rise to greater punishment than failure attributed to lowability (Weinif & Kukla, ].9,70). Fur failure perceived as due to lack ofaka,/ results in lower future expectancies of success than failurebelieveeto be caused by a lack of effort (Weiner, Nierehberg, & Goldstein, 1976). These disparities show that ability andeffortdiffer in one or more respects, although both are ,considered properties of the person.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    46 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us