Memory Consolidation

Memory Consolidation

OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Sun Jun 16 2013, NEWGEN CHAPTER 2 1 Memory Consolidation John T. Wixted and Denise J. Cai Abstract Memory consolidation is a multifaceted concept. At a minimum, it refers to both cellular consolidation and systems consolidation. Cellular consolidation takes place in the hours after learning, stabilizing the memory trace—a process that may involve structural changes in hippocampal neurons. Systems consolidation refers to a more protracted process by which memories become independent of the hippocampus as they are established in cortical neurons—a process that may involve neural replay. Both forms of consolidation may preferentially unfold whenever the hippocampus is not encoding new information, although some theories hold that consolidation occurs exclusively during sleep. In recent years, the notion of reconsolidation has been added to the mix. According to this idea, previously consolidated memories, when later retrieved, undergo consolidation all over again. With new findings coming to light seemingly every day, the concept of consolidation will likely evolve in interesting and unpredictable ways in the years to come. Key Words: cellular consolidation, systems consolidation, reconsolidation, sleep and consolidation Th e idea that memories require time to consoli- position of the interfering list within the retention date has a long history, but the understanding of interval mattered such that interference occurring what consolidation means has evolved over time. soon after learning had a more disruptive eff ect than In 1900, the German experimental psychologists interference occurring later in the retention inter- Georg Müller and Alfons Pilzecker published a val. Th is led them to propose that memories require monograph in which a new theory of memory and time to consolidate and that retroactive interference forgetting was proposed, one that included—for is a force that compromises the integrity of recently the fi rst time—a role for consolidation. Th eir basic formed (and not-yet-consolidated) memories. In method involved asking subjects to study a list of this chapter, we review the major theories of consol- paired-associate nonsense syllables and then testing idation—beginning with the still-relevant account their memory using cued recall after a delay of several proposed by Müller and Pilzecker (1900)—and we minutes. Typically, some of the list items were for- consider a variety of recent developments in what gotten, and to investigate why that occurred, Müller has become a rapidly evolving fi eld. and Pilzecker (1900) presented subjects with a sec- ond, interfering list of items to study before mem- Th e Early View: Consolidation and ory for the target list was tested. Th ey found that Resistance to Interference this interpolated list reduced memory for the target According to Müller and Pilzecker’s (1900) list compared with a control group that was not view, consolidation consists of “trace hardening” exposed to any intervening activity. Critically, the (cf. Wickelgren, 1974) in the sense that some 436 221_Ochsner-V1_Ch21.indd1_Ochsner-V1_Ch21.indd 443636 66/19/2013/19/2013 11:32:49:32:49 AAMM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Sun Jun 16 2013, NEWGEN physiological process perseverates and eventually become stronger, so it is important to keep in mind renders the memory trace less vulnerable to inter- which meaning of a “stronger memory trace” applies ference caused by new learning. Th e kind of inter- in any discussion of consolidation. One way that a ference that a consolidated trace theoretically resists trace might become stronger is that it comes to more diff ers from the kind of interference that most accurately refl ect past experience than it did when it experimental psychologists have in mind when was fi rst formed, much like a snapshot taken from a they study forgetting. In the fi eld of experimental Polaroid camera comes into sharper focus over time. psychology, new learning has long been thought to A trace that consolidated in this manner would yield generate interference by creating competing asso- an ever-clearer memory of the encoding event in ciations linked to a retrieval cue, not by aff ecting response to the same retrieval cue. Another way that the integrity of a fragile memory trace (e.g., Keppel, a trace might become stronger is that it becomes 1968; Underwood, 1957; Watkins & Watkins, ever more likely to spring to mind in response to 1975). Traditionally, this kind of interference has a retrieval cue (even more likely than it was when been investigated using an A-B, A-C paired-associ- the memory was fi rst formed). A memory trace that ates paradigm in which the same cue words (the A consolidated in either of these two ways would sup- items) are paired with diff erent to-be-remembered port a higher level of performance than it did at the target words across two lists (the B and C items, end of training, as if additional learning occurred respectively). In a standard retroactive interference despite the absence of additional training. paradigm, for example, the memory test consists of Still another way that a trace can become stron- presenting the A items and asking participants to ger is that it becomes hardened against the destruc- recall the B items. Having learned the A-C associa- tive forces of interference. A trace that hardens over tions after learning the A-B associations, the abil- time (i.e., a trace that consolidates in that sense) ity of participants to recall the B items is typically may simultaneously become degraded over time impaired, and this impairment is usually assumed due to the interfering force of new learning or to to refl ect retrieval competition from the C items. some other force of decay. As an analogy, a clay Th e powerful eff ect of this kind of “cue overload” replica of the Statue of Liberty will be at its fi nest interference on retention has been well established when it has just been completed and the clay is still by decades of psychological research, but it is almost wet, but it will also be at its most vulnerable. With certainly not the only kind of interference that the passage of time, however, the statue dries and causes forgetting. becomes more resistant to damage even though it Th e kind of interference envisioned by Müller may now be a less accurate replica than it once was and Pilzecker (1900) does not involve overloading (because of the damage that occurred before the a retrieval cue but instead involves directly com- clay dried). Müller and Pilzecker’s (1900) original promising the integrity of a partially consolidated view of consolidation, which was later elaborated by memory trace. In what even today seems like a Wickelgren (1974), was analogous to this. Th at is, radical notion to many experimental psycholo- the consolidation process was not thought to render gists, Müller and Pilzecker (1900) assumed that the the trace more representative of past experience or interference was nonspecifi c in the sense that the to render it more likely to come to mind than it was interfering material did not have to be similar to at the time of formation; instead, consolidation was the originally memorized material for interference assumed to render the trace (or its association with to occur. Instead, mental exertion of any kind was a retrieval cue) more resistant to interference even thought to be the interfering force (Lechner et al., while the integrity of the trace was gradually being 1999). “Mental exertion” is fairly vague concept, and compromised by interference. Wixted (2004a) suggested that the kind of inter- Th ese considerations suggest a relationship vening mental exertion that Müller and Pilzecker between Müller and Pilzecker’s (1900) view of con- (1900) probably had in mind consists specifi cally solidation and the time course of forgetting. More of new learning. Th e basic idea is that new learning, specifi cally, the fact that a memory trace hardens per se, serves as an interfering force that degrades in such a way as to become increasingly resistant recently formed and still fragile memory traces. to interference even as the trace fades may help to Loosely speaking, it can be said that Müller and explain the general shape of the forgetting func- Pilzecker (1900) believed that the memory trace tion (Wixted, 2004b). Since the seminal work becomes strengthened by the process of consolidation. of Ebbinghaus (1885), a consistent body of evi- However, there is more than one way that a trace can dence has indicated that the proportional rate of wixted, cai 437 221_Ochsner-V1_Ch21.indd1_Ochsner-V1_Ch21.indd 443737 66/19/2013/19/2013 11:32:49:32:49 AAMM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Sun Jun 16 2013, NEWGEN forgetting is rapid at fi rst and then slows to a point long ago by Ribot (1881/1882), but he had no at which almost no further forgetting occurs. Th is way of knowing what brain structures were cen- general property is captured by the power law of trally involved in this phenomenon. Th e experience forgetting (Anderson & Schooler, 1991; Wixted & of H.M. made it clear that the relevant structures Carpenter, 2007; Wixted & Ebbesen, 1991), and it reside in the MTL, and the phenomenon of tem- is enshrined in Jost’s law of forgetting, which states porally graded retrograde amnesia suggests an that if two memory traces have equal strength but extended but time-limited role for the MTL in the diff erent ages, the older trace will decay at a slower encoding and retrieval of new memories. Th at is, rate than the younger one from that moment on the MTL is needed to encode new memories, and it (Jost, 1897). One possibility is that the continuous is needed for a time after they are encoded, but it is reduction in the rate of forgetting as a trace ages is a not needed indefi nitely.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    20 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us