Attention and Implicit Memory Tests: the Effects Ofvarying Attentionalload on Conceptual Priming

Attention and Implicit Memory Tests: the Effects Ofvarying Attentionalload on Conceptual Priming

Memory & Cognition 1997,25 (1),11-17 Attention and implicit memory tests: The effects ofvarying attentionalload on conceptual priming NEIL W. MULLIGAN Illirwis State University, Normal, Illinois The role of attention during encoding is important to many current accounts of the implicit/explicit memory distinction. Some accounts suggest that implicit memory tests reflect automatic (non-attention­ demanding) encoding processes, whereas other accounts (such as the transfer-appropriate-processing view) suggest that performance on conceptual implicit tests requires attention during encoding. The present study manipulates attention at encoding over several levels (by varying short-term memory load) and examines the effects on the category-exemplar production task (a conceptual implicit mem­ ory test) and its explicit counterpart, category-cued recall. Dividing attention decreased performance on both tests, but in different ways. Mild divisions of attention reduced recall but not conceptual prim­ ing. Strong divisions of attention reduced performance on both tests and, in addition, eliminated con­ ceptual priming entirely. These findings resolve apparently conflicting results in the literature and help to clarify the relationship between attention and performance on implicit memory tests. Over the past 15 years, a great deal ofresearch in cog­ typically have poorer performance than younger adults nitive psychology and neuropsychology has focused on on explicit tests but not on implicit tests (see Light, 1991, the distinction between explicit and implicit memory (e.g., for a review). Experimental manipulations, such as the Graf & Masson, 1993; Roediger & McDermott, 1993; levels-of-processing and the read/generate manipulation, Schacter, 1987). Explicit memory refers to intentional or also produce dissociations on explicit and implicit mem­ conscious recollection of prior experiences and is mea­ ory tests (see Roediger & McDermott, 1993, for a recent sured by directing subjects to think back about some prior review). event and report on it, as is done with free-recall, cued­ One potentially important difference between implicit recall, and recognition-memory tests. Implicit memory, and explicit tests centers on the controlled/automatic pro­ in contrast, refers to changes in behavior that are produced cessing distinction. Controlled (or attention-demanding) by prior experience and that affect performance on mem­ encoding processes appear to be necessary for later ex­ ory tests that do not require any intentional or conscious plicit memory (e.g., Fisk & Schneider, 1984), but auto­ recollection, such as the identification of perceptually matic (or non-attention-demanding) encoding processes degraded stimuli or the completion of word fragments. may suffice for later implicit memory (e.g., Eich, 1984). Memory for prior events is inferred from increased ease Several studies indicate that very poorly attended stim­ in identifying, generating, or otherwise processing pre­ uli, which may show no retention on explicit memory viously experienced information. The enhanced perfor­ tests, may nevertheless lead to significant levels ofprim­ mance on implicit memory tests is called priming (Roe­ ing on implicit memory tests (Bornstein, Leone, & Gai­ diger & McDermott, 1993). ley, 1987; Eich, 1984; Hawley & Johnston, 1991; Jelicic, The principles that govern performance on explicit Bonke, Wolters, & Phaf, 1992; Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, and implicit memory tests differ in many ways. Particu­ 1980; MacKay, 1973; Mandler, Nakamura, & Van Zandt, larly striking examples come from neuropsychology and 1987; Merikle & Reingold, 1991; Seamon, Marsh, & cognitive gerontology. Compared with healthy control sub­ Brody, 1984; cf. Woods, Stadler & Cowan, 1995). Other jects, amnesics are profoundly impaired on explicit mem­ studies have demonstrated that division ofattention dur­ ory tests but often not on implicit memory tests (see Shi­ ing encoding impairs explicit test performance but may mamura, 1986, 1993, for reviews). Likewise, older adults leave the amount of priming completely unaffected (Gabrieli et aI., 1995; Isingrini, Vazou, & Leroy, 1995; Jacoby, Woloshyn, & Kelley, 1989; Mulligan & Hartman, This research was supported, in part, by a university research grant 1996; Parkin, Reid, & Russo, 1990; Parkin & Russo, 1990; from Illinois State University, These results were presented at the an­ nual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Los Angeles, in November Russo & Parkin, 1993; Smith & Oscar-Berman, 1990). 1995. I would like to thank Philip Merikle, Michael Masson, Kathleen Such findings have prompted some researchers to suggest McDermott, Tim Curran, Marilyn Hartman, Elliot Hirshman, and Gor­ that performance on implicit memory tests largely re­ don Redding for very useful comments and suggestions. The assistance flects automatic encoding processes, whereas performance of Mark Stilling and Kristin Van Schelven is gratefully acknowledged. Address correspondence to N. Mulligan, Department of Psychology, on explicit tests is crucially dependent on encoding pro­ Illinois State University, Campus Box 4620, Normal IL 61790-4620 cesses that require attentional resources (Besson, Fisch­ (e-mail: [email protected]). ler, Boaz, & Raney, 1992; Graf & Mandler, 1984; Isin- 11 Copyright 1997 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 12 MULLIGAN grini et aI., 1995; Jacoby,Toth, & Yonelinas, 1993; Jacoby fected (see Gabrieli et aI., 1995, and Mulligan & Hart­ et aI., 1989; Jelicic et aI., 1992; Parkin et aI., 1990; Par­ man, 1996, for reviews). This implies that attentional re­ kin & Russo, 1990).1 sources (beyond those necessary for identification) play A somewhat more differentiated view on the relation­ little or no role in perceptual implicit memory. ship between attention and implicit tests of memory is With regard to conceptual implicit tests, the picture is offered by the transfer-appropriate processing (TAP) not as clear. Some studies indicate that conceptual prim­ framework, a prominent account ofimplicit and explicit ing requires little or no attention during encoding (lsingrini memory (e.g., Blaxton, 1989, 1992; Roediger & McDer­ et aI., 1995; Koriat & Feuerstein, 1976), whereas other mott, 1993; Roediger, Weldon, & Challis, 1989).The TAP studies indicate that the conceptual priming is crucially view proposes that performance on a memory test bene­ dependent on attention (Gabrieli et aI., 1995; Mulligan & fits to the extent that the cognitive processes carried out Hartman, 1996). during encoding are reengaged at test (Morris, Bransford, To begin resolving this inconsistency, I compare two & Franks, 1977). Furthermore, the TAP framework dis­ very similar studies that produced conflicting results. tinguishes between two broad classes of cognitive pro­ Both Isingrini et al. (1995) and Mulligan and Hartman cessing: conceptual processing-the analysis ofmeaning (1996) used the category-exemplar production task to or semantic features-and perceptual processing-the measure conceptual priming. In this task, subjects are analysis ofperceptual or surface-level features (Blaxton, first presented with a series ofexemplars from different 1989; Roediger & McDermott, 1993; Roediger et aI., taxonomic categories. Later, subjects are presented with 1989). The most commonly used explicit tests rely on con­ the names of categories and asked to rapidly produce ex­ ceptual processing (e.g., free and cued recall, recognition), amples. Although the subjects are not informed ofthis, whereas many commonly used implicit tests rely heavily some ofthe categories correspond to studied exemplars on perceptual processing (e.g., perceptual identification, and some do not. Conceptual priming is computed as the word-fragment completion). Consequently, many im­ difference between the proportion of the studied exem­ plicit and explicit tests benefit from different types ofen­ plars produced and an appropriate baseline measure. In coding procedures (Craik, Moscovitch, & McDowd, 1994; addition to the category-exemplar production test, both Roediger et aI., 1989; Weldon, Roediger, Beitel, & John­ Isingrini et al. and Mulligan and Hartman used category­ ston, 1995). cued recall, an explicit memory test that uses the same re­ According to the TAP framework, it is critical to note trieval cues. that not all implicit tests are perceptual. Some, such as In both experiments, the study words were presented free association and category-exemplar production, en­ visually and attention was divided with a concurrent au­ courage the reengagement ofconceptual processes (Blax­ ditory detection task. Isingrini et al. (1995) found that ton, 1992; Roediger & McDermott, 1993; Roediger et aI., dividing attention significantly reduced performance on 1989). Performance on conceptual implicit memory tests the category-cued recall task, but not on the category­ bears interesting similarities with and differences from exemplar production task.? In contrast, Mulligan and performance on perceptual implicit memory tests. For Hartman (1996) found robust effects ofdivided attention instance, both amnesics and older adults have shown nor­ on both tasks. Furthermore, dividing attention elimi­ mal levels of priming on both types ofimplicit tests (Blax­ nated conceptual priming but not recall. Isingrini et al.'s ton, 1992; Graf, Shimamura, & Squire, 1985; Light & results support the idea that conceptual priming does not Albertson, 1989). In contrast, several other variables (e.g., require attentional encoding and, more generally, that

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    7 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us