Adaptive Motivation and Emotion in Education

Adaptive Motivation and Emotion in Education

BBSXXX10.1177/2372732216644450Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain SciencesLinnenbrink-Garcia et al. 644450research-article2016 Climate, Motivation, and Emotion Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences Adaptive Motivation and Emotion in 2016, Vol. 3(2) 228 –236 © The Author(s) 2016 DOI: 10.1177/2372732216644450 Education: Research and Principles for bbs.sagepub.com Instructional Design Lisa Linnenbrink-Garcia1, Erika A. Patall2, and Reinhard Pekrun3 Abstract Students frequently experience various types of motivation and emotion that contribute to their engagement and learning. However, translating research on motivation and emotion into educational practice and policy has so far been limited. To facilitate greater synergy among research, practice, and policy, this overview addresses educationally relevant motivation and emotion. This summary discusses different forms of motivation or emotion, their relevant theoretical basis, evidence on how they relate to academic engagement and learning, and potential classroom supports for adaptive motivation and emotion. The article concludes with five instructional design principles that can guide educators and policymakers in promoting adaptive student motivation and emotion: (a) support students’ feelings of competence, (b) enhance autonomy, (c) use personally relevant and active tasks, (d) emphasize learning and de-emphasize social comparison, and (e) encourage feelings of belonging. Keywords motivation, emotion, education, academic engagement, academic achievement, instructional practice Tweet Linnenbrink-Garcia, 2012). Yet, both are also widely misun- derstood. Thus, we begin by providing working definitions Instructional design (that emphasizes competence, auton- for both constructs. Next, we overview several key forms of omy, relevance, learning, and belonging) supports adaptive motivation and emotion, review evidence of their links with motivation and emotion. academic engagement and learning, and discuss potential classroom supports. We close with five instructional design Key Points principles to guide both educators and policymakers as to how classrooms may most profitably promote student moti- •• Academic motivation includes forms related to com- vation and emotion, and note several cautions and areas for petence (can I do this?) as well as values and goals future research relevant for the translation of theory and (why do I want to do this?). research to educational policy and practice. •• Academic emotions involve coordinated psychologi- cal processes, including affective, cognitive, physio- logical, motivational, and expressive components, What Are Motivation and Emotion? that affect learning. And Why the Definitions Matter •• Adaptive motivation and emotion support students’ The words motivation and emotion are part of the common engagement, learning, and persistence in educational vernacular; however, researchers’ use of these terms often contexts. differs from everyday speech. Motivation refers to the pro- •• To enhance students’ most adaptive motivation and emo- cesses of both initiating and sustaining behavior (Schunk, tion, educational contexts should support feeling compe- Meece, & Pintrich, 2014). Motivational researchers consider tent, enhance autonomy, use personally relevant and active tasks, emphasize learning and de-emphasize social 1 comparison, and encourage feelings of belonging. Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA 2The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA 3University of Munich, Munich, Germany Introduction Corresponding Author: Lisa Linnenbrink-Garcia, Department of Counseling, Educational Motivation and emotion are everywhere in classroom set- Psychology & Special Education, Michigan State University, 620 Farm Lane, tings. Both are critical for supporting students’ engagement Room 513E Erickson Hall, East Lansing, MI 48824, USA. and learning (Linnenbrink-Garcia & Patall, 2016; Pekrun & Email: [email protected] Linnenbrink-Garcia et al. 229 how students’ self-related beliefs, cognitions, goals, and achievement expectancies reflect individuals’ predictions experiences shape engagement and learning. Motivation about how well they will do on upcoming tasks (expec- includes various forms related to competence (can I do this?) tancy–value theory; Eccles et al., 1983). A related con- as well as values and goals (why do I want to do this?). struct is self-concept, referring to cognitive beliefs about Motivation is also contextual. A student’s motivation may various aspects of the self, including cognitive evaluations vary over time and as a function of the educational context; of ability (Bong & Skaalvik, 2003). These competence- teacher, parent, or peer interactions; the subject area; or even related motivation constructs overlap considerably. the specific task. As such, altering educational contexts Regardless of terminology, competence-related beliefs offers ample opportunity to support more adaptive forms of should support student learning and achievement, as believ- motivation. ing one has control over one’s own outcomes is necessary for Emotion involves systems of coordinated psychological engaging and putting forth effort in a given task. Empirical processes, including affective, cognitive, physiological, research supports this claim. Competence beliefs shape stu- motivational, and expressive components (Shuman & dents’ choices, effort and persistence, self-regulatory strate- Scherer, 2014). For example, anxiety involves uneasy, ner- gies, emotions, and achievement (Bandura, 1997; Pekrun & vous feelings (affective), worries (cognitive), physiological Perry, 2014; Usher, 2016). For example, students with higher arousal, avoidance motivation, and anxious facial expres- academic self-efficacy are more likely to use metacognitive sion. Some authors define emotions as intense, short-lived strategies, persist despite failure, experience fewer negative episodes (and distinguish them from longer lasting, lower emotions and more positive emotions, select challenging intensity affective states, that is, moods). Valence and activa- courses, and attain higher achievement. tion further describe emotions. Valence differentiates posi- The educational environment can support competence tive states, such as enjoyment and happiness, from negative beliefs (e.g., Usher & Pajares, 2008). For example, mastery states, such as anger or boredom. Physiologically activating experiences, vicarious experiences, verbal persuasion, and states, such as excitement, can be distinguished from deacti- affective or physiological experiences all influence self- vating states, such as relaxation. efficacy. In experiments, creating tasks that are well-aligned with students’ existing skills (e.g., Harter, 1978), having stu- Education-Related Motivation and dents observe similar models successfully demonstrate a task (e.g., Schunk & Hanson, 1985), encouraging students Emotion and Relevant Theory to set specific, challenging, but attainable goals (e.g., Locke Extensive research addresses motivation in education & Latham, 2002), and providing positive, encouraging, and (Linnenbrink-Garcia & Patall, 2016). And, there is a growing informational feedback (e.g., Vallerand & Reid, 1988) all body of theoretical and empirical work on how and why are strategies that support students’ competence beliefs. emotions emerge in the classroom and their relation to Although school-based interventions are rare, those that uti- engagement and learning (Pekrun & Linnenbrink-Garcia, lize some combination of these strategies are generally suc- 2012). The next section describes useful theory and reviews cessful in supporting perceived competence and subsequent evidence related to five educationally relevant types of moti- achievement (e.g., Kitsantas, Robert, & Doster, 2004; Siegle vation: competence beliefs, causal attributions and implicit & McCoach, 2007). For example, in one cluster-randomized theories of intelligence, interests and values, intrinsic and intervention, the fifth-grade students of teachers trained on extrinsic motivation, and achievement goals. We close by goal setting, providing positive feedback, and using model- describing research on emotion. (Table 1 overviews these ing had higher self-efficacy, compared with the students of constructs.) teachers who did not receive this training (Siegle & McCoach, 2007). Competence Beliefs and Expectancies Causal Attributions and Implicit Theories of Competence beliefs and expectancies have powerful con- sequences for students’ academic outcomes. Self-efficacy, Intelligence one of the most researched, refers to individuals’ beliefs People are motivated to understand outcomes they experi- about their capacity to perform, that is, to execute behav- ence, especially unexpected or negative ones (attribution iors at particular levels (social cognitive theory, Bandura, theory of achievement motivation; Weiner, 1985). In their 1997). Applied to education, academic self-efficacy search to explain an outcome, students may arrive at many includes students’ expectations and beliefs about their abil- possible causal attributions (i.e., ability, effort, luck, or task ity to learn materials, develop skills, or master tasks. Self- difficulty). Three dimensions organize these attributions and efficacy is future-oriented, reflecting students’ beliefs specify why they matter: locus, whether the cause is internal about what they will be able to do, and is specific to a task to the individual (e.g., ability, effort) or external (e.g.,

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    9 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us