Sleep Problems Across Development: a Pathway to Adolescent Risk Taking Through Working Memory

Sleep Problems Across Development: a Pathway to Adolescent Risk Taking Through Working Memory

J Youth Adolescence DOI 10.1007/s10964-014-0179-7 EMPIRICAL RESEARCH Sleep Problems Across Development: A Pathway to Adolescent Risk Taking Through Working Memory April Gile Thomas • Kathryn C. Monahan • Angela F. Lukowski • Elizabeth Cauffman Received: 22 April 2014 / Accepted: 23 August 2014 Ó Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014 Abstract Problematic sleep can be detrimental to the middle childhood to adolescence. Although sleep problems development of important cognitive functions, such as in infancy, early childhood, and middle childhood were not working memory, and may have the potential for negative directly related to adolescent working memory, sleep behavioral consequences, such as risk-taking. In this way, problems during adolescence were associated with poorer sleep problems may be particularly harmful for youth— adolescent working memory. In turn, these deficits in whose cognitive abilities are still developing and who are working memory were related to greater risk taking in late more susceptible to risky behavior. Using data from a large, adolescence. In summary, the present results suggest that national, longitudinal study, continuity and change in sleep sleep problems in earlier periods are indicative of risk for problems were examined from 2 to 15 years of age and sleep problems later in development, but that sleep problems associated with deficits in working memory at age 15 and in adolescence contribute uniquely to deficits in working risk taking behaviors at age 18. Participants (N = 1,364 memory that, in turn, lead to risky behavior during late children; 48.3 % female) were assessed for sleep problems adolescence. (parent-report), working memory (behavioral task), and risk taking behavior (youth self-report). The sample was pre- Keywords Sleep problems Á Sleep disturbances Á dominantly White (80.4 %); additional races represented in Working memory Á Risk taking Á Adolescence the sample included Black/African American (12.9 %), Asian/Pacific Islander (1.6 %), American Indian/Eskimo/ Aleut (.4 %), and Other (4.7 %). The findings suggest that Introduction sleep problems are likely to cascade across development, with sleep problems demonstrating continuity from infancy It has been estimated that 25–40 % of children suffer from to early childhood, early childhood to middle childhood, and some type of sleep problem (Owens 2005). Moreover, the occurrence of sleep problems in childhood does not appear to be transitory: developmental continuity in sleep prob- lems has been documented from infancy to childhood, such & A. G. Thomas ( ) Á A. F. Lukowski Á E. Cauffman that 41 % of infants identified as having sleep problems at Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California, Irvine, 4201 Social and Behavioral Sciences 8 months experienced continuing difficulties at 3 years Gateway, Irvine, CA 92697-7085, USA (Zuckerman et al. 1987). Continuity has also been observed e-mail: [email protected] across childhood, such that children who experienced sleep A. F. Lukowski problems before age 2 years were more likely to experi- e-mail: [email protected] ence continuing sleep issues between 4 and 12 years of age E. Cauffman (Stein et al. 2001). Some evidence suggests that this con- e-mail: [email protected] tinuity also remains into adolescence for more general sleep problems, but not for more specific sleep problems, K. C. Monahan University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA such as nightmares, bedwetting, or talking/walking in sleep e-mail: [email protected] (Friedman et al. 2009). 123 J Youth Adolescence Adolescence is a time marked by dramatic changes in impact functional outcomes, such as working memory and sleep patterns (Colrain and Baker 2011). During puberty, risk taking behavior. In the present research, we use a adolescents experience significant reductions in slow wave longitudinal dataset to examine (a) continuity and change sleep (Carskadon 2002), as well as changes in their circa- in sleep problems from 2 to 15 years of age, and (b) the dian rhythm known as a ‘‘delayed phase preference,’’ association between sleep problems at various stages of which involves a desire to stay up later in the evening and development, adolescent working memory, and risky sleep in later in the morning (Hagenauer et al. 2009). behavior. Essentially, adolescents’ sleep patterns reflect a change from being an ‘‘early bird’’ to a ‘‘night owl.’’ School start Sleep and Working Memory times have largely been incompatible with adolescents’ natural preference for going to bed late and sleeping in, Working memory, a component of executive functioning, leading many adolescents to experience an increase in is a multi-faceted ability that involves selective attention, daytime sleepiness (Carskadon et al. 1998; Carskadon retrieval of past experiences held in long-term memory, 2011) and report a greater occurrence of insufficient sleep and manipulation of information held in the short-term (Colrain and Baker 2011). Although poor sleep quality or memory (Kane and Engle 2002). Performance on tasks of quantity may be detrimental to cognitive functioning at any working memory has been found to improve linearly from age, it may be especially harmful during adolescence due age 4 to 15 years (Gathercole et al. 2004). Working to the significant biological and psychosocial changes that memory skills continue to become refined throughout occur at this stage (Dahl and Lewin 2002). adolescence, as demonstrated by changes in brain activity One aspect of cognitive functioning that seems to be associated with working memory from childhood through particularly influenced by poor sleep is working memory, a adolescence (see review by Best et al. 2009). With age, component of executive functioning. In fact, poor sleep prefrontal brain regions appear to become more specialized quality has been associated with decreased working for working memory. Importantly, brain regions recruited memory performance in children and adolescents (Steenari for working memory (such as the prefrontal cortex) seem to et al. 2003). Working memory is the capacity for storing be negatively affected by sleep deprivation (Muzur et al. and manipulating information in one’s mind (Baddeley 2002). 2010) and research suggests that it may play a role in Although working memory continues to develop across adolescent risk taking through its influence on impulse childhood and into adolescence, it appears that both vari- control (Romer et al. 2011). Working memory serves as a ability in normative sleep patterns and the incidence of cognitive filter, allowing a person to hold information in sleep problems has been associated with working memory one’s mind in order to consider potential consequences of across different periods of development from infancy to one’s actions. In this way, deficits in working memory may adolescence. For example, in a study of infants and tod- contribute to risky decision-making. dlers, greater proportions of time spent sleeping during the Across development, risky behavior tends to follow an night relative to the day at 12 months was associated with inverted U-shaped developmental trajectory that includes better performance on tasks of impulse control and con- normative increases in risk taking during adolescence, flict-executive functioning (a factor score comprised of followed by declines during the transition to adulthood working memory, set shifting, and inhibitory control) at (Steinberg 2008). This peak in risk taking that occurs 26 months; higher percentages of time spent sleeping during middle to late adolescence is thought to be the result during the night relative to the day at 18 months were of an imbalance between low impulse control and high associated with concurrent working memory scores at reward-seeking tendencies (Steinberg 2010). As youth 18 months as well as with impulse control at 26 months transition to adolescence, their sleep patterns also undergo (Bernier et al. 2010). important changes that may exacerbate this cognitive– Concurrent sleep problems have been associated with affective imbalance and contribute to risky behavior (Tel- working memory in school-aged children as well. Perfor- zer et al. 2013). mance deficits were observed in relationship to variability Thus, adolescence appears to be a time of greater like- in normative sleep habits in 6- to 13-year-old children on lihood of experiencing sleep problems. Yet, for many working memory tasks (Steenari et al. 2003). In particular, adolescents, sleep problems do not begin here. Surpris- the findings indicated that reduced sleep efficiency and ingly, although considerable research has examined how duration, as well as increased sleep latency (operational- poor sleep at a given time is associated with concurrent or ized as time to transition from wakefulness to sleep), were subsequent outcomes, rarely do these studies take into differentially associated with working memory deficits. account the pattern of sleep problems across time and how Likewise, in a study of 174 high school students, sleep sleep problems at various stages of development may duration was found to be positively associated with 123 J Youth Adolescence working memory. Specifically, those students who slept problems. The present study seeks to replicate this finding, less than 8 h per night performed worse on two types of specifically with working memory, and extend it to complex working memory tasks than those who slept examine the influence on risky behavior in late between 8 and 9 h per night (Gradisar et al. 2008).

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    18 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us