
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (1), 185-191 Protected values: No omission bias and no framing effects CARMEN TANNER University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland and DOUGLAS L. MEDIN Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois Previous studies have suggestedthat people holding protectedvalues (PVs) show a bias against harm- ful acts, as opposed to harmful omissions (omission bias). In the present study, we (1) investigatedthe relationship between PVs and acts versus omissions in risky choices, using a paradigm in which act and omission biases were presentedin a symmetricalmanner, and (2) examined whether people holding PVs respond differently to framing manipulations. Participants were given environmental scenarios and were asked to make choices between actions and omissions. Both the framing of the outcomes (posi- tive vs. negative) and the outcome certainty (risky vs. certain) were manipulated. In contrast to previ- ous studies, PVs were linked to preferences for acts, rather than for omissions. PVs were more likely to be associated with moral obligations to act than with moral prohibitions against action. Strikingly, peo- ple with strong PVs were immune to framing; participants with few PVs showed robust framing effects. People often express moral opinions concerning envi- omission bias) than did people without a PV. Omission ronmental problems, such as global warming, pollution, bias with people holding PVs has been shown in numer- or endangered species. Research on judgment and deci- ous studies and in a variety of environmental and social sion making conducted by Ritov, Baron, and associates contexts (see Markman & Medin, 2002, for a review). (e.g., Baron & Ritov,1994; Baron & Spranca, 1997; Ritov Overall, the dominant interpretation is that PVs are deon- & Baron, 1990, 1999) has shown that people sometimes tologicalprohibitionsagainstaction that shape a tendency have protected values (PVs), which are absolute values toward omissions. that people protect from tradeoffs (Fiske & Tetlock, 1997). In sharp contrast, there is related research on environ- Furthermore, PVs are believed to arise from deontological mental behaviorthat suggeststhat PVs are not only about principles, rather than from consequentialist assessments prohibitions but can also be associated with commitment of gains and losses. Importantly, PVs have been linked to to action. Studies in which the role of moral norms in en- omission bias: a tendency to favor the omission over the vironmental behaviors has been investigated have con- act when a choice must be made between a harmful act sistently found that people endorsing moral values are (e.g., killing somebody) and an otherwise equivalent more inclined to express such values through active be- harmful omission (e.g., letting somebody die). In such haviors, rather than through inaction (e.g., Black, Stern, cases, people often judge harmful acts to be morally worse & Elworth, 1985; Cialdini, Reno, & Kallgren, 1990; than harmful omissions. Consider this paraphrase of a typ- Hopper & Nielsen, 1991; Stern, Dietz, & Kalof, 1993). ical example from Ritov and Baron (1990): “An epidemic People with such value orientations are more willing to will cause 1,000 children to die, and a vaccine is available pursue environmental protection in the face of negative that would prevent these deaths but would cause 100 chil- economic or social outcomes (Axelrod, 1994). They may dren to die. Would you use the vaccine?” Ritov and Baron be driven (at least partially) by nonconsequentialist (de- found that often people rejected this tradeoff on the ontological) principles (their rationale for action is that it groundsthat they wouldnot want to cause the deathof any is “the right thing to do”). Rather than moral prohibitions children. A few participants said that they would not vac- against action, this research indicates a role for “oughts” cinate if the procedure caused the death of even a single in terms of moral obligation to act. child. People who held PVs for human life (independently This research on environmental values and moral obli- assessed) showed a greater reluctance to trade off (larger gations has received little attention from the judgment and decision making (JDM) literature. JDM research has suggested that PVs are closely linked to omission bias, This research was supported by Swiss National Science Foundation but this relation may have been overgeneralized.The em- Grant 8210-61241to the first author and NSF Grant SES-9910156to the second author. Address correspondence to C. Tanner, University of phasis on omission bias may also have contributed to ac- Zurich, Social and Business Psychology, Rämistrasse 62, 8001 Zurich, tion effects’ being largely ignored in decision making re- Switzerland (e-mail: [email protected]). search (see also Patt & Zeckhauser, 2000). Action effects 185 Copyright 2004 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 186 TANNER AND MEDIN are interesting because they conflict with omission bias which an outbreak of disease threatens to kill 600 peo- and other well-known decision rules, such as loss aver- ple and one option will save 200 people for sure and the sion and status quo bias, both of which favor inaction. other has a one-third chance of saving all 600 and a two- The present study focuses on the relationship between thirds chance of saving no one. In the alternative fram- PVs and action tendencies in decision making. ing, the same outcomes are described in terms of lives Comparing the literature on environmental behavior lost. People are more risk seeking in the negative frame and action effects based on moral obligation, on the one and more risk averse in the positive frame. Our modifi- hand, and the literature on omission bias and inactionef- cation was to make one option an active choice and the fects on the other, raises a central question: Which cir- other a default consequence. For example, participants cumstances produce one pattern of results versus the might be told that if they adopt Plan A, 200 people will other? One candidate factor is whether acts versus omis- be saved for sure, but if they do nothing, there is a one- sions are about negative or positive outcomes. Prohibi- third chance that all 600 will be saved and a two-thirds tions against action may be salient when the act can be chance that no one will be saved. Both the framing of the seen as causing harm. In contrast, obligation to act may environmental outcome focus (positive vs. negative out- be more salient when the act can be seen as promoting comes) and the certainty of the outcomes (risky vs. cer- something good. In line with this, Patt and Zeckhauser tain) associated with the options were manipulated. (2000) recently proposed that action bias is complemen- To our knowledge,no previous study has examined PVs tary to omission bias, the latter focusing on losses and and their relation to framing. The framing paradigm as- the former on gains. This suggests that act versus omis- sesses the relationship between PVs and acts/omissions in sion bias can be manipulated by situationalcues, such as a more symmetrical context (responses indicatingavoid- positiveversus negativeframing of options.In our study, ance of harmful acts and those indicatinga duty to act were we explore how PVs are related to framing. equally possible). In our study, people were given four We believe that omission bias and tradeoff reluctance hypothetical environmental scenarios. Previous work on may not be solely tied to moral prohibitions. In an un- omission bias has also used environmental scenarios, so published study that we (along with Chun-Hui Miao) there is no reason to expect that environmentalscenarios conductedusing the paradigm employed by Ritov,Baron, per se will affect performance. and associates (Baron & Ritov, 1994; Baron & Spranca, The experiment addressed three related questions. 1997; Ritov & Baron, 1990, 1999), we replicated the (1) How strongly are PVs related to acts versus omissions finding that PVs are associated with greater omission and moral prohibitionsversus moral obligations?(2) Is the bias, but the justifications provided by the participants prevalence of obligation versus prohibition and act versus suggested that responding typically was not guided by omission bias sensitive to negative/positive framing? moral prohibitions. The most common reasons given for (3) Do people holding PVs respond differently to framing rejecting tradeoffs involved either differentiating the manipulations? components of the tradeoffs or a moral obligationto act. On the basis of the environmental behavior literature, For example, in a scenario in which the participants were we would expect choices favoring acts over omissions. asked to trade 100 square miles of old growth forest that From the work of Ritov, Baron, and collaborators, one were part of a national park to save 1,000 square miles would predict that PVs should lead to omissions, rather of old growth forest from being logged, the participants than to acts. Furthermore, if a loss framing makes peo- often declined on the grounds that the park might have ple with PVs more likely to follow prohibitions against historical and sentimental value and/or cited an obliga- action, one would expect more omission bias under loss tion to act to protect national parks from logging inter- framing. ests. Justifications for declining tradeoffs on grounds of The third question is a result of the claim that PVs are moral prohibitions were rarely provided. deontologicalrules. Because PVs are based on deontolog- Furthermore, the typical paradigm in which omission ical rules, people are expected to attend more to the ac- bias has been found has something of an asymmetrical tivity per se than to the magnitude of the consequences. flavor. In most studies in which omission bias has been People holdingstrong PVs may follow rules that apply to examined, this tendency has been measured by asking the act. Given that they try to be consistent with their participants for a threshold amount of harm from action principles,people holding PVs should resist framing ma- (Ritov & Baron, 1999).
Details
-
File Typepdf
-
Upload Time-
-
Content LanguagesEnglish
-
Upload UserAnonymous/Not logged-in
-
File Pages7 Page
-
File Size-