Attenuation of Size Illusion Effect in Dual-Task Conditions

Attenuation of Size Illusion Effect in Dual-Task Conditions

Human Movement Science 67 (2019) 102497 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Human Movement Science journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/humov Attenuation of size illusion effect in dual-task conditions ⁎ T Hiromu Katsumata Department of Sports and Health Science, Daito-Bunka University, Tokyo, Japan ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: We over-estimate or under-estimate the size of an object depending its background structure Perceptual illusion (e.g., the Ebbinghaus illusion). Since deciding and preparing to execute a movement is based on Visuomotor control perception, motor performance deteriorates due to the faulty perception of information. Dual task paradigm Therefore, such cognitive process can be a source of a failure in motor performance, although we Ventral/dorsal stream feel in control of our performance through conscious cognitive activities. If a movement ex- ecution process can avoid distraction by the illusion-deceived conscious process, the effect of the visual illusion on visuomotor performance can be eliminated or attenuated. This study in- vestigated this hypothesis by examining two task performances developed for a target figure inducing the Ebbinghaus size illusion: showing visually perceived size of an object by index finger-thumb aperture (size-matching), and reaching out for the object and pretending to grasp it (pantomimed grasping). In these task performances, the size of the index finger-thumb aperture becomes larger or smaller than the actual size, in accordance with the illusion effect. This study examined whether the size illusion effect can be weakened or eliminated by the dual-task con- dition where actors’ attention to judge the object’s size and to produce the aperture size is in- terrupted. 16 participants performed the size-matching and pantomimed grasping tasks while simultaneously executing a choice reaction task (dual task) or without doing so (single task). Using an optical motion capture system, the size-illusion effect was analyzed in terms of the aperture size, which indicates the visually perceived object size. The illusion effect was atte- nuated in the dual task condition, compared to it in the single task condition. This suggests that the dual task condition modulated attention focus on the aperture movement and therefore the aperture movement was achieved with less distraction caused by illusory information. 1. Introduction In many daily activities, visual information is critical for high performance; however, our visual perceptual system does not always provide precise information. For example, we may overestimate or underestimate an object’s size depending on its back- ground structure. An example of this misperception is the Ebbinghaus illusion, in which the visually perceived size of a circle is judged to be larger or smaller than its actual size when the circle is surrounded by other smaller or larger circles (Fig. 1: inset A). Therefore, when participants were asked to match the shape of an aperture produced using the index finger and thumb with the visually estimated size of a center circle of the Ebbinghaus figure (size-matching task), the size of the index finger–thumb aperture was larger or smaller than the actual size, which was in accordance with the illusion effect (Haffenden & Goodale, 1998). In contrast, in some studies, when participants grasped a circular object, which induces the Ebbinghaus illusion, they produced the index finger and thumb aperture movement according to the size of the target object without being affected by the size illusion (Aglioti, DeSouza, ⁎ Address: Department of Sports and Health Science, Daito-Bunka University, 560 Iwadono, Higashi-Matsuyama City, Saitama Pref., Japan. E-mail address: [email protected]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2019.102497 Received 17 June 2018; Received in revised form 8 June 2019; Accepted 6 July 2019 Available online 18 July 2019 0167-9457/ © 2019 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. H. Katsumata Human Movement Science 67 (2019) 102497 Fig. 1. Time duration of the task paradigm. At the beginning of each trial, the gaze fixation figure of “+” was displayed in the center of a screen. After 1000 ms, a pre-number, either zero or another number, was displayed for 1000 ms: The number 0 indicated single task, and a non-zero number indicated dual-task. 1200–1600 ms after the pre-number disappeared, a target figure (smaller looking or larger looking) appeared for 1000 ms. The figure was replaced by the fixation figure to fade out the afterimage of the target figure and to prepare for the next trial. (A) Ebbinghaus figure; (B) Initial posture: size-matching was performed with this posture. The distance between the starting hand position and the screen’s center was approximately 38 cm. The eye-screen distance was approximately 48 cm from a chin-rest, which was used to stabilize the participant’s head position during the movement. (B) Pantomimed grasping was performed by reaching toward the target figure. & Goodale, 1995; Haffenden & Goodale, 1998; Haffenden, Schiff, & Goodale, 2001). These findings led to the proposal that the movement to grasp an object is not based on the same representation about the object’s size as a conscious perception about the size. However, some studies have also reported contradictory findings that the grasping movement is affected by size illusion (Franz, Gegenfurtner, Bülthoff, & Fahle, 2000; Pavani, Boscagli, Benvenuti, Rabuffetti, & Farnè, 1999). These conflicts in findings are an important issue because deciding and preparing to execute a movement are based on perception; therefore, motor performance deteriorates due to faulty perception of information. Thus, even though we feel in control of our performance through conscious cognitive activities, such cognitive processes can be sources of failures in motor performance. If a movement execution process can avoid distraction by the illusion-deceived conscious process, errors in visuomotor performance could be reduced. Therefore, the question is not only whether a movement is affected by size illusion but also whether the effect of the visual illusion can be eliminated or attenuated. Motivated by the above proposition, examining the illusion effect on cognition-based motor performance can be a useful method to determine how the cognitive process plays a role in executing motor performance based on perception. This study addresses the aforementioned question by examining whether size-illusion effect can be weakened or eliminated by dual-task conditions where actors’ conscious cognitive activities to execute a size-matching task in response to an illusory visual target are disturbed. To this end, I disturbed a participant’s conscious cognitive process for size-matching performance under a dual- task condition. A dual-task paradigm, in which a primary task is performed while simultaneously executing a secondary task, was employed to examine the automaticity of the primary task performance or magnitude of cognitive load to execute the task (Cameron, Franks, Enns, & Chua, 2007; Goh, Gordon, Sullivan, & Winstein, 2014; McNevin & Wulf, 2002). In the dual-task situation, cognitive load for achieving the goal is increased and the participants’ attention on producing the aperture movement is distracted. In this study, I used a choice reaction task as the secondary task because the task movement is produced based on a cognitive decision about stimulus identification and response selection with respect to the stimulus. By focusing the participants’ attention on cognitive information processing for the choice reaction task, attention on size matching is expected to be disturbed. Therefore, the judgment made about the object’s size and awareness required to produce the aperture size will be interrupted. If the size-illusion effect on the aperture is weakened or eliminated in the dual task, the dual-task condition can modulate attention on the aperture movement; distracting attention from illusory information is effective in preventing performance error caused by misperception. When performing size matching, the participants produce an aperture configuration based on the visually recognized size of the object. For this visuomotor process, attention must be paid to the object’s configuration and aperture movement that are being consciously performed. Therefore, this type of conscious process can be susceptible to the target’s illusory background. In contrast, when a participant is asked to reach for the center circle of the Ebbinghaus figure and pretend to grasp it (pantomimed grasping), it 2 H. Katsumata Human Movement Science 67 (2019) 102497 does not explicitly require us to show the perceived size of the object by the aperture configuration. However, participants reportedly scaled the size of an aperture produced using the thumb and index finger to grasp an object according to its size (Jeannerod, 1981, 1984). Based on this finding, the aperture size produced during pantomimed grasping will be scaled according to the perception of the object’s size. From this perspective, although pantomimed grasping is regarded as cognition-based motor performance, pre- shaping the aperture configuration to the object’s size, as the hand approaches the object, is an implicit process that is performed unconsciously. Therefore, examining the size-illusion effect on this movement in the dual-task condition will be interesting. This study investigates the influence of the dual-task condition in the magnitude of the size-illusion effect on size matching and pantomimed grasping. The results are discussed in

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    9 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us