
Author's personal copy Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. This article was originally published in the International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the author’s benefit and for the benefit of the author’s institution, for non-commercial research and educational use including without limitation use in instruction at your institution, sending it to specific colleagues who you know, and providing a copy to your institution’s administrator. All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including without limitation commercial reprints, selling or licensing copies or access, or posting on open internet sites, your personal or institution’s website or repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission may be sought for such use through Elsevier’s permissions site at: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial From Coman, A., 2015. Collective Memory, Psychology of. In: James D. Wright (editor-in-chief), International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, Vol 4. Oxford: Elsevier. pp. 188–193. ISBN: 9780080970868 Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. unless otherwise stated. All rights reserved. Elsevier Author's personal copy Collective Memory, Psychology of Alin Coman, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Abstract Collective memory has been the subject of productive interactions within the social sciences ever since its initial concep- tualization. Employing a social interactionist framework in which large-scale phenomena are empirically investigated as interactions between cognition and social dynamics, the article presents psychology’s contribution to understanding the formation and the dynamics of collective memories. Collective memory has had a long history within the social what is social about memory, he cautions against looking sciences. It served, in the period since Halbwachs’s landmark into the brain or in some ‘nook of (the) mind’ to understand conceptualization (1980/1925), as a meeting place for inter- memory, for memories “are recalled by me externally, and disciplinary conversations involving sociology, history, anthro- the groups of which I am part at any given time give me the pology, and psychology (Hirst and Manier, 2008). As any means to reconstruct them” (Halbwachs, 1980: 46). It is in constructive dialog, conceptualizing collective memory has light of this unbreakable link between the individual and its been ridden with theoretical disputes ever since the emergence social embeddedness that Halbwachs proposes the study of of the concept. Methodological individualism, an approach the social frameworks in which memory is manifested. aimed at deterministically deriving collective phenomena This ambiguity of locating collective memory in either from the psychological make up of individuals and their individual minds or in the social processes that govern their interactions, constituted the main issue of contention (Udéhn, creation has received extensive attention among social scientists. 2002). Although, it is beyond the scope of this article to In contrast with Halbwachs’s acceptance of the ambiguity, most provide a review of methodological individualism and its current approaches seek to resolve it by clearly differentiating nuances, the burgeoning literature on the psychology of between endeavors that commit to one location or the other. collective memory provides substantial evidence that bridging Young (1993), for instance, differentiates between ‘collected between micro and macroprocesses is a worthy endeavor memories,’ defined as the aggregation of individually (DiMaggio, 1997). In what follows, I will briefly present produced social memories, and ‘collective memory,’ seen as approaches within the social sciences that allow for collectivistic cultural activities or productions (see Olick, a psychological exploration of collective memory, I will then 1999; for a similar distinction). In final analysis, he argues, articulate the psychological assumptions of such an endeavor, “societies cannot remember in any other way than through and finally, I will review the burgeoning psychology literature their constituents’ memories” (1993: xi). In a similar attempt aimed at bridging between micro and macroprocesses. to provide clarity to the concept of collective memory, Jan Assmann distinguishes between communicative and cultural memories. The former are conceptualized as conversationally Collective Memory: A Case for Its Psychological constructed memories that have limited time horizons, the Foundations latter are seen as long lasting, publicly available cultural products (e.g., monuments, texts, images, rituals) designed to It is widely accepted that just as individuals have memories of emphasize meaningful events in the history of the group. It is their own personal pasts, so do collectivities have memories of only in those instances in which communicative memories get the events that are central to their identity. What is contentious, transformed into ‘objectivized culture’ that they become though, is whether to understand these collective memories, cultural memories (Assmann, 1995: 130). Individuals and one needs to take into account how individuals remember, or their interactions are, according to Assmann, critical only simply restrict oneself to the study of the social processes when considering transient communicative memories. The involved in their formation. Widely credited with the articu- focus of collective memory scholars should be, he claims, the lation of the concept, Maurice Halbwachs was mindful of the stable cultural memories and the struggles behind their fact that individual memory is critical to understanding enactment. collective memory. “It is individuals as group members who What emerges in the social sciences is, then, a dichotomy as remember,” he claims (Halbwachs, 1980: 46). The critical to whether understanding collective memories requires the notion that individuals remember as group members stands integration of memory processes that are specific to individual at the crux of his conceptualization of memory as inherently memory (Olick, 1999). On the one hand, there are efforts to social: from the very basic usage of a particular group’s carve out the ‘spaces’ where individuals are critical units language to reminisce to the social dimension that when discussing collective memory, in concepts such as accompanies the act of communicating a memory. Given the collected memories and communicative memories. On the belief that it is impossible to separate what is individual and other hand, approaches that follow an antiindividualistic 188 International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, Volume 4 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.32021-9 International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 188–193 Author's personal copy Collective Memory, Psychology of 189 stance (Assman, 1995; Connerton, 1989) advocate that the nonsense syllables and took precautions to make these study of collective memory be completely divorced from its syllables as distinct from natural language as possible. The anchoring within individual minds. To resolve this reasoning was that any associations that these syllables might antagonistic dichotomy, Olick suggests a framework, under trigger would interfere with one’s ability to understand how the heading of ‘social memory studies,’ that would allow memory works. disciplines to carry out their independent research programs Stripping these dimensions away, Sir Frederic Barlett (1932) in a way that respects their theoretical and methodological claimed, left nothing to be studied. Taking a perspective differences. Collected memory scholars would study the consistent with Halbwachs’s notion that memory is aggregation of memories, whereas collective memory scholars inherently social, Bartlett embarked on programmatically would carry out investigations of institutionalized objects. studying the social dimensions of memory. For Bartlett, A social interactionist approach stands in contrast to these memory was akin to a tennis stroke. Just as when serving dichotomous perspectives. It accepts that one cannot easily a ball, different contextual factors, such as wind speed, separate, as Halbwachs contended, the individual from the position of the sun, and the location of the opponent, will social. Social processes are constituted by individuals’ interac- affect one’s serve, so is remembering influenced by the tions, and so they are dependent on individual characteristics, context in which it is manifested. No two strokes, even by whereas individual behavior is dramatically affected by the the same individual, will be exactly the same because of the social contexts in which it is manifested. Integrating this inter- variations in the different contextual factors. It follows that dependence, a social interactionist position insists that one no two acts of remembering, even of the same memory by cannot understand how social artifacts and practices affect the same individual, will be exactly the same. In this memory without considering individual cognition (Hirst conception, memory is less of a reproductive replica of the and Echterhoff, 2012). This approach builds on recent past, and more of a reconstructive process affected by a host philosophical developments in
Details
-
File Typepdf
-
Upload Time-
-
Content LanguagesEnglish
-
Upload UserAnonymous/Not logged-in
-
File Pages7 Page
-
File Size-