The Barberplaid Illusion

The Barberplaid Illusion

vision Res., Vol. 36, No. 19, pp. 3061–3075,1996 Pergamon Publishedby ElsevierScienceLtd. PII: S0042-6989(96)00064-8 Printedin Great Britain @ 0042-6989/96$15.00+ 000 The Barberplaid Illusion: Plaid Motion is Biased by Elongated Apertures BRENT R.BEUmER,* JEFFREYB.MULLIGAN,*LELANDs.STONE*t Received28August1995;injinalform1 February1996 The perceived direction of motion of plaids windowed by elongated spatial Gaussians is biased toward the window’s long axis. The bias increases as the relative angle between the plaid motion and the long axis of the window increases, peaks at a relative angle of-45 deg, and then decreases. The bias increases as the window is made narrower (at fixed height) and decreases as the component spatial frequency increases (at fixed aperture size). We examine several models of human motion processing (cross-correlation, motion-energy, intersection-of-constraints, and vector-sum), and show that none of these standard models can predict our data. We conclude that spatial integration of motion signals plays a crucial role in plaid motion perception and that current models must be explicitly expanded to include such spatial interactions. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd. Plaids Motionmodels Direction discrimination Aperture Intersection-of-constraints rule INTRODUCTION pattern is uniquely determined. Thus, one might expect Human perception of motion depends not only on the that the type of window in which one-dimensional patterns are moved would determinehow this ambiguity physical motion of objects, but also on the conditions is resolved, and thus which of the many possible under which the motion is viewed.A simple yet dramatic directions of motion is actually perceived. On the other example of this is the barberpole illusion, in which the hand, the direction of motion of two-dimensional perceived direction of motion of an obliquely oriented patterns, such as plaids, is unambiguous (Adelson & drifting grating is vertical, when viewed through a Movshon, 1982), because the multiple constraints vertically oriented rectangular aperture; but the same providedby the componentsallow only a single solution motion appears horizontal,when the rectangularaperture [see Fig. l(b)]. The intersection-of-constraints rule is horizontal. The influence of the aperture on the specifieshow this unique resultantpattern velocity could perceived motion of one-dimensionalpatterns has been be computedfrom the componentvelocitiesand orienta- extensively examined (e.g. Wallach, 1935; Mulligan, tions (Fennema & Thompson, 1979; Adelson & Mov- 1991;Power & Mouldon,1992;Kooi, 1993;Mulligan& shon, 1982). Is the human visual system able to extract Beutter, 1994). The question that we examine in this this unique correct velocity independentof the aperture, paper is whether the perceived direction of motion of or does the window shape affect the processing of the two-dimensionalpatterns such as plaids is also affected motion signals? by the type of viewing window.We addressthis question In primates, evidence from anatomy, physiology,and by measuring the perceived direction of moving plaids psychophysicssuggests that motion processing of two- windowed by elongated spatial Gaussians. dimensional velocity appears to occur in a two-stage The direction of motion of a one-dimensionalpattern process. First, directionallyselective mechanismsdetect viewed through a restricted aperture is inherently the motionof localone-dimensionalfeaturesin the image ambiguous, as shown in Fig. l(a). The motion is (Hubel& Wiesel, 1968;Watsonet al., 1980;De Valoiset consistent with any velocity vector that falls on the al., 1982b)and then a second stage integratesthese one- constraint line, because only the velocity component in dimensionalsignals over a larger area of the visual field the direction perpendicular to the orientation of the to extract the two-dimensionalpattern velocity (Adelson & Movshon, 1982; Maunsell & Van Essen, 1983a,b; Albright, 1984; Mikami et al., 1986a,b;Movshonet al., *MS 262-2, Human and Systems Technologies Branch, Flight 1986, 1988; Newsome et al., 1986; Ungerleider & Managementand HumanFactors Division,NASAAmes Research Center, Moffett Field CA 94035-1000,U.S.A. Desimone, 1986; Rodman & Albright, 1989; Welch, ~To whom reprint requests shouldbe addressed. 1989; Stone, 1990). In primate cerebral cortex, the first 3061 3062 B. R. BEUTTERet al. psychophysicalevidence for noncomponentdriven one- stage velocity estimation [e.g. Derrington & Badcock (1992)]. Additionally, there are clearly other motion processing pathways leading to MT, directly from cortical areas V2 and V3 and from the” Superior Colliculus via the pulvinar, which may also play an important role in velocity estimation (Maunsell & Van Essen, 1983a; Ungerleider et al., 1984; De Yoe & Van Essen, 1985;Ungerleider& Desimone, 1986;Rodmanet al., 1989, 1990). Modelers have also explored the ways primate visual cortex might estimate velocity. These models fall into severalclasses. Bulthoffet al. (1989) have proposedthat velocity is estimated by finding the maximum of an image cross-correlation function. Motion-energy type FIGURE 1. (a) Two one-dimensionalgratingsoriented ~ 45 deg from models (Watson & Ahumada, 1983; van Santen & the vertical are shownin circular apertures.The velocitycomponentin Sperling, 1984, 1985;Adelson & Bergen, 1985;Watson the direction perpendicularto the orientationof the grating is fixedby the constraint line (solid diagonal lines), while the componentin the & Ahumada, 1985; Heeger, 1987) determine perceived parallel directions is ambiguous. Velocities consistent with the velocity using more biologically plausible processes. constraint line are shown by the arrows. The velocity that is usually First, the image is decomposed into its spatio-temporal perceived is shownby the solid filled arrow. (b) When these two one- components (much like what is done in Vi). Then, dimensional gratings are added together, the result is a two- velocityis estimatedby findingthe singlepatternvelocity dimensionalplaid pattern, which has a uniquevelocity. The constraint lines for each grating intersect at a single point, which determinesthe most consistentwith the entire motion-energyspectrum. unique IOC velocity of the plaid (solid filled arrow). The untilled Intersection-of-constraints(IOC) models (Fennema & arrows which represent possible velocities of the one-dimensional Thompson, 1979; Adelson & Movshon, 1982) are also gratings in (a), satisfy the constraint line of only one of the component explicitly two-staged. First, the motions of the compo- gratings,but are inconsistentwith the additionalconstraintimposedby the other grating, and therefore do not correspond to possible plaid nent gratings are estimated separately, and then the IOC velocities. rule is used to compute the pattern velocity that is consistentwith all of the componentconstraints(Fig. 1). Chubb and Sperling (1988, 1989) first proposed the existence of two motion pathways: a Fourier pathway, operating directly on the stimulus, and a non-Fourier pathway that contains a nonlinear preprocessing stage, stage of motion processing occurs in primary visual that performs a rectification or squaring prior to the cortex (VI). Motion-sensitiveneurons in V1 have small motion processing. Wilson et al. (1992) formulated a receptive fields tuned to stimuli of a specific size, motion model incorporatingboth these pathways. Their orientation, and direction of motion (Hubel & Wiesel, vector-sum model measures both Fourier and non- 1968; De Valois et al., 1982b). Each of these cells Fourier motion in separate pathways,and then combines responds maximally to the component of motion in the these signals using a vector-sum rule to compute the direction perpendicular to its preferred orientation and direction of motion of the pattern. shows little or no response to motion parallel to its In this paper, we first describe the effect of aperture preferred orientation. Thus, when an object moves, VI shape on the perceived direction of moving two- neurons respond to the local motion of one-dimensional dimensionalpatterns (plaids). We then use these results features in the image, and therefore cannot individually to determine if any of the above models can predict signal the velocity of the full two-dimensionalpattern. human performance for this type of plaid stimulus. However, the actual two-dimensional pattern velocity Preliminary results have been presented elsewhere can be recovered by combining the ,one-dimensional (Beutter et al., 1994a,b). signalsfrom multipleV1 neurons.Directionallyselective VI neurons project to the middle temporal (MT) area (Maunsell & Van Essen, 1983a;Movshon & Newsome, METHODS 1984), where most neurons appear to respond preferen- tially to motion (Zeki, 1974; Maunsell & Van Essen, Observers 1983b;Albright, 1984;Albrightet al., 1984).Neuronsin Four observers between the ages of 33 and 40yr MT have larger receptivefields(Maunsell& Van Essen, participated in the experiments. One observer, PS,was 1983b) and there is some evidence that MT neurons naive but also strabismic. integrate the one-dimensional edge motion signals to compute the two-dimensionalpattern velocity (Movshon Stimuli and apparatus et al., 1986; Rodman & Albright, 1989; Britten et al., The stimulus, Z(2,t),in these experiments was a 1993). The above notwithstanding, there is also some driftingplaid,P(Z, t), windowedspatiallyby a stationary APERTURESBIAS PERCEIVEDPLAID MOTION 3063 elongated Gaussian, W(7), and temporally by a trape- zoidal function,H(t),

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    15 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us