Perception & Psychophysics 2003, 65 (3), 429-457

The dynamical foundations of motion formation: Stability, selective , and perceptual continuity

HOWARD S. HOCK Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, Florida

GREGOR SCHÖNER Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bochum, Germany and MARTIN GIESE Max Planck Institute for Biological , Tübingen, Germany

A dynamical model is used to show that global motion pattern formation for several different ap- parent motion stimuli can be embodied in the stable distribution of activationover a population of con- currently activated, directionally selective motion detectors. The model, which is based on motion de- tectors being interactive, noisy, and self-stabilizing, accounts for such phenomena as bistability, spontaneous switching, hysteresis, and selective adaptation. Simulations show that dynamical solu- tions to the motion correspondence problem for a bistable stimulus (two qualitativelydifferent are formed) apply as well to the solution for a monostable stimulus (only one pattern is formed) and highlight the role of interactions among sequentially stimulated detectors in establishing the state de- pendence and, thereby, the temporal persistence of percepts.

In this article, an intuitivelyaccessible dynamical model motion correspondenceproblem for a bistablestimulus (for of motion pattern formation is proposed that accounts in a which two qualitativelydifferent patterns are formed) ap- unified manner for bistability,spontaneousswitching, hys- plies as well to the solution for a monostable stimulus (for teresis, and the effects of adaptation. The model is based which only one pattern is formed), and new predictions on substantialevidence that motion detectors in the visual emerge regarding solutionsto the correspondenceproblem. system are interactive (they mutually influence each oth- Our theoretical objective, therefore, is to unify tradi- er’s activation), that the activation levels of neurons fluc- tional approaches, which have emphasized what is per- tuate randomly (Softky & Koch, 1993), and most signifi- ceived when a motion stimulus is presented (i.e., the solu- cantly, that mechanisms intrinsic to individual detectors tion to the motion correspondence problem; see, e.g., stabilize their activation.On this basis, it is shown, for sev- Ullman, 1979), and dynamical approaches, which have eral different apparent motion stimuli, that perceived emphasizedthe process of perceptualchange(e.g.,Ditzinger global motion patterns can be embodied in the stable dis- & Haken, 1995; Kawamoto & Anderson, 1985; Koechlin, tribution of activation over a population of concurrently Anton, & Burnod, 1999; Schöner & Hock, 1995). Al- activated, directionally selective motion detectors; higher thoughmost of the same conceptsare inherent in other dy- order pattern detectors are not required to account for namical models (e.g., Chey, Grossberg, & Mingolla,1997; global pattern formation for these stimuli. The dynamical Grossberg & Mingolla, 1985; Grossberg & Rudd, 1989; model shows that interactions between currently stimu- Koechlin et al., 1999; Williams, Phillips, & Sekuler, 1986; lated detectors and detectors that will be stimulated in the Wilson, Ferrera, & Yo,1992), the proposed model stands immediate future are crucial for establishingthe temporal apart for the following reasons. (1) It aims for theoretical persistence of a perceived pattern (i.e., its temporal stabil- parsimony and generalizability; the dynamical represen- ity). It is demonstrated that the dynamical solution to the tation is derived from just a few very simple principles that apply irrespective of the particulars of the neurophysio- logical substrate (in contrast, e.g., with neurophysiologi- Grant supportfrom the Deutsche Volkswagenstiftungis gratefully ac- cally specific mechanisms inherent in the dynamicalmod- knowledged. We also thank David Nichols for his help with the simula- tions. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to els of such investigators as Grossberg, Mingolla, and H. S. Hock, Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, colleaguesor Koechlinet al.). (2) The functionof activation- Boca Raton, FL 33431 (e-mail: [email protected]). stabilizing mechanisms is made explicit. (3) Interactions

429 Copyright 2003 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 430 HOCK, SCHÖNER, AND GIESE between simultaneously and sequentially stimulated de- are possible (Attneave, 1974; Kolers, 1972).A “solution,” tectors are distinguished with respect to their functional which entails establishingwhich of the alternative motion significance for pattern formation and pattern stability. paths is perceived, is required for both monostable and (4) The dynamical representations are constrained by a bistable motion stimuli. For example, both horizontal and wide range of dynamical data (measurements of sponta- diagonalmotionsare possiblefor the translationalmotion neousswitching,hysteresis,and adaptation).(5) The frame- stimulusillustratedin Figure 1A, but the solutionto the mo- work is generalizableto motion pattern formation in other tion correspondence problem always results in the percep- paradigms. tion of horizontal motion (Kolers, 1972); the translational stimulus is monostable. In contrast, both horizontal and IS STABILIZATION NECESSARY? vertical motions are possible for the motion quartet illus- EVIDENCE FROM BISTABLE STIMULI trated in Figure 1B, and there are two solutionsto the mo- tion correspondence problem—one resulting in the per- Natural, everyday percepts are very stable. It rarely hap- ception of horizontal motion, the other in the perception pens that two qualitatively different percepts are formed of vertical motion;the motion quartetstimulusis bistable. for the same stimulus (bistability), and the experience of The implications of bistability are examined in this spontaneous switching between alternative percepts is study, using the motion quartet as an exemplary stimulus. likewise rare. Because everyday experiencesof monostabil- As is illustratedin Figure 1B, the motion quartet is formed ity are so pervasive, perceptual stability (and the potential by simultaneously presenting two elements located in di- for it to be lost) is not always recognized as an important agonallyoppositecorners of an imaginary rectangle, then problem for the perceptual psychologist. Historically, simultaneouslypresentingtwo elements in the other diag- however, stability was once a central issue. William James onally oppositecorners, then the first pair again, and so on (1890) famously recognized the problem of stable per- (Chaudhuri & Glaser, 1991; Hock, Kelso, & Schöner, cepts emerging from the “blooming, buzzing confusion’’ 1993; Hoeth, 1968; Kruse, Stadler, & Wehner, 1986; Ra- of sensory input, and Wolfgang Köhler (1920/1938) the- machandran & Anstis, 1985; P. von Schiller, 1933). The orized that perceptual experience was isomorphic to the elements are perceived to move either horizontallyor ver- stable, equilibrium states of electrical fields formed in the tically, but the two directions are never perceived at the brain. In both of the above, it was assumed that stabilityis same time, even though motion detectors selective for established by brain mechanisms in response to stimulus both horizontal and vertical motion are stimulated at the input. This view, however, was effectively challenged by same time. Hock et al. (1993) varied the aspect ratio of the James Gibson (1966), who attributed the stability of real- quartet, the vertical dividedby the horizontal distance be- world percepts to the tuning of our visual system to un- tween element positions,and showed that (1) perceptionis ambiguous, invariant properties of stimulation that di- bistable for aspect ratios not too different from 1.0— rectly specify the source of the stimulation, the objects in either the horizontal or the vertical motion pattern is per- the world and their spatial layout. From this perspective, ceived for the same stimulus—and (2) perception sponta- there is no “blooming, buzzing confusion’’ and no iso- neously switches between the two patterns, the switching morphism with respect to internal brain states. Percepts rate being highest for aspect ratios near 1.0. formed for natural stimuli are stable because they are uniquely specified by information in the stimulus, not be- Adaptation and Switching cause of intrinsic neural stabilizing mechanisms. In the past, perceptual switching for bistable stimuli has Although Gibson (1966) has had great impact on cur- been attributed to the adaptation,or neural fatigue, of the rent thinking, bistable stimuli for which two qualitatively perceived pattern and, at the same time, recovery from the different patterns can be formed provide an important effects of prior adaptation of previously perceived mo- constraint on theories of perception. This is because it is tions (Köhler & Wallach, 1944; Spitz & Lipman, 1962). very unlikely that stabilizing mechanisms have evolved On this basis, the pattern perceived through the course of just for the perceptionof bistable stimuli and are irrelevant these adaptation-induced changes is the one with the to pattern formation for monostablestimuli. Our expecta- greater strength. This kind of account of perceptualswitch- tion is that understanding the perception of bistable stim- ing does not require the introduction of a stabilization uli will provide insights into pattern formation when per- mechanism, but it is inconsistent with recent evidence ception is monostable. concerning the influence of adaptationon the motion pat- terns perceived for the motion quartet. Hock, Schöner, and Motion Pattern Formation Hochstein (1996) have shown that both perceived and un- Solutions to the motion correspondence problem lie at perceived motion directions simultaneously adapt for the the heart of pattern formation for global motion patterns. motion quartet; detectors respondingselectively to motion The motion correspondence problem is concerned with in horizontaland vertical directions simultaneouslyadapt how pairwise correspondencesare establishedacross suc- because they are simultaneouslystimulated(hence, one is ccessive sets of simultaneously presented elements. The not recovering from prior adaptation while the other is “problem” arises when different combinations of corre- adapting).1 In addition, Hock, Schöner, and Voss (1997) spondencematches, and therefore, different motion paths have determined that adaptation can increase the likeli- DYNAMICS OF MOTION PATTERN FORMATION 431

Figure 1. (A) Two frames of a monostable,translational apparent mo- tion stimulus for which only one percept is possible. (B) Two frames of a bistable, motion quartet stimulus for which two percepts are possible. hood of a switch for the motion quartet but that random cance of random influences on activation is particularly fluctuations in activation are the agent for the switch. evident for the bistable motion quartet when stimulus- Hence, differential adaptation of the perceptual alterna- initiated activation is very similar for both horizontal and tives is not necessary for perceptual switching.2 It can be vertical motions (at an aspect ratio slightly greater than concluded from these studies that adaptation contributes 1.0; Chaudhuri & Glaser, 1991). Then, even very small to but does not provide a sufficient account of perceptual random perturbationswould change the relative activation switching for bistable stimuli. of the two alternatives from one moment to the next. At first glance, representing the activation of a detector Random Noise and Switching (u) by the additive combination of a deterministic contri- In signal detection theory (Green & Swets, 1966), the butionfrom the stimulus(S ) and a stochastic contribution presence of random noise is treated as a problem in the de- from random perturbations (N ) might seem sufficient to tection of monostable stimuli that produce activation lev- account for the bistability of pattern formation when the els of the same order of magnitude as the random fluctu- horizontal and the vertical motions of the motion quartet ations in activation in which they are embedded. Such are equallystimulated.However, it can be seen in Figure 2A approaches, however, do not address random influences that if this were the case, switches based on whether hor- on the perception of bistable stimuli, which typically en- izontal or vertical is more activated would be too rapid to tail stimulus-initiated activation levels that are much account for the persistence of horizontal or vertical mo- greater in magnitude than the noise. (At issue is which of tion that characterizes perception during the extended in- two possible patterns is formed for a bistable stimulus, not tervals during which there are no switches. This would be the detectability of the patterns’ attributes.) The signifi- the case even if there was mutual inhibition between the 432 HOCK, SCHÖNER, AND GIESE

Figure 2. (A) Simulation of fluctuating activation for horizontal and vertical motion detectors based on the equation u = S + N, where both the stimulus activation, S = 1.0, and the strength of the Gaussian white noise, N = .03, are the same for both motion directions. (B) Same as above, except that there is mutual in- hibition between the two directions. The strength of the inhibitory coupling is 1.0. detectors responding selectively to the horizontal and the that the activation state of a detector at each moment in vertical motion directions; activation then would be re- time be predisposed by its immediately preceding activa- duced for both directions, but their levels would not sepa- tion state. The roles of interactionand stabilizationmech- rate, and rapid noise-induced switching would remain anisms in establishing this state dependence is central to (Figure 2B). the dynamical theory developed in this article. Random perturbations, therefore, can alter the relative activationof equally stimulateddetectors,but they are not Interaction and Stabilization sufficient to produce persistent activational states unless There is abundantneurophysiologicalevidence consis- perception is state dependent. That is, the formation of a tent with the essential contribution of interaction to state temporally stable percept for a bistable stimulus requires dependence.The short- and long-range connectivitythat is DYNAMICS OF MOTION PATTERN FORMATION 433 the basis for interactive detector networks has been neu- rophysiologically identified (Kisvárday, Tóth, Rausch, & Eysel, 1997; LeVay, 1988; Ts’o, Gilbert, & Wiesel, 1986), and Braitenberg (1978) has estimated that 95% of the input to each cortical neuron comes from connectivity with other cortical neurons.Neural connectivityalso is in- dicated by the nonclassical behavior of receptive fields; that is, stimuli that lie outside the classical receptive field, and thus do not directly affect its activation,can modulate the activation produced by stimuli presented inside the classical receptive field (Allman, Miezin, & McGuinness, 1985; Gilbert & Wiesel, 1990). The idea of neural con- nectivity as the basis for the formation of stable patterns can be traced back to Hebb (1949) and has led to the de- velopment of a wide variety of interactive neural network models of pattern formation in motion perception (e.g., Chey et al., 1997; Dawson, 1991; Grossberg & Mingolla, 1985; Grossberg & Rudd, 1989; Nowlan & Sejnowski, 1995;Wilson, Ferrera, & Yo,1992; Wilson & Kim, 1994). Because of this interconnectedness, each detector in a network is subject to the time-varying activational effect of the stimulus, activation-dependent interactive influ- ences from other detectors, activation-dependentadapta- tion effects, and random perturbations that increase and decrease its activation over time. All these contributionsto a detector’sactivation changes its interactive influence on other detectors, which in turn changes their influence on the detector’sactivation.This ongoingrecycling of change, or recurrence, reflects the state dependence of the net- work; the evolving activation state of a detector depends on its previous activationstate, as well as on the activation state of the detectorswith which it interacts.If unchecked, recurrence can lead to either exploding levels of activa- tion or no activation at all. However, when detectors have intrinsic mechanisms that stabilize their activation, recur- rence results in the formation of stable percepts. On the basis of such stabilizing mechanisms, the distribution of activation over a populationof interconnectedmotion de- tectors changes until the rate of change for each detector approaches zero (i.e., its activation stabilizes). It is only then that activation remains relatively unchanged for a sufficiently long period of time for a temporally persistent pattern to be realized in perception (Schöner & Hock, 1995). In this way, the stimulus does not simply specify a particular activation level. Instead, it initiates a recurrent Figure 3. Illustration of how a detector behaves in the presence cycle of activation change that culminates with activa- of a mechanism that stabilizes its activation. The rate of change tion’ssettling into a stable distribution over ensembles of of activation, du/dt (i.e., how activation is likely to change in the stimulated detectors.3 immediate future), depends inversely on the current state of ac- tivation (u). This is shown (A) without stimulus-initiated activa- CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATIONS tion and (C) with stimulus-initiated activation.The rate of change in activation is negative (activation tends to decrease) if activation OF A DYNAMICAL MODEL has been pushed upward by a random perturbation, and vice versa if it has been pushed downward. The one-dimensional vec- It is not the objective of this article to explore biophys- tor field describes how activation, from whatever level it has ical mechanisms responsible for the self-stabilization in- reached because of a random perturbation, is likely to change in the immediate future (B) without stimulus-initiated activation trinsic to the operation of individual detectors (see Koch, and (D) with stimulus-initiated activation. Activation is brought 1999; Koch & Poggio, 1987; Wilson, 1998). It suffices to toward the stable fixed point at a rate indicated by the length of characterize self-stabilization by an inverse functional re- vector. 434 HOCK, SCHÖNER, AND GIESE

Figure 4. Illustration of how the stabilizing mechanism advances the activa- tion of a detector from the no-stimulus resting level toward the fixed point level initiated by the presentation of a stimulus to which the detector is responsive. At successive moments in time, the increment in activation to a higher level u du dt ( t+1) is determined by the positive rate of change ( / ) at the current activa- u tion level ( t). Activation approaches the fixed point exponentially, at a rate de- termined by the time scale (tau) of the stabilizing mechanism. lationshipbetween the detector’sactivation(u) and its rate detector’s activation is stabilized at the value of u for of change in activation (du/dt). As was indicated above, which du/dt 5 0 (where the function crosses the horizon- the “target’’for the stabilizingmechanism is a zero rate of tal, “activation”axis in Figures 3A and 3C). In Figure 3A, change in activation, not a particular activation level; the there is no stimulus present to which the detector re- DYNAMICS OF MOTION PATTERN FORMATION 435

Figure 5. Illustration of how the stabilizing mechanism opposes perturbations that produce random fluctuationsin activation that shift activation away from the stimulus- determined fixed point. At successive moments in time, the activation value reached as a result of the perturbation is opposed by a change in activation determined by the rate of change (du/dt) at the perturbed activation value. sponds, so activation is stabilized at the detector’sresting to increase activation in the immediate future. As is illus- level, u* 5 h. The stable fixed point correspondingto this trated in Figure 5, successive random perturbations that no-stimulus resting level is below the activation level of increase and decrease activation would be opposed by the zero, which is arbitrarily designated as the threshold value stabilizingmechanism, keeping activation close to the sta- required for perception. ble fixed point value determined by the stimulus. When a stimulusis presented that is consistentwith the directional selectivity of a motion detector, an activational Vector Fields change is imposed by the stimulus that shifts the stable The dynamics for a local detector can be represented fixed pointto an activationvalueof u* 5 S 1 h (Figure 3C). graphically by a one-dimensional vector field in which a As is illustrated in Figure 4, activation will increase at a vector is attached to each value of activation. Each vector rate determined by the time scale of the stabilizingmech- points toward increased or decreased activation (in the im- anism, tau, until it reaches the stable fixed point value. If mediate future), its magnitude indicating how strongly a random perturbationwere to decrease activation relative self-stabilizing mechanisms will shift activation in the in- to the fixed point activation level of the detector, the sta- dicated direction,either without (Figure 3B) or with (Fig- bilizing mechanism would oppose this change by tending ure 3D) stimulation.The vector fields illustratethat,start- 436 HOCK, SCHÖNER, AND GIESE

modificationof activationlevels for the stimulated detec- tors by mutual interaction. If the global pattern is com- posed of both horizontal and vertical motion components (as is the case for the motion quartet), activationvariables uh and uv each can be described by a one-dimensionalvec- tor field with a stable fixed point.They also can be jointly represented by a two-dimensional vector field with a sin- gle stable fixed point (with equal stimulus-initiated acti- vation for uh and uv in Figure 6A; with greater activation for uh than for uv in Figure 6B). Each vector in the joint field is determined by the vector sum of each detector’s stabilization-determined opposition to perturbations that shift activation away from its fixed point values. Vector fields representing interactionsamong horizon- tal and vertical detectors are based on the assumptionthat the contributions of a local detector to the activation of other local detectors depends monotonically but nonlin- early on its activation. At detector activation levels near zero, the greater the activationof a detector,the greaterits interactive influence on other detectors (i.e., interaction increases with activation). However, at more negative and more positive activation levels, changes in the activation of a detector have relatively little effect on the magnitude of its interaction with other detectors. This form of non- linearity, which is adequately represented by a sigmoidal function (Grossberg, 1973), is illustrated in Figure 10. Vector fields representing the effects of inhibiting in- teractions among local motion detectors are presented in Figure 7. For the motion quartet, an increase in the acti- vation of horizontal motion detectors (uh ) means that the activation of vertical motion detectors (uv) will decrease in the immediate future. The negativechangein activation for vertical motion is indicatedby the downward arrows in Figure 7A. (The magnitudeof the change, as indicated by Figure 6. Joint two-dimensional vector fields for the variables, u u the length of each arrow, is determined by the previously v and h, representing the activation of vertical and horizontal motion detectors. The vector field is formed by the vector addi- describedsigmoidalnonlinearity;the inhibitory influence tion of two one-dimensional vector fields (a one-dimensional vec- of the horizontal detector is large when it is positively ac- tor field is illustrated in Figure 3D). The stable-fixed pointfor the tivated, small when its is negatively activated, and inter- activation variables is indicated by the large black dot. (A) Vec- mediate for activation values near zero.) The complemen- tor field when the activation of horizontal and vertical motions tary inhibiting effect of vertical activation on activation are equal. (B) Vector field when activation is asymmetrical (in this example, there is greater activation of horizontalthan of ver- for horizontalmotion is shown in Figure 7B, and the joint tical motion). vector field describing their mutual inhibitionis shown in Figure 7C (vector sums are taken at each point in the field). One can anticipate from the divergent vector field in ing from any possible activationlevel resulting from a ran- Figure 7C how nonlinear inhibiting interactions among dom perturbation, activation is likely to evolve in time the stimulateddetectors can lead to two separate activation (following the arrows) to the vicinity of the stable fixed states (bistability), rather than to a single, monostable point.The decreasingsize of the arrows in the vector field state. That is, mutual inhibitory interaction will tend to diagrams reflects the exponential decrease in the size of move activation toward either the upper left (high values the activation change produced in reaction to a perturba- of uv and low,potentiallysubthresholdvalues of uh)orto- tion as the stabilizing mechanism brings activation back ward the lower right (high values of uh and low,potentially toward the fixed pointvalue.The rate of decrease depends subthreshold values of uv). on the value of tau. Depicted in Figure 8A is a joint vector field combining the vector field representing the symmetrical contribution Global Motion Patterns of the stimulus (from Figure 6A) with the vector field rep- The formation of a global motion pattern entails(1) the resenting mutual inhibitory interaction (from Figure 7C). simultaneous stimulation of detectors at different spatial The combined vector field has two stable fixed points, or locations(there is more than one local motion)and (2) the , reflecting the bistability of perception for the DYNAMICS OF MOTION PATTERN FORMATION 437

Figure 7. Joint two-dimensional vector fields representing inhibitory inter- actions among the activation variables. (A) The inhibitory influence of the ac- u tivation variable for horizontal motion ( h ) on the activation variable for ver- u tical motion ( v). (B) The inhibitory influence of the activation variable for u u vertical motion ( v) on the activation variable for horizontal motion ( h ). (C) The combined vector fields, by vector addition, representing the mutual in- hibitory interactions between the horizontal and the vertical activation vari- ables. motion quartet. Included in Figure 8A is a dark line that from the fixed point with greaterhorizontalactivationand separates the vector field into two regions, the basins of at- toward the fixed point with greater vertical activation.The traction of the two stable fixed points.The attractorbound- shift of the boundary is the result of a change in ary (short for the boundary of the basins of attraction) the locations in the vector field where the “forces’’ at- emerges at those locations in the vector field from which tracting activation to the alternative fixed points are bal- either attractor can be reached following even a tiny ran- anced.As a result of these shifts of the fixed pointsand at- dom fluctuation in activation. The dynamics of the inter- tractor boundary, one fixed point is closer to the attractor acting detectors moves the activation state toward the sta- boundary, and the other is farther away. With sufficiently ble fixed point within one or the other basin of attraction. large asymmetries in stimulus-determined activation (for Changes in the aspect ratio of the motion quartet result large or small aspect ratios of the motion quartet), one of in different levels of stimulation for horizontal and verti- the fixed points merges with the attractor boundary, re- cal motion.For example,when the horizontalinterelement sulting in the disappearanceof one of the two basins of at- distance is shorter than the vertical interelementdistance, traction. Perception is monostable beyond these critical there is greater stimulation of horizontal than of vertical values. Pattern formation, therefore, is probabilistic(sub- motion detectors(Burt & Sperling, 1981;Gilroy,Hock, & ject to the effects of noise) only within the bistable range Ploeger, 2001). This asymmetry is represented by the joint of the stimulus. vectorfield for horizontaland vertical motion in Figure 6B. When combinedwith the vector field representingmutual Pattern Formation: What Is Perceived When a inhibitory interaction (Figure 7C), the vector field repre- Stimulus Is Presented? sentationillustrated in Figure 8B is formed. As compared The general answer to this question from a dynamical with the symmetrical case, both stable fixed points are perspective is that the perceived motion pattern depends shifted toward higher horizontaland lower vertical activa- not just on the stimulus and interaction, but also on the tion, and significantly, the attractor boundary shifts away immediately preceding activational state of the to-be- 438 HOCK, SCHÖNER, AND GIESE

tion quartet) is presented, both horizontal and vertical de- tectors are stimulated.As activation increases for these de- tectors toward stimulus-determined values, the inhibitory vector field depicted in Figure 7C is encountered. Activa- tion is thereby “steered” by the inhibitory field toward one or the other of the two fixed points establishedby the com- bination of stimulation and interaction.The likely destina- tion is determined by the location of the no-stimulus fixed point relative to the attractor boundary, as follows. For asymmetric motion quartets with aspect ratios greater than 1.0, there is more stimulation of horizontal

Figure 8. Joint two-dimensional vector fields that combine, by vector addition,the vector fields representing the stabilization of stimulus-initiated activation for the horizontal and the vertical variables (Figure 6) with the vector field representing the mutual inhibitory interactions between these variables (Figure 7C). The inhibitory field results in a bifurcation that leads to two stable fixed points: Horizontal activation is greater than vertical acti- vation near one stable fixed point (lower-right quadrant of the vector field), and vice versa near the other (upper-left quadrant of the vector field). Threshold values for motion to be perceived are at an activation value of 0, so in these examples, only hori- zontal motion is perceived when activation is near the stable fixed point in the lower-right quadrant,and only vertical motion is per- ceived when activation is near the stable fixed point in the upper- left quadrant. (A) When the combined vector field results from symmetrical stimulation of horizontaland vertical motion detec- tors, the stable fixed points are symmetrical with respect to a 45º diagonal line through the center of the vector field. Horizontal and vertical motion are equally activated at their respective sta- ble fixed points, and the 45º diagonalalso is the attractor bound- Figure 9. (A) When a stimulus is presented, the no-stimulus, ary that separates the basins of attraction for the two stable fixed stable fixed point (filled circle) is replaced by two stable fixed points. (B) When the combined vector field is based on asym- points (open circles) whose basins of attraction are separated by metrical stimulation of horizontal and vertical motion detectors, an attractor boundary (black line). What is perceived then de- the stable fixed points are asymmetrical with respect to a 45º di- pends on the position in the vector field of the no-stimulus fixed agonalline through the center of the vector field. In this example, point relative to the attractor boundary. Percepts requiring ran- horizontal motion is more strongly activated than vertical motion dom fluctuations for activation to cross the attractor boundary near their respective stable fixed points, and the attractor bound- are less likely than percepts for which the stimulus-initiated fixed ary is shifted toward the stable fixed point for vertical motion. point is on the same side of the boundary as the no-stimulusfixed point. (B) Once a percept is established, its stability depends on the distance of the stable fixed point from the attractor boundary. stimulated detectors. In the case in which there is no pre- This determines the size of the random fluctuation in activation that is necessary for activation to cross the attractor boundary ceding stimulus,activationis stabilizedat a value near the and produce a spontaneous change in perception. Switches re- detectors’ resting level and, therefore, below the threshold quiring large fluctuations are less likely than switches requiring level required for perception.When a stimulus (e.g., a mo- small fluctuations. DYNAMICS OF MOTION PATTERN FORMATION 439 detectors than of vertical motion detectors, so the fixed change the detectors’ relative activation,reversing the ef- points and attractor boundaries are shifted with respect to fect of the inhibitory vector field. As is illustrated by the the no-stimulus resting level of activation (Figures 8B and vector field representation in Figure 9A, the fluctuation 9A). The no-stimulus fixed point lies on one side of the at- would shift activation across the attractor boundary,caus- tractor boundary, in the basin of attraction for horizontal ing it to move to the otherfixed point (resulting in the per- motion, so activation will move toward the fixed point for ception of the competing, vertical motion pattern). which there is above-threshold activation for horizontal For symmetric motion quartets (and in the absence of motion and subthreshold activation for vertical motion. differential adaptation of horizontal and vertical motion), As is illustrated in Figure 10A, activation for horizontal the no-stimulus fixed point lies on the attractor boundary. motion detectors rises more quicklyfrom the no-stimulus Activation values along the attractor boundary are very level than does activation for vertical motion detectors, unstable, so even a very small random fluctuation is suf- because the former are more strongly stimulated. As a re- ficient to move activationoff the boundary (with a proba- sult, the activation of horizontal detectors reaches a level bility of .5 into one of the two basins of attraction). That (still subthreshold for perception) where they begin in- is, activation for horizontal and vertical motion detectors hibiting vertical detectors before the opposite occurs (Fig- rises at the same rate from the no-stimulus level, but ran- ure 10B). Although this usually leads to stabilization at dom fluctuations occurring while activation is still sub- the fixed point activationvalue for horizontalmotion, it is threshold will result in one motion direction beginningto possible, as the detectors’ activation is increasing, for a inhibitthe other before the reverse occurs. In this way, each random fluctuation of sufficient magnitude to suddenly fixed point will be reached equally often, so either the hor-

Figure 10. (A) The time course of activation change following the pre- sentation of a stimulus. In this example, activation rises more quickly for u u h than for v because the horizontal motion detector is more strongly u stimulated than the vertical motion detector. (B) As a result, h begins u to have an interactive influence on v before the reverse can occur. 440 HOCK, SCHÖNER, AND GIESE izontal or the vertical motion pattern will be formed with there is similar stimulation of horizontal and vertical mo- equal likelihood. tion detectors), so the inhibitory vector field resulting The dynamical solution to the motion correspondence from the adaptation of horizontal and vertical detectors problem for the motion quartet thus depends on which will have a less distortiveeffect on the attractor boundary among the stimulatedmotion detectors is the first to initi- established by the neutral test stimulus and the location of ate inhibitionof its competitorsas activationrises from the the no-stimulus fixed point relative to the attractor bound- no-stimulus level toward one or the other of the two pos- ary (Figure 11B). The no-stimulus fixed point again will sible fixed point activation levels. The horizontal pattern fall in the basin of attraction for vertical motion when the is perceived when the activation of horizontal detectors is test stimulus is presented, but it will be closer to the at- above and the activation of vertical detectors is below the tractor boundary, allowing greater opportunity for a ran- threshold level required for perception (the lower-right dom fluctuation to shift activation back into the basin of quadrant in Figure 10A), and vice versa for the perception attraction for horizontal motion. The motion pattern per- of the vertical motion pattern (the upper-left quadrant). ceived during adaptation would affect subsequent pattern formation, but not as strongly as when stimulation is Selective Adaptation and Dynamical Stability asymmetrical. When a motion detector is activated by a stimulus, its It can be concluded that adaptationis never restricted to fixed point activationlevel depends on the extent to which the motions perceived during the adaptation phase of the an attribute of the stimulus is consistent with its selectiv- experiment; it occurs for unperceived as well as perceived ity. Over time, adaptation results in a reduction of detec- motion directions. What varies is the selectivity of the tor activation that is proportional to the level of activation adaptation.That is, the effect of adaptationon subsequent initiated by the stimulus (Giaschi, Douglas, Marlin, & Cy- pattern formation is more selective for the pattern per- nander, 1993), likely through the influence of activation- ceived during adaptationwhen there is a greater difference suppressing, inhibiting mechanisms. On this basis, adap- in adaptation between perceived and unperceived motion tationprovidesa stimulus-dependentinhibitorycontribution directions. This depends on the stability of the adapting to the detector’sactivationthat is specific to its selectivity. percept (Hock et al., 1996). When the adaptingstimulusis removed, activation relaxes back toward the detector’sno-stimulusfixed point,but the Perceptual Continuity: Hysteresis adaptive effects of activation (its inhibitory vector field) Perceptual continuity depends on there being a smooth will continue to be felt even as the activation producing it transition between the activation state established by the diminishes to levels that are subthreshold for the percep- current stimulus and the new activation state that will be tion of the adapting stimulus. (The time scale of the adap- forthcoming as a result of a changein stimulation.A clear tation is assumed to be substantially slower than the time example is provided by the hysteresis observed for the scale of the detector’sstabilizing mechanism, which gov- motion quartet when its aspect ratio is gradually increased erns how quickly the detector’s activation changes.) The or decreased. When the aspect ratio favors the perception adaptation-induced inhibition shifts both the no-stimulus of the horizontal motion pattern, stabilizing mechanisms fixed point activation level and the attractor boundaryini- maintain that percept, resisting changes in aspect ratio that tiated by the subsequently presented test stimulus. When would switch perception to the vertical motion pattern. In the stimulation produced by the neutral test quartet is terms of the vector field representation, changes in para- symmetrical, the fixed point that is reached and, thus, the meter do not require that the activationalstate of the stim- pattern perceived for the test stimulus depend on the asym- ulated detectors be reestablishedfollowing a return to no- metry of the adaptation,as follows. stimulus activation values. Instead, the activation state There is much more adaptationof perceived than of un- remaining from the preceding parameter value keeps ac- perceived motion directions when the adapting percept is tivationwithin the same basin of attraction after the aspect highly stable (e.g., horizontal motion perceived when ratio has changed (and the stable fixed point and attractor there is much stronger stimulation of horizontal than of boundary of the dynamical representation are slightly vertical motion detectors). The highly asymmetrical in- shifted). This state dependencecontinuesthrough a series hibitoryvector field resulting from the adaptationwill sub- of changesin aspect ratio, the percept remaining the same stantially shift the attractor boundary established by the even as the values of aspect ratio are reached that would neutral test stimulus and the location of the no-stimulus otherwise favor the competingpercept (Hock et al., 1993). fixed point relative to the attractor boundary (Figure 11A). The changes in activation and the dynamical represen- As a result, the no-stimulus fixed point will fall well tationinitiatedby successive changesin the aspect ratio of within the basin of attractionfor vertical motion when the the motion quartet are illustrated in Figure 12. As the as- test stimulus is presented, so it is very likely that activa- pect ratio is gradually increased or gradually decreased, tion will move to the fixed point within that basin. The the stable fixed point of the initially established percept, motion pattern perceived during adaptation would have a as well as the current activation state, shifts toward lower strong influence on subsequent pattern formation. levels of activation, the attractor boundary shifting in the Adaptationis less asymmetricalwhen the adaptingper- opposite direction (toward the fixed point). An aspect cept is less stable (e.g., horizontal motion perceived when ratio eventually is reached for which the fixed point DYNAMICS OF MOTION PATTERN FORMATION 441

Figure 11. Illustration of how adaptation affects pattern formation for a bistable neutral test stimulus following an interval during which neither the adaptation nor the test stimulus is presented. The prior adaptation shifts the fixed points and attractor boundary of the vector field formed when the test stimulus is presented, as well as the no-stimulus activation level reached dur- ing the interval preceding the presentation of the test stimulus. (A) Asymmet- rical stimulation (more horizontal than vertical stimulation) during the adap- tation phase results in asymmetrical adaptation; the adapting vector field, indicated by light gray arrows, points mostly toward lower levels of horizontal activation (adaptation is strongly selective to the perceived horizontal motion). The no-stimulus activation state falls well within the basin of attraction for the vertical motion pattern, the perception of which is highly probable. (B) Sym- metrical stimulation (equal horizontal and vertical stimulation) during the adaptation phase results in less asymmetrical adaptation than that shown in panel A; the adapting vector field points toward lower levels of horizontal and vertical activation (adaptation is less selective to the perceived horizontal mo- tion). The no-stimulus activation state falls near the attractor boundary, so given the occurrence of fluctuations large enough for activation to cross the at- tractor boundary, the perception of horizontal motion is more likely than that shown in panel A. merges with the boundary, whereupon there is a bifurca- creasing the parameter. However, switches due specifically tion marked by a sudden switch to a much different acti- to such bifurcationsare difficult to observe in the presence vational state near the fixed point of the only remaining of random noise. This is because, as the fixed point gets basin of attraction. Hysteresis occurs when two such bi- closer to the bifurcationpoint, the likelihoodincreases that furcation points exist, one encountered by gradually in- a spontaneousfluctuationwill “prematurely”cross activa- creasing a relevant parameter, the other by gradually de- tion into the opposing basin of attraction, before the ini- 442 HOCK, SCHÖNER, AND GIESE

ceived at the top of the quartet when that motion is stim- ulated, but rightward motion will not be stimulated again at that locationuntil after an interval of time during which leftward motionis stimulatedinstead. The dynamicsmust account for this periodic flow of activation between such alternately stimulateddetectors, as well as for the switches between competing solutions to the motion correspon- dence problem that result from either random changes in activation or changes in stimulation. Eight directionally selective motion detectors are re- quired to represent the eight motions that can occur for the motion quartet over a succession of frame changes (Fig- ure 13A). Four are stimulatedduring the first frame change, the other four, with the oppositedirectionalselectivity,are stimulated during the second frame change, and so on, back and forth. The eightdynamicalequationsrequired to account for the time-varying activationof each motion di- rection are presented in the Appendix. Each of the eight activation variables is influenced by interaction with the

Figure 12. Hysteresis effect resulting from transitions between bistability and monostability. (A) As the aspect ratio is increased during ascending trials, the stable fixed point for vertical motion moves toward the attractor boundary, and the attractor bound- ary moves toward that stable fixed point. There is then a bifur- cation. The fixed point disappears as it merges with the attractor boundary, and there is a sudden transition to the fixed point for horizontal motion, the only fixed point that remains (perception becomes monostable). (B) The same occurs during descending trials, except that the transition pointis at a different aspect ratio. The two bifurcation points are reached when there are no ran- dom fluctuations that result in spontaneous jumps across the at- tractor boundary before it merges with a fixed point. tially occupied basin of attraction has the opportunity to disappear (Hock et al., 1993).

A DYNAMICAL MODEL

Accounting for phenomena such as hysteresis with the motion quartet provides a strong challenge to dynamical Figure 13. (A) Eight motion directions for the motion quartet stimulus, the basis of Simulations 1–8. (B) Eight motion direc- theory, because continuitymust be establishedacross tem- tions for the translating rod stimulus, the basis for Simulation10. poral intervals during which relevant detectors are not (C) Luminance bars that increase the activation of diagonalmo- stimulated. For example, rightward motion might be per- tion detectors for the translating rod, the basis for Simulation11. DYNAMICS OF MOTION PATTERN FORMATION 443 other seven. The latter include (1) inhibitory interactions elements, the distance between pairs of elements constitut- among orthogonalmotion directions at each corner of the ing a particularlystrong determinantof affinity that leads to quartet that are stimulatedduring the same frame change, the nearest neighbor solution (e.g., Burt & Sperling, (2) inhibitory interactions among orthogonal motion di- 1981; Hock et al., 1993; Shechter, Hochstein, & Hillman, rections at each corner of the quartet that are stimulated 1988; Ullman, 1979). Minimal mapping additionally de- during successive frame changes, (3) inhibitory interac- pends on the modificationof stimulus-determinedelement tions between opposite motion directions on opposite affinities by local split competition (when an element pre- sides of the quartet, (4) inhibitory interactionsbetween de- sented during one time interval has possible matches with tectors with motion in opposite directions over the same two or more elements presented during the next time in- path, and (5) excitatory interactionsbetween the same di- terval) and local fusion competition(when two or more el- rections on opposite sides of the quartet.4 ements presented during one time interval have the same element as a possible match during the next time interval). Pattern Formation This competitionis captured in the dynamicalmodel by in- The results of simulationsbased on this dynamicalmodel hibitory interaction among motion detectors that respond of motion pattern formation are contrasted with predictions selectively to different motion directions. based on Ullman’s(1979) minimal mapping solution to the Ullman (1979) further specified that the effectiveness motion correspondence problem. Ullman has argued that of split and fusion competitiondependson the presence of solutions to the correspondence problem depend on the differences in affinity for the pairs of elements defining stimulus-determined affinity of potentially corresponding the alternative motion paths; there is no competitionwith-

Figure 14. Simulation 1: the time-varying activation for each of the eight motion directions of the mo- tion quartet. Either the horizontal or the vertical motion directions receive activation above the threshold level (zero) required for perception. The simulation is based on the equal stimulation of all motion direc- S 5 S 5 tions ( h v 16), the implementation of the detector interactions described in the text, and a level of noise strength (N 5 0.7) sufficient to produce spontaneous changes between the horizontal and the verti- cal motion patterns. 444 HOCK, SCHÖNER, AND GIESE out differences in affinity, so the alternativemotions of an strength) over the entire set of possible element pairings. element would be simultaneously perceived. With the ad- Dawson (1991) subsequently developed a connectionist ditional constraint that all elements are covered (i.e., ele- model of motion correspondence that incorporates Ull- ments cannot simply appear and disappear), Ullman’s man’s principles, again stressing the relative distance be- minimal mapping solution determines the set of element tween the elements (or equivalently, the relative velocity pairings that minimizes cost (maximizes correspondence of the competing motions) as a primary constraint.

Figure 15. Simulation2: the time-varying activation for two of the motion directions of the motion quartet (all eight are included in the simulation).Stimulationis equal for all eight mo- tion directions, but there is less noise strength than in Simulation 1. (A) A reduced noise level (N = 0.07) is sufficient to result in the formation of either the horizontal or the vertical mo- tion pattern. (B) Without noise (N = 0), there is always equal activation of all motion detec- tors. (C) This is the case even when stimulation of horizontal and vertical directions are S S greatly increased ( h = v =24). DYNAMICS OF MOTION PATTERN FORMATION 445

Simulation 1: Dynamical solution to the motion to rise above threshold; that is, there is a spontaneous correspondence problem. The initial simulation in Fig- change from the perception of the vertical motion pattern ure 14 is based on the equal stimulation of all horizontal to the perception of the horizontal motion pattern. Con- and vertical directions,the presence of interactionsamong sistent with Hock, Schöner, and Voss’s(1997) experimen- the detectors (including those stimulated during the same tal results, this shows that the presence of random noise is and successive frame changes), and a level of noise suffi- sufficientto accountfor perceptual switching;differential cient to produce perceptual switching. It can be seen that adaptation of horizontal and vertical detectors is not nec- for the first seven pairs of frame changes of the simulated essary for switching,for the obviousreason that there was trial, there is periodicflow of back-and-forth activation of no adaptation,differential or otherwise, in this simulation. vertical motion over the same path (alternating up/down Finally,the simulationindicatesthat inhibitorydetector motion as the two directions are alternately stimulated). interactions, together with stabilizingmechanisms intrin- For example, downward motion continuesto be perceived sic to each detector, are sufficient to capitalize on noise- on the right side of the quartet over the succession of induced differences in activation between the competing frame changes during which it is stimulated, even though horizontaland vertical directions,separating their activation there are interveningframes during which upward motion values (in contrast with Figure 2, for which there was no is stimulated over the same motion path. separation).Even thoughthere was equalhorizontaland ver- It also can be seen in Figure 14 that activation is closely tical stimulation (in Ullman’s[1979] terms, equal affinity), coupled for the vertical detectors on the left and the right activation for either horizontal or vertical motion was sides of the quartet (at an above-threshold level early in above the threshold for perception, but never both simul- the simulated trial), as well as for all the horizontaldetec- taneously. This was consistent with experimental obser- tors on the top and the bottom of the quartet (at a sub- vation and contrary to the predictionof Ullman’sminimal threshold level early in the simulated trial), signifying the mapping theory that competing motions are perceived si- perception of the vertical motion pattern. The simulation multaneously when affinities are matched (because there thus demonstrates how a perceived motion pattern can be is no split or fusion competition without differences in embodied in the stable distribution of activation over a affinity). populationof low-level, directionallyselective motion de- Simulation 2: The effect of noise. In the preceding tectors. That is, after each frame change, activation values simulation,there was sufficient noise to produce solutions stabilize near a fixed-point attractor, as described in the to the motion correspondence problem (either horizontal first part of this article. It is not necessary to evoke more or vertical motion is perceived, never both at the same specialized, higher order pattern detectors (e.g., detectors time) despite equal stimulation of horizontal and vertical that respond selectively to shearing motions, which occur motion detectors. Solutions also can be obtained with across opposite sides of the motion quartet; see, e.g., much less noise—for example, 10% of the noise strength Tanaka, Fukada, & Saito, 1989). in Simulation 1 (Figure 15A). However, in the complete After the seventh pair of frame changes, there is a ran- absence of noise,there would be no solutionto the motion dom fluctuation in the simulation of sufficient magnitude correspondence problem (Figure 15B); when horizontal for the activation of all vertical detectors to drop below and vertical stimulation are matched, noise is necessary threshold and for the activation of all horizontal detectors to break the . When both directions are more

Figure 16. Simulation3: the time-varying activation for two of the motion directions of the S S motion quartet when there is more stimulation of horizontal ( h = 16.5) than vertical ( v = 16.0) motion directions. Although the relative stimulus strengths favor the perception of hor- izontal motion, the presence of noise (N = 0.7) is sufficient to result in above-threshold acti- vation for vertical motion, violating the nearest neighbor principle. 446 HOCK, SCHÖNER, AND GIESE

Figure 17. Simulation4: the time-varying activation for two of the motion directions of the motion quartet when the interactive coupling of all simultaneously stimulated detectors is set to zero. Regardless of whether all motion directions are equally stimulated (A) or there is S 5 S 5 more stimulation of horizontal ( h 18) than of vertical motion directions ( v 16), both horizontal and vertical detectors receive above-threshold activation. strongly stimulated,equal above-thresholdactivationlev- ping theory, it can account for the perception of motion els are obtained for both, incorrectly signifying their si- patterns that violate the nearest neighbor principle (Hock multaneousperception (Figure 15C). Activationlevels for et al., 1993). This is illustratedby Simulation3 (Figure 16), this simulation are low because, in the absence of noise, which shows that the verticalmotion pattern can be formed neither motion direction is able to dominate, so the acti- even though differences in motion path length result in vation of both is greatly reduced by inhibition. more stimulationof horizontalthan of vertical motion de- Neither Ullman’s (1979) minimal mapping theory nor tectors. (Gilroy et al., 2001,have providedevidencefor an Dawson’s (1991) connectionist version includes the ran- inverse linear relationship between detector activation and dom fluctuations that are necessary to account for solu- motion path length.) Switching occurs relatively early in tions to the correspondenceproblem that occur in percep- this simulation,reflecting the instabilityof patterns formed tion when stimulus-initiated activation is matched for when the stimulus favors the competing pattern (see also competing motion directions. (In this set of simulations the vector field representation in Figure 9B). and those that follow,the graphs represent the time course Simulation 4: The function of interactions among si- of activation for only one horizontal and one vertical di- multaneously stimulated detectors. Interactions among rection. The periodic flow of back-and-forth activation simultaneously stimulated detectors are the basis for so- and the close coupling of all horizontal motions and all lutions to the motion correspondence problem and, thus, vertical motions reoccur in every simulation.) the formation of motion patterns. It can be seen for Simu- Simulation 3: Noise-induced violationsof the near- lation 4 that removing detector interactionsamong simul- est neighbor principle. An important feature of the dy- taneously stimulated horizontal and vertical detectors re- namical representation is that, contrary to minimal map- sults in their simultaneous above-threshold activation, DYNAMICS OF MOTION PATTERN FORMATION 447

Figure 18. Simulation5: the time-varying activation for two of the motion directions of the motion quartet when the interactive coupling of all sequentially stimulated detectors is set to zero. Horizontal motion and vertical motion are not perceived at the same time, but neither motion pattern is temporally persistent. This is shown for two levels of noise strength: (A) N 5 0.7 and (B) N 5 0.2. regardless of whether horizontal and vertical motion are the means by which pattern formation during a frame is equally(Figure 17A) or unequally(Figure 17B) stimulated. influencedby the preceding activationalstate. Persistence This signifies the simultaneous perception of horizontal does not occur without this state dependence. and vertical motion, a failure to solve the motion correspon- dence problem that never occurs for the motion quartet. Continuity in Perception Simulation 5: The function of interactions among The preceding simulation shows that state dependence successively stimulated detectors. In contrast with the accounts for the continuityof a perceived motion direction previous simulation, which removed interactions among even though there are intervening intervals during which simultaneously stimulated detectors, Simulation 5 re- that direction is not stimulated. The simulations that fol- moves interactionsamong motion detectors that are stim- low account for perceptual continuity despite changes in ulated during successive frame changes. The motion cor- how strongly a motion directionis stimulatedas compared respondence problem continues to be solved following with competing motion directions. each frame change (the perception of either horizontal or Simulation 6: Hysteresis. Hysteresis effects are sim- vertical motion is signified), but there is no persistence of ulatedin Figure 19 by changingthe aspect ratio of the mo- perception from one frame to the next (Figure 18A). That tion quartet, gradually increasing or gradually decreasing is, the pattern formed when the stimulus is presented (in the path length for vertical motion. This decreases or in- this simulated trial, the horizontal motion pattern) imme- creases the stimulation of vertical detectors while the diately switches to the verticalmotion patternand then in- stimulation of horizontal detectors remains constant (the coherentlyback and forth between the two. The same occurs length of the horizontal motion path is fixed). The occur- when the simulation is done with less noise (Figure 18B). rence of hysteresis is indicated in the simulation by dif- The simulationsthus demonstrate that “future shaping” ferent switching points (denoted by arrows), dependingon interactions among successively stimulated detectors are whether the initial aspect ratio favored the formation of 448 HOCK, SCHÖNER, AND GIESE

Figure 19. Simulation6: the time-varying activation for two of the motion directions of the motion quartet when its aspect ratio is gradually decreased by decreasing the path length of vertical motion (A) or is gradually increased by increasing the path length of vertical motion (B). Hysteresis is indicated by switching points (bifurcations) occurring at different aspect ratios, depending on the direction in which the aspect ratio of the quartet is changed.

the horizontal or the vertical motion pattern (see also the equal horizontal/vertical stimulation are in the center of vector field representation in Figure 12). For trials com- each graph). The simulation is thus consistent with Hock posed of a descending sequence of aspect ratios, the hor- et al.’s (1993) evidence for hysteresis.5 Perceived motion izontal motion pattern is perceived despite decreases in directions persist despite changes in path length that result the length of the vertical motion path relative to the length in the greater stimulationof competingorthogonalmotion of the horizontalmotion path to values favoring the verti- directionsand despitethere beinginterveningframes dur- cal motion pattern, and vice versa for trials composed of ing which the opposite motions are stimulated over the an ascending sequence of aspect ratios (the frames with same motion path. Because the nearest neighbor con- DYNAMICS OF MOTION PATTERN FORMATION 449

Figure 20. Simulation 7: the time-varying activation for two of the motion directions of the mo- tion quartet when its aspect ratio is gradually decreased by decreasing the path length of vertical motion, but with the interactive coupling of all sequentially stimulated detectors set to zero (as in Simulation 5). Hysteresis effects are lost; switching occurs as soon as the aspect ratio reverses from favoring horizontalmotion to favoringvertical motion.This is shown for two levels of noise strength: (A) N 5 0.7 and (B) N 5 0.2. straint is so strong in minimal mapping theory, as well as because pattern formation for each stimulus is strongly af- in Dawson’s(1991) connectionistversion, neither can ac- fected by the immediatelypreceding activationstate of the count for its reversal as a result of hysteresis. stimulated motion detector network. Simulation 7: Hysteresisrequires state dependence. Simulation 8: Selective adaptation. Continuityin per- In this simulation, the interactions among motion detec- ception is adversely affected by adaptation, particularly tors that are stimulated during successive frame changes when there is much more stimulation and, therefore, much are removed. For simulated trials with descending aspect more adaptation of the perceived than of the unperceived ratios, the elimination of these interactionsis sufficient to motion directions (i.e., when adaptation is highly selec- eliminate hysteresis effects; switching occurs when the tive for the motion perceived during adaptation).This is il- relative stimulation of horizontaland vertical detectors re- lustrated in the first part of this simulation (Figure 21A), verses. Vertical motion and horizontal motion are per- which shows much greater adaptation of the perceived ceived equally often at the point of reversal, as is shown horizontal motion direction than of the unperceivedverti- for two noise levels in Figure 20. cal motion direction, as well as the discontinuity in per- It can be concluded that interactions among succes- ception that is likely as a result of the asymmerical adap- sively stimulated detectors, which are the means through tation. That is, it is probable that there will be a switch which perception is state dependent (see Simulation 5), from the perception of horizontal motion to the percep- are necessary in order to obtain hysteresis effects. That is, tion of vertical motion when the adapting stimulus is re- when a sequence of stimuli is presented, hysteresis occurs placed by a neutral test stimulus. 450 HOCK, SCHÖNER, AND GIESE

Figure 21. Simulation 8: The time course of adaptation and the time-varying activation for two of the S 5 motion directions of the motion quartet. (A) When there is more stimulation of horizontal ( h 24) than S 5 of vertical ( v 16) motion during the adaptation phase, adaptationis much greater for the perceived hor- izontal than for the unperceived vertical motion. Following a blank interval, the large asymmetry in adap- tation typically results in switching to vertical motion during the test phase. (B) When there is equal stim- S 5 S 5 ulation of horizontal ( h 20) and vertical ( v 20) motion during the adaptation phase, adaptation is similar for the perceived horizontal and unperceived vertical motion. Following a blank interval, the rela- tively symmetrical adaptation frequently results in the continued perception of horizontal motion during the test phase.

When horizontal and vertical motion are equally stimu- unique prediction in which motions thought to be impos- lated, adaptation is less selective to the perceived horizon- sible are perceived. tal motion than in the above case. This is illustrated in the Simulation 9: The motion triplet. It was shown in Sim- second part of this simulation (Figure 21B), which shows ulation 4 that interactions among simultaneously stimu- similar adaptationfor the perceivedhorizontaland the un- lated detectors are necessary to solve the motion corre- perceived vertical motion directions, as well as the conti- spondence problem for the motion quartet. The effect of nuity in perception that is more likely as a result of less those interactions is to push the activation of all the stim- asymmetrical adaptation. That is, it is less probable that ulated horizontal motion detectors or all the stimulated there will be a switch from the perception of horizontal vertical detectors below the threshold level required for motion to the perception of vertical motion when the perception. When the visual element in one corner of the adapting stimulus is replaced by a neutral test stimulus. motion quartet is removed, motions that would start or end at that location are eliminated. The four remaining mo- Generalization and Predictions tions for the motion triplet are illustrated in Figure 22A. The simulations that follow show that the dynamical The critical case occurs when one motion path (e.g., representation for the motion quartet generalizes to the vertical) is much longer than the other. On the basis of motion triplet and lead to a unique prediction regarding minimal mapping theory, distance-determined affinities the simultaneous perception of horizontal and vertical for the triplet would be sufficiently different for horizon- motion. The dynamical representation also generalizes to tal motion to be perceived on the basis of the nearest a monostabletranslational motion stimulus and leads to a neighbor constraint. For the dynamical representation, DYNAMICS OF MOTION PATTERN FORMATION 451

Figure 22. Simulation9: (A) three frames of the motion triplet stimulus that is the basis for this simulation. (B) the time-varying activation for two of the motion directions of the mo- tion triplet. Both horizontaland vertical detectors have above-threshold activation when they are stimulated, leading to the prediction (inconsistent with the nearest neighbor solution) that horizontal and vertical motion can be perceived simultaneously for the motion triplet. eliminating motions starting or ending at the upper-right nique in which Ullman’s (1979) cover principle (i.e., an corner of the quartet eliminates their potential inhibitory element cannot appear and disappear) was not a factor.6 influences on other stimulated detectors. As can be seen Simulation 10: The monostable perception of trans- for Simulation 9 (Figure 22B), this results in the simulta- lational motion. The accurate perception of motion di- neous above-threshold activation of both horizontal and rection for a translating rod depends on the perceived mo- vertical motion (all parameters other than the stimulus tion of its end points. Otherwise, the aperture problem strength for vertical motion are the same as those for the arises, and motion is perceived in the direction orthogonal motion quartet, Simulation 1). The simultaneous percep- to the orientation of the rod (Adelson & Movshon, 1982; tion of horizontal and vertical motion is predicted by the Wallach, 1976). The perception of the motion paths for dynamical simulation, even though the activation levels the end points of a horizontallytranslatingrod is the basis for the two motion directions are different. for this simulation. The motion correspondence problem Contrary to minimal mappingtheory and consistentwith remains to be solved because the diagonal motion paths il- the prediction from the dynamical representation of the lustrated in Figure 13B are logically possible. motion triplet,Gilroy et al. (2001)have found that both hor- The purposeof this simulationwas to determinewhether izontal motion and vertical motion are perceived simulta- the dynamical representation developed for the motion neously for the motion triplet, even when detector activa- quartet also solves the motion correspondence problem tion is stronger for horizontal motion because its motion for the translating rod stimulus. Some of the motion di- path is substantiallyshorter than the vertical motion path. rections for this stimulus are different from the motion It is noteworthy that (1) the difference in path length for quartet (diagonal motions replace vertical motions), so the motion triplet was sufficient to result in the monostable somewhat stronger interaction strengths are introduced perceptionof horizontalmotion for the motion quartet and into the simulationfor motion directionsthat differ by 45º (2) the simultaneousperception of horizontaland vertical than for motion directionsthat differ by 90º. This was based motion for the motion triplet was obtained with a tech- on Marshak and Sekuler’s (1979) evidence involving the 452 HOCK, SCHÖNER, AND GIESE

Figure 23. (A) Simulation 10: The time-varying activation for two of the motion directions of the translating rod stimulus, as illustrated in Figure 13B. The perception of horizontalmo- tion always is signified. (B) Simulation 11: the time-varying activation for two motion direc- tions of the translating rod stimulus when luminance bars increase the activation of diago- S 5 nal motions, as illustrated in Figure 13C ( d 20, as compared with 14 in Simulation 10). Bistable diagonal and horizontal motion perception is signified.

“repulsion” of perceived motion direction. In addition, ceived for the translating rod stimulus could be taken as longer path lengths for the diagonal than for the horizon- indicative of a strong constraint against perceiving them tal motions are accounted for by weaker motion strengths (Kolers, 1972). However, it can be predicted from the dy- for the diagonal motions (Gilroy et al., 2001). namical representation that the perception of diagonal As is illustratedin Simulation10 (Figure 23A), the per- motions would become possible if the stimulus-initiated ception of horizontal translationalmotion is always signi- activation was sufficiently boosted for the diagonal mo- fied (the stimulus is monostable). The reason is that hori- tions, as compared with the horizontal motions. This is zontal motions in the same direction at the top and the demonstrated in Simulation 11, which differs from Simu- bottom of the stimulus are simultaneously stimulated, so lation 10 only with respect to the increased stimulus- they have mutually excitatory effects on each other’sacti- initiated activation of the diagonal motions (Figure 23B). vation, and in addition,the crossing diagonal motions are Switching from diagonal to horizontal motion occurs in simultaneously stimulated, so they strongly inhibit each the simulated trial after the sixth pair of frame changes, other. The same dynamical representation therefore can signifying perceptual bistability. account for pattern formation for both the bistable motion The means for testing this predictioncome from Gilroy quartet and the monostable translationalstimulus. Differ- et al.’s(2001) evidence that motion detectoractivationcan ences in what the motion patterns look like for these stim- be increased by placing a brighter-than-backgroundlumi- uli largely stem from differences in which motion direc- nance bar between the start and the end of the motion path tions are stimulated simultaneously and which are (Figure 13C). Consistentwith this prediction,either diag- stimulated sequentially. onal or horizontal motion is perceived when this is done Simulation 11: Making the translational stimulus for the translationalstimulus, with the same kind of switch- bistable. The fact that diagonal motions are never per- ing behavior as that predicted by Simulation 11. DYNAMICS OF MOTION PATTERN FORMATION 453

GENERAL DISCUSSION mapping theory that only motion over the shorter path would be perceived for the motion triplet. The dynamicalmodel and simulationsin this article and More generally, the reported simulations show that the in Giese (1999) are based on just a few, very general neural perceived motion pattern can be embodied in the distri- principles: (1) Detectors respond selectively to particular bution of activation over a populationof low-level, direc- stimulus attributes at particular locations on the retina, as tionally selective motion detectors. At least for the stim- determined by their receptive field properties; (2) detec- uli studiedthus far, neithera decisionprocess in which the tor activationis stabilized by intrinsic neural mechanisms; “costs” of alternative correspondence solutions are mini- (3) detectors are interconnected; they can enhance each mized (Ullman, 1979) nor higher order pattern detectors other’sactivationthrough activation-dependentexcitatory are necessary to account for motion pattern formation. So- interactionsand/or diminisheach other’sactivationthrough lutions to the motion correspondence problem are inher- activation-dependentinhibitory interactions; (4) detectors ent in most dynamical theories, but in none of them is the are subject to random influences on their activation level; functional significance of activation-stabilizing mecha- and (5) adaptation reduces a detector’sactivation in pro- nisms made explicit,and in none are interactionsbetween portion to its current level of activation. simultaneously and successively stimulated detectors dis- It has been shown in this article that when these princi- tinguishedwith respect to their functionalsignificance for ples are embedded in a dynamical representation,they can pattern formation and pattern stability. account for pattern formation, pattern stability,continuity in perception, and selective adaptation for the motion Conclusion: The Process of Pattern Formation quartet. In addition, Giese (1999) has shown that, in sim- In conclusion, the theory and simulations presented in plified form, the same principles can be the basis for this article provide an account of motion pattern formation quantitative simulations of the results of psychophysical that is potentially generalizable to a wide range of visual experiments based on the motion quartet. Although all stimuli. Accordingly, pattern formation begins with the these phenomena could potentially have been comprehen- presence of a stimulus that potentially stimulates a num- sively addressed by earlier dynamicalmodels (e.g., Bartsch ber of different detectors. As activation rises for each de- & van Hemmen, 1997;Carmesin & Arndt, 1996; Ditzinger tector, a number of different influences on its activation & Haken, 1995; Francis & Grossberg, 1996; Grossberg & are encountered: (1) intrinsic stabilization mechanisms Mingolla,1985;Grossberg & Rudd,1989,1992;Kawamoto that determine the rate at which activation increases to- & Anderson, 1985; Koechlin et al., 1999; Nowlan & Sej- ward its stimulus-determinedvalue, (2) residual inhibition nowski, 1995; Williams et al., 1986), this is the first time from prior adaptation,(3) residual interaction from previ- that it has in fact been done. The results of the dynamical ously stimulated detectors (the vehicle for state depen- simulations in this article have been contrasted with Ull- dence), and (4) interactions from simultaneously stimu- man’s(1979)minimal mappingtheory for explaininghow lated detectors. All these influencesare felt, and the motion the visualsystem solves the motioncorrespondenceprob- correspondence problem solved, through their effect on lem. It was demonstrated that because random noise and the relative activation of the stimulated detectors, before stabilizationmechanisms are not a part of Ullman’stheory the activation of any of the stimulated detectors crosses (or Dawson’s, 1991, connectionist version), it cannot ac- the threshold levelrequired for perception.These solutions count for either horizontal or vertical motion being per- were shown to apply to monostable as well as to bistable ceived for the motion quartet (but not both simultane- pattern formation. In either case, stabilizationmechanisms ously) when their when their path-length–dependent that respond quicklyto noise-inducedfluctuationsin acti- affinities are matched. Nor can it account for noise- vation are necessary for coherent motion perception,sug- induced violationsof the nearest neighborprinciple when gesting that the prevention of noise-inducedincoherence, their path-length dependent affinities are not matched. It not just the impossibility of bistability, is why everyday was further demonstrated that interactions between suc- experience is so stable. cessively stimulated detectors provide the state depen- Finally, the dynamical framework described in this ar- dence that is the basis for continuityin perception despite ticle for the motionquartetwas shown to generalizeto pat- gaps in time during which a motion is not stimulated and tern formation for other stimuli. This was shown for both despitechangesin stimulationthat favor a competingmo- the motion triplet and the translatingrod stimulus(the lat- tion percept. These “future-shaping” interactions, which ter with and without the luminancebars that foster the per- are not a part of Ullman’s theory (or Dawson’s connec- ception of crossing motion). Although there was a large tionist version), account for hysteresis-inducedviolations number of parameters in the simulations, a wide range of of the nearest neighbor principle. Finally, the simultane- phenomenawere addressed, each parameter was linked to ous perception of horizontal and vertical motion for the an identifiable, experimentally relevant parameter (e.g., motion triplet, despite large differences in path-length– stimulus strength, interaction strength, noise level), and determined affinities, can be accounted for by the same parameter values were held constant across a number of dynamical representation (although lacking the motions different simulations.Moreover, the results of the simula- that were not stimulated)that was the basis for the motion tions were not highlydependenton the specific parameter quartet simulations. It incorrectly followed from minimal values that were chosen. Currently in progress is an ex- 454 HOCK, SCHÖNER, AND GIESE tension of the dynamical approach to the counterphase Grossberg,S., & Rudd, M. E. (1992). Cortical dynamics of visual mo- row of dots, a directionally ambiguous apparent motion tion perception: Short-range and long-range apparent motion. Psy- stimulus for which motion pattern formation is self- chological Review, 99, 78-121. Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley. organized,ruling out the detection of higher order pattern- Hock, H. S., & Balz, G. W. (1994). Spatial scale dependent in-phase specifying features as the basis for pattern perception and anti-phase directional biases in the perception of self-organized (Hock & Balz, 1994; Hock, Park, & Schöner, 2001). motion patterns. Vision Research, 34, 1843-1861. Hock, H. S., Kelso, J. A.S., & Schöner, G. (1993).Bistability and hys- REFERENCES teresis in the organization of apparent motion patterns. Journal of Ex- perimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 19, 63-80. Adelson, E. H., & Movshon, J. A. (1982). Phenomenal coherence of Hock, H. S., Kogan, K., & Espinoza, J. K. (1997). Dynamic, state- moving visual patterns. Nature, 300, 523-525. dependent thresholds for the perception of single-element apparent Allman, J., Miezin, F., & McGuinness, E. (1985). Direction- and motion: Bistability from local cooperativity. Perception & Psy- velocity-specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field chophysics, 59, 1077-1088. in the middle temporal visual area (MT). Perception, 14, 105-126. Hock, H. S., Park, C. L., & Schöner, G. (2001, November). A dynami- Attneave, F. (1974). Apparent movement and the what–where connec- cal model for self-organized motion pattern formation. Paper presented tion. Psychologia, 17, 108-120. at the 42nd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Orlando, FL. Bartsch, A., & van Hemmen, J. L. (1997). A neural model of strobo- Hock, H. S., Schöner, G., & Hochstein, S. (1996). Perceptual stabil- scopic alternative motion. Network, 8, 107-126. ity and the selective adaptation of perceived and unperceived motion. Braitenberg, V. (1978). Cortical architechtonics: General and areal. Vision Research, 36, 3311-3323. In M. A. B. Brazier & H. Petsche (Eds.), Architechtonics of the cere- Hock, H. S., Schöner, G.,& Voss, A. (1997). The influence of adapta- bral cortex (IBRO MonographSeries, Vol. 3, pp. 443-466).New York: tion and stochastic fluctuations on spontaneous perceptual changes Raven. for bistable stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 509-522. Burt, P., & Sperling, G. (1981). Time, distance, and feature trade-offs Hoeth, F. (1968). Bevorzugte Richtungen bei stroboskopischen Alter- in visual apparent motion. Psychological Review, 88, 171-195. nativbewegungen [Preferred directions in stroboscopic alternative Carmesin, H.O., & Arndt, S. (1996).A neural network model for stro- motion]. Psychologische Beiträge, 16, 494-527. boscopic alternative motion. Biological Cybernetics, 75, 239-251. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt. Chang, J. J., & Julesz, B. (1984). Cooperative phenomena in apparent Kawamoto, A. H., & Anderson, J. A. (1985). A neural network model movement perception of random-dot cinematograms. Vision Re- of multistable perception. Acta Psychologia, 59, 35-65. search, 24, 1781-1788. Kisvárday,Z.F., Tóth, E., Rausch,M., & Eysel,U. (1997).Orientation- Chaudhuri, A., & Glaser, D. A. (1991). Metastable motion anistropy. specific relationship between populations of excitatory and inhibitory Visual Neuroscience, 7, 397-407. lateral connections in the visual cortex of the cat. , 7, Chey, J. S., Grossberg, S., & Mingolla,E. (1997). Neural dynamics 605-618. of motion grouping:From aperture ambiguity to object speed and di- Koch, C. (1999). Biophysics of computation. New York: Oxford Uni- rection. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 14, 2570-2594. versity Press. Dawson, M. R. W. (1991). The how and why of what went where in ap- Koch, C., & Poggio, T. (1987). Biophysics of computation: Neurons, parent motion: Modelling solutions to the motion correspondence synapses, and membranes. In G. M. Edelman, W. E. Gall, & W. M. problem. Psychological Review, 98, 569-603. Cowan (Eds.), Synaptic function (pp. 637-697).New York: Wiley. Deadwyler, S. A., & Hampson, R. E. (1995). Ensemble activity and Koechlin, E.,Anton, J. L., & Burnod, Y. (1999). Bayesian inference behavior: What’s the code? Science, 270, 1316-1318. in populationsof cortical neurons: A model of motion integration and Ditzinger, T., & Haken, H. (1995). A synergetic model of multi- in area MT. Biological Cybernetics, 80, 25-44. stability in perception. In P. Kruse & M. Stadler (Eds.), Ambiguity in Köhler, W. (1938). Physical Gestalten. Reprinted in W. D. Ellis (Ed.), mind and nature (pp. 255-274).Berlin: Springer-Verlag. A source book of Gestalt psychology (pp. 17-54). London: Routledge Francis, G., & Grossberg, S. (1996). Cortical dynamics of form and & Kegan. (Original work published 1920) motion integration: Persistence, apparent motion, and illusory con- Köhler, W., & Wallach, H. (1944). Figural after-effects: An investi- tours. Vision Research, 36, 149-173. gation of visual processes. Proceedings of the American Philosophi- Giaschi, S., Douglas, R. M., Marlin, S. G., & Cynander, M. S. cal Society, 88, 269-357. (1993).The time course of direction-specific adaptation in simple and Kolers, P.A. (1972). Aspects of motion perception. Oxford: Pergamon. complex cells in cat striate cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 70, Kruse, P., Stadler, M., & Wehner,T. (1986).Direction and frequency 2024-2034. specific processing in the perception of long-range apparent move- Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems.New ment. Vision Research, 26, 327-335. York: Houghton Mifflin. LeVay, S. (1988). The patchy intrinsic projections of visual cortex. Giese,M. A. (1999). Dynamic neural field theory of motion perception. Progress in Brain Research, 75, 147-161. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Levinson, E., & Sekuler, R. (1975).The independence of channels in Gilbert, C. D., & Wiesel, T. N. (1990). The influence of contextual human vision selective for direction of movement. Journal of Physi- stimuli on the orientation selectivity of cells in the primary visual cor- ology, 250, 347-366. tex of the cat. Vision Research, 30, 1689-1701. Marshak, W., & Sekuler, R. (1979). Mutual repulsion between mov- Gilroy, L. A.,Hock, H. S., & Ploeger,A. (2001). Differential activa- ing visual targets. Science, 205, 1399-1401. tion solution to the motion correspondence problem. Perception & Nawrot, M., & Sekuler, R. (1990). Assimilation and contrast in mo- Psychophysics, 63, 847-861. tion perception: Explorations in cooperativity. Vision Research, 30, Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psy- 1439-1451. chophysics. New York: Wiley. Nowlan, S. J., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1995). A selection model for mo- Grossberg, S. (1973). Contour enhancement, short term memory, and tion processing in area (MT) of primates. Journal of Neuroscience, constancies in reverberating neural networks. Studies in Applied 15, 1195-1214. , 52, 213-257. Petersik, J. T. (1990). Global cooperativity of the short-range process Grossberg, S., & Mingolla, E. (1985). Neural dynamics of motion in apparent movement: Evidence obtained with contour-containing perception: Direction fields, apertures, and resonant grouping. Psy- stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 360-368. chological Review, 92, 173-211. Ramachandran,V.S., & Anstis, S. M. (1985).Perceptual organization Grossberg, S., & Rudd, M. E. (1989). A neuronal architecture for vi- in multistable apparent motion. Perception, 14, 135-143. sual motion perception: Group and element apparent motion. Neural Schiller, P.H. (1996). On the specificity of neurons and visual areas. Networks, 2, 421-450. Behavioural Brain Research, 76, 21-35. DYNAMICS OF MOTION PATTERN FORMATION 455

Schiller,P.von (1933). StroboskopischeAlternativversuche. Psycholo- tation with motion trapezoid configurationsthat were either horizontally gische Forschung, 17, 179-214. oriented (so that oblique and horizontal motion competed) or vertically Schöner, G., & Hock, H. S. (1995). Concepts for a dynamical theory oriented (so that oblique and vertical motion competed). Adaptation to of perceptual organization: An example from apparent movement. In the perceived horizontalmotion was indicated by the perception of more P. Kruse & M. Stadler (Eds.), Ambiguity in mind and nature (pp. 275- oblique motion for the horizontally oriented trapezoid for the larger 310). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. adapting aspect ratio. Adaptation to the unperceived vertical motion was Shechter, S., Hochstein, S., & Hillman, P. (1988). Shape similarity indicated by the perception of more obliquemotion for the vertically ori- and distance disparity as apparent motion correspondence cues. Vi- ented trapezoid for the smaller adapting aspect ratio. sion Research, 29, 1013-1021. 2. Hock, Schöner, and Voss (1997) varied the duration of trials for a Snowden, R. J. (1989). Motions in orthogonal directions are mutually motion quartet with an aspect ratio of one and found that when horizon- suppressive. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 6, 1096- tal motion was the initial percept, the probability of at least one switch 1101. per trial increased with trial duration. This increase followed a temporal Softky, W. R., & Koch, C. (1993).The highly irregular firing of corti- independence function calculated on the basis of the probability of a cal cells is inconsistent with temporal integration of random ESPSs. switch during a pair of frames remaining constant over the course of Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 334-350. every trial (differential adaptation would have increased this probability Spitz, S., & Lipman, L. (1962). Some factors affecting Necker cube re- as a function of trial duration). versal rate. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 15, 611-625. 3. This article focuses on the activation of individualdetectors that re- Tanaka, K., Fukada, Y., & Saito, H.-A. (1989). Underlying mecha- spond selectively to the attributes of visual stimuli. However, when even nisms of the response specificity of expansion/contraction and rota- a small local stimulus is presented, the overlap of receptive fields for dif- tion cells in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the ferent detecting units results in a finite number being activated simulta- macaque monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 62, 642-656. neously, with different local ensembles (first called cell assemblies by Thiele, A., Dobkins, K. R., & Albright, T. D. (2000). Neural corre- Hebb, 1949) being activated to isolate the detectors’ response to indi- lates of contrast detection at threshold. Neuron, 26, 715-724. vidual stimulus dimensions (e.g., Deadwyler & Hampson, 1995; P. H. Ts’o, D. Y., Gilbert, C. D., & Wiesel, T.N. (1986). Relationships be- Schiller, 1996). The local ensemble would presumably “inherit’’ the tween horizontal interactions and functional architecture in cat striate noise-stabilizing mechanisms intrinsic to each of its members. For the cortex as revealed by cross-correlation analysis. Journal of Neuro- purposes of this article, the terms detector and detector ensemble are in- science, 6, 1160-1170. terchangeable. Ullman, S. (1979). The interpretation of visual motion. Cambridge, 4. Evidence for inhibitory competition between motions in orthogo- MA: MIT Press. nal as well as opposite directions has been reported by many investiga- Wallach,H. (1976).The direction of motion of straight lines. In H. Wal- tors (e.g., Levinson & Sekuler, 1975; Snowden, 1989; Thiele, Dobkins, lach (Ed.), On perception (pp. 201-216). New York: New York Times & Albright, 2000), so they are a necessary part of the dynamical repre- Book Co., Quadrangle Books. sentation. Evidence for excitatory interactions among motion detectors Williams, D., Phillips, G., & Sekuler, R. (1986). Hysteresis in the with the same directional selectivity comes from studies with random perception of motion direction as evidence for neural cooperativity. cinematograms (e.g., Chang & Julesz, 1984; Nawrot & Sekuler, 1990; Nature, 324, 253-255. Petersik, 1990; Williams & Sekuler, 1984) and directionally ambiguous Williams, D., & Sekuler, R. (1984). Coherent global motion percepts rows of dots (Hock & Balz, 1994). from stochastic local motions. Vision Research, 24, 55-62. 5. Hock et al. (1993) introduced a modification of the method of limits Wilson, H. R. (1998). Spikes, decisions, and actions: Dynamical foun- that eliminates response persistence as a source of hysteresis effects. Tri- dations of neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press. als with ascending or descending sequences of aspect ratios varied with re- Wilson, H. R., Ferrera, V.P., & Yo, C. (1992). A psychophysically spect to their end-point aspect ratio, and participants indicated without motivated model for two-dimensionalmotion perception. Visual Neuro- speed stress whether there was a switch from an initially established per- science, 9, 79-97. cept at any time during the trial. When switches occurred was determined Wilson, H. R., & Kim, J. (1994). A model for motion coherence and by how deeply trials receiving switch responses penetrated the range of transparency. Visual Neuroscience, 11, 1205-1220. aspect ratios for which perception for the motion quartet is bistable. 6. Gilroy et al.’s (2001) motion triplets were based on the generalized NOTES version of apparent motion (Hock, Kogan, & Espinoza, 1997), for which all element locations are simultaneously visible, although with different 1. Hock et al. (1996) adapted subjects to motion quartets for which luminance values. As is illustrated in Figure 22A, motion is perceived horizontal motion was perceived when the vertical motion path was rel- when the luminance values are exchanged during successive frames. Be- atively short (aspect ratio 5 1.25) and when the vertical motion path was cause an element always was present at each corner of the triplet, the relatively long (aspect ratio 5 2.25). They tested for the effects of adap- constraint of the cover principle did not apply.

(Continued on next page) 456 HOCK, SCHÖNER, AND GIESE

APPENDIX The dynamicalmodel of motion perceptionfor themotion quartet(Figure 1B) invokesthe eight possiblemo- 5 tions illustratedin Figure 13A, each representedby an activationvariable,ui (i Br, Bl, Tr, Tl, Ru, Rd, Lu, Ld). 2 The dynamics of each activation variable sums the contributionof the intrinsic stabilizationmechanism ( ui ), j additive inputs [resting level, h, and stimulation,Si (t)], Gaussianwhite noise, i (t), as well as interactionsfrom the seven other activation variables. Interactionsare mediated by the sigmoidal nonlinearity s (u) = 1 . 1exp+-( bu)

The interactionstrengths,wf , depend on the angle, f,between the motion directions. The complete set of dynamical equations is as follows. t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uBr uBr h SBr(t) q Br(t) 1 s 2 s 2 s w0 (uTr) w180 (uTl) w180 (uBl) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w90 (uLu) w90 (uLd) w90 (uRd) w90 (uRu). t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uBl uBl h SBl(t) q Bl(t) 1 s 2 s 2 s w0 (uTl) w180 (uTr) w180 (uBr) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w90 (uRu) w90 (uRd) w90 (uLd) w90 (uLu). t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uTr uTr h STr(t) q Tr(t) 1 s 2 s 2 s w0 (uBr) w180 (uBl) w180 (uTl) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w90 (uLd) w90 (uLu) w90 (uRu) w90 (uRd). t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uTl uTl h STl(t) q Tl(t) 1 s 2 s 2 s w0 (uBl) w180 (uBr) w180 (uTr) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w90 (uRd) w90 (uRu) w90 (uLu) w90 (uLd). t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uRu uRu h SRu(t) q Ru(t) 1 s 2 s 2 s w0 (uLu) w180 (uLd) w180 (uRd) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w90 (uBl) w90 (uBr) w90 (uTr) w90 (uTl). t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uRd uRd h SRd(t) q Rd(t) 1 s 2 s 2 s w0 (uLd) w180 (uLu) w180 (uRu) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w90 (uTl) w90 (uTr) w90 (uBr) w90 (uBl). t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uLu uLu h SLu(t) q Lu(t) 1 s 2 s 2 s w0 (uRu) w180 (uRd) w180 (uLd) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w90 (uBr) w90 (uBl) w90 (uTl) w90 (uTr). t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uLd uLd h SLd(t) q Ld(t) 1 s 2 s 2 s w0 (uRd) w180 (uRu) w180 (uLu) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w90 (uTr) w90 (uTl) w90 (uBl) w90 (uBr).

Input functions,Si(t), representthe time coursesof signalsfrom motion detectors,approximatedhere as square 5 that alternateon and off every 200 msec, as shown in the simulationfigures: Si (t) S0 following a frame 5 change that stimulates motion i, and Si (t) 0 following a frame change that does not stimulate motion i. t 5 52 b 5 5 5 5 All simulationsare based on one set of parameters: 20 msec, h 5, 1, w90 7, w180 w0 3.5, 5 5 q 0.7, and S0 16, with the following exceptions.For Simulation 2, noise strength, q, was reduced to 0.07 5 (part A) and 0 (parts B and C). Stimulus strength was increased to S0 24 for all horizontal and vertical di- rectionsin partC of Simulation2. For Simulation 3, stimulus strength,S0 , was 16.5for all horizontaldirections. For Simulation 6, S0 for vertical motion varied from 12 to 20 for descending trials and from 20 to 12 for as- cending trials. For Simulation 7, S0 varied for vertical motion from 12 to 20 for descending trials. In Simula- tions 4, 5, and 7, interactionstrengths were selectivelyset to zero, as is described in the text. The motion triplet (Figure 22A) was simulated on the basis of the same equations (Simulation 9). Because the activationvariablesuTr, uTl, uRu ,anduRd were never stimulated (correspondinginputs equal zero), they did not contribute to the dynamic interactionsand were omitted from the simulations. The two stimulus strengths for vertical and horizontal were 14 and 16, respectively. DYNAMICS OF MOTION PATTERN FORMATION 457

APPENDIX (Continued) The translating rod stimulus is modeled with the same dynamical representationbut involves a different set of possible motions, as is illustrated in Figure 14B. The analogous set of equations is as follows. t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uBr uBr h SBr(t) q Br(t) 1 s 2 s 2 s w0 (uTr) w180 (uTl) w180 (uBl) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w45 (uDur) w45 (uDdl) w45 (uDul) w45 (uDdr). t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uBl uBl h SBl(t) q Bl(t) 1 s 2 s 2 s w0 (uTl) w180 (uTr) w180 (uBr) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w45 (uDur) w45 (uDdl) w45 (uDul) w45 (uDdr). t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uTr uTr h STr(t) q Tr(t) 1 s 2 s 2 s w0 (uBr) w180 (uBl) w180 (uTl) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w45 (uDur) w45 (uDdl) w45 (uDul) w45 (uDdr). t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uTl uTl h STl(t) q Tl(t) 1 s 2 s 2 s w0 (uBl) w180 (uBr) w180 (uTr) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w45 (uDur) w45 (uDdl) w45 (uDul) w45 (uDdr). t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uDur uDur h SDur(t) q Dur(t) 2 s 2 s 2 s w180 (uDdl) w90 (uDul) w90 (uDdr) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w45 (uBl) w45 (uBr) w45 (uTr) w45 (uTl). t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uDdl uDdl h SDdl(t) q Ddl(t) 2 s 2 s 2 s w180 (uDur) w90 (uDul) w90 (uDdr) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w45 (uTl) w45 (uTr) w45 (uBr) w45 (uBl). t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uDul uDul h SDul(t) q Dul(t) 2 s 2 s 2 s w180 (uDdr) w0 (uDur) w90 (uDdl) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w45 (uBr) w45 (uBl) w45 (uTl) w45 (uTr). t Ç 52 1 1 1 j uDdr uDdr h SDdr(t) q Ddr(t) 2 s 2 s 2 s w180 (uDul) w90 (uDur) w90 (uDdl) 2 s 2 s 2 s 2 s w45 (uTr) w45 (uTl) w45 (uBl) w45 (uBr). 5 The strength of the new interactionwas w45 9. For Simulation 10, diagonalmotion input strength was 14, and for Simulation 11, diagonalmotion input strengthwas 20. All other parameter values were identicalto the motion quartet. Adaptation dynamics were introducedfor Simulation 8. For each activation variable, ui , an adaptation vari- t 5 able, vi , evolves with a time constant, v 1,000 msec according to t 52 1 1D s v vi vi h v h (ui). 52 In the absence of positive activation, the adaptation variable relaxes to the resting level, h v 5. For strong 1D 5 positive activation,the maximum level of adaptationis h v h 5. The adaptationvariables,in turn, act as in- hibitory input to the dynamics of each activation variable, but through a sigmoidal nonlinearity,only positive levels of adaptation are effective: t Ç 5 ¼2 s ui cv (vi). 5 The adaptive strength was cv 5.

(Manuscript received May 23, 2001; revision accepted for publication September 19, 2002.)