Basic Dimensions Defining Mania Risk: a Structural Approach

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Basic Dimensions Defining Mania Risk: a Structural Approach Psychological Assessment © 2016 American Psychological Association 2017, Vol. 29, No. 3, 304–319 1040-3590/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pas0000337 Basic Dimensions Defining Mania Risk: A Structural Approach Kasey Stanton June Gruber University of Notre Dame University of Colorado Boulder David Watson University of Notre Dame Mania is the core criterion for bipolar disorder, a chronic and severe psychiatric illness centrally associated with positive affective disturbance. Many self-report measures have been created to assess symptoms of, and risk for, mania but there are notable disparities in their length, scope, and content. Thus, the goal of this study was to determine the structure and correlates of a number of widely used “bipolar-relevant” (BR) measures (e.g., Hypomanic Personality Scale, Altman Self-Rating Mania Scale, General Behavior Inventory, Mood Disorder Questionnaire). Data from a community sample (Study 1, N ϭ 329) and a student sample assessed at two time points (Study 2; Ns ϭ 382 and 308, respectively) provided strong evidence that the BR measures were characterized by both (a) a well-defined common dimension when a single factor was extracted, and (b) a clear structure of Emotional Lability and Activated Positive Affect upon extracting two factors. The general factor showed a relatively nonspecific pattern of associations with personality and psychopathology. In contrast, the Emotional Lability factor showed its strongest relations with neuroticism and depressive symptoms, displaying comparatively weaker relations with measures of extraversion and positive emotionality. Conversely, although Acti- vated Positive Affect also associated positively with depressive symptoms and with neuroticism in some instances, its strongest relations were with measures of extraversion and high arousal positive emotion- ality. These findings suggest that measures defining Emotional Lability seem to assess mood volatility to a greater extent, whereas measures defining the Activated Positive Affect factor capture an intense, high arousal form of positive emotionality. Keywords: bipolar disorder, mania, positive affect, negative affect, five-factor model of personality The study of mood and affective experiences is broadly defined is known about how the positive affective system—which is using the dimensions of positive affect and negative affect (Wat- closely related to reward seeking and sensitivity (DeYoung, 2013; son, Wiese, Vaidya, & Tellegen, 1999). Positive affect is a term Quilty, DeYoung, Oakman, & Bagby, 2014)—may be a source of used to represent individual differences in positive feeling states dysfunction (Gruber, Dutra, Hay, & Devlin, 2014; Gruber, Kogan, such as joyfulness, attentiveness, and self-assurance, whereas neg- Quoidbach, & Mauss, 2013). Exploring positive affect’s relations ative affect is used to describe the tendency to experience negative with psychopathology is of interest not only because these asso- feelings of sadness, anxiety, guilt, and anger (Shiota, Keltner, & ciations have been understudied, but also because evidence sug- John, 2006; Watson, 2000). There is growing interest in under- gests that positive affect associates negatively with some forms of standing affective disturbance across a range of psychological psychopathology (e.g., depression, schizotypy) but positively with disorders (e.g., Dillon, Deveney, & Pizzagalli, 2011; Jazaieri, others (e.g., bipolar disorder, substance use); in contrast, negative Urry, & Gross, 2013). Although the role of negative affective affect demonstrates ubiquitous positive relations with psychopa- dysregulation in psychopathology is fairly well characterized, less thology (Gruber, 2011a; Gruber, Johnson, Oveis, & Keltner, 2008; This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. Watson & Naragon-Gainey, 2010). This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. In particular, elevated levels of positive affect appear to be a This article was published Online First June 2, 2016. particularly salient feature of bipolar disorder (BD; Gruber, Kasey Stanton, Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame; 2011a), as individuals with BD show greater positive emotional June Gruber, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of reactivity across a wide range of situations, suggesting that in- Colorado Boulder; David Watson, Department of Psychology, University creased positive affect is a trait-like feature of BD that is invariant of Notre Dame. across contexts (du Pont, Welker, Gilbert, & Gruber, in press; Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included Gruber et al., 2014). Moreover, individuals with BD struggle to in the study. The above authors declare that they have no conflict of interest downregulate positive emotion intensity and engage in emotion (financial or non-financial), and APA ethical standards were followed in regulation strategies that prolong and intensify problematic posi- the conduct of this study. All of the reported research was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Notre Dame. tive mood states (Gruber, 2011a, 2011b; Gruber et al., 2013, 2014; Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kasey Watson & Naragon-Gainey, 2010). Despite this compelling evi- Stanton, Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, 118 Hag- dence that positive emotionality is strongly implicated in BD, gar Hall, Notre Dame, IN 46556. E-mail: [email protected] important gaps in assessing positive affective disturbance in BD 304 DIMENSIONS DEFINING MANIA RISK 305 remain, as measures of BD traits and symptoms include content and the Social Vitality scale associate positively with extraversion assessing positive emotional dysregulation to widely varying de- (Stanton et al., 2016; Watson & Naragon-Gainey, 2014). In con- grees (Gruber, 2011b; Gruber et al., 2008). trast, the HPS Mood Volatility scale correlates weakly with extra- version but demonstrates strong relations with neuroticism. The Present Investigation: The Temperament Evaluation of the Memphis, Pisa, Paris, and Assessment Issues in BD Research San Diego Autoquestionnaire (TEMPS-A; 50 items; Akiskal, Ak- iskal, Haykal, Manning, & Connor, 2005) is similar to the HPS in There are notable disparities in how BD and bipolar-relevant that it assesses characteristics conferring risk for BD. More spe- (BR) traits are assessed via self-report measures. We focus on cifically, the TEMPS-A—which includes content tapping emo- self-report measures here given that (a) BD symptoms and affect tional lability, elevated positive emotionality, and dysthymia— are frequently assessed using this method, (b) BD measures vary assesses temperamental styles associated with increased risk for widely in their length, format, and content, and (c) self-report disorders characterized by mood lability, including BD (Akiskal et provides a valid format of assessment for ratings of affect and of al., 2005). traits and symptoms related to BD (Miller, Johnson, & Eisner, The expanded version of the Inventory of Depression and Anx- 2009; Pendergast et al., 2015; Watson, 2000). As noted earlier, iety Symptoms (IDAS-II; Watson et al., 2012) is a recently devel- although affective dysfunction and BD are closely intertwined, oped instrument that contains content related to BD. More specif- widely used measures of BD and mania incorporate assessment of ically, the IDAS-II includes a Euphoria (five items; e.g., “felt positive and negative affective dysregulation to varying degrees: elated for no reason”) scale tapping elevated mood and a Mania Some include little measurement of mood, some focus solely on scale (five items; e.g., “thoughts jumped rapidly”) assessing racing assessing either positive or negative affect, and others assess both thoughts characteristic of hypomanic and manic episodes. Al- types of affect. though IDAS-II Euphoria and Mania tap distinct sets of content, For instance, the five items in the Altman Self-Rating Mania they are substantially intercorrelated and both show strong con- Scale (ASRM; Altman, Hedeker, Peterson, & Davis, 1997) pri- vergence with interview ratings of mania (Watson et al., 2012). marily seem to assess high arousal positive affect (e.g., activity In addition to these measures, several widely used omnibus level, happiness). The Hypomania Checklist-32 (HCL-32; Angst et personality instruments such as the Personality Assessment Inven- al., 2005) instructs respondents to describe how they felt during “a tory (PAI; Morey, 2007) and the Minnesota Multiphasic Person- period when you were in a ‘high’ state,” and a significant portion ality Inventory-2 (MMPI-2; Butcher et al., 2001) also include of its item content also assesses elevated positive affect (e.g., “feel scales assessing BR traits and symptoms. The PAI includes a more energetic”); however, unlike the ASRM, the HCL-32 also Mania scale assessing increased levels of energy and excitement, includes content assessing risk-taking and irritability. Similar to whereas the MMPI-2 includes a Hypomania scale tapping content the HCL-32, the Mood Disorder Questionnaire (MDQ; 13 items; related to elated mood, irritability, and racing thoughts. In addi- Hirschfeld et al., 2000) appears to include content measuring tion, the MMPI-2–Restructured Form (MMPI-2–RF; Ben-Porath euphoric mood, risk taking, and irritability. & Tellegen, 2008) includes both a Hypomanic Activation
Recommended publications
  • (HCL-32) Compared to the Mood Disorder Questionnaire
    brief report Reliability and validity of a Brazilian version of the Hypomania Checklist (HCL-32) compared to the Mood Disorder Questionnaire (MDQ) Confiabilidade e validação da versão brasileira do Questionário de Hipomania (HCL-32 VB) comparado ao Questionário de Transtornos de Humor (MDQ) Odeilton Tadeu Soares,1 Doris Hupfeld Moreno,1 Eduardo Calmon de Moura,1 Jules Angst,2 Ricardo Alberto Moreno1 1 Mood Disorders Unit (GRUDA), Department and Institute of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Universidade de São Paulo (USP), São Paulo, SP, Brazil 2 Zurich University Psychiatric Hospital, Zurich Abstract Resumo Objective: Bipolar disorders are often not recognized and undertreated. Objetivo: O transtorno bipolar muitas vezes não é reconhecido e deixa de ser The diagnosis of current or past episodes of hypomania is of importance tratado adequadamente. O diagnóstico de episódios atuais ou passados é importante, in order to increase diagnostic certainty. The Hypomania Checklist-32 a fim de aumentar a certeza diagnóstica. O Questionário de Autoavaliação is a self-applied questionnaire aimed at recognizing these episodes. As de Hipomania-32 é um questionário autoaplicável para o rastreamento desses part of the international collaborative effort to develop multi-lingual episódios. Como parte do desenvolvimento em vários idiomas do Questionário de versions of the Hypomania Checklist-32, we aimed to validate the Autoavaliação de Hipomania-32, nós objetivamos validar a versão brasileira e Brazilian version and to compare its psychometric properties
    [Show full text]
  • Applying a Discrete Emotion Perspective
    AROUSAL OR RELEVANCE? APPLYING A DISCRETE EMOTION PERSPECTIVE TO AGING AND AFFECT REGULATION SARA E. LAUTZENHISER Bachelor of Science in Psychology Ashland University May 2015 Submitted in partial fulfillment of requirements for the degree MASTER OF ARTS IN PSYCHOLOGY At the CLEVELAND STATE UNIVERSITY May 2019 We hereby approve this thesis For SARA E. LAUTZENHISER Candidate for the Master of Arts in Experimental Research Psychology For the Department of Psychology And CLEVELAND STATE UNIVERSITY’S College of Graduate Studies by __________________________ Eric Allard, Ph.D. __________________________ Department & Date __________________________ Andrew Slifkin, Ph. D. (Methodologist) __________________________ Department & Date __________________________ Conor McLennan, Ph.D. __________________________ Department & Date __________________________ Robert Hurley, Ph. D. __________________________ Department & Date Student’s Date of Defense May 10, 2019 AROUSAL OR RELEVANCE? APPLYING A DISCRETE EMOTION PERSPECTIE TO AGING AND AFFECT REGULATION SARA E. LAUTZENHISER ABSTRACT While research in the psychology of human aging suggests that older adults are quite adept at managing negative affect, emotion regulation efficacy may depend on the discrete emotion elicited. For instance, prior research suggests older adults are more effective at dealing with emotional states that are more age-relevant/useful and lower in intensity (i.e., sadness) relative to less relevant/useful or more intense (i.e., anger). The goal of the present study was to probe this discrete emotions perspective further by addressing the relevance/intensity distinction within a broader set of negative affective states (i.e., fear and disgust, along with anger and sadness). Results revealed that participants reported relatively high levels of the intended emotion for each video, while also demonstrating significant affective recovery after the attentional refocusing task.
    [Show full text]
  • Perceived Social Rank, Social Expectation, Shame and General Emotionality Within Psychopathy
    Perceived social rank, social expectation, shame and general emotionality within psychopathy Sarah Keen D. Clin.Psy. Thesis (Volume 1), 2008 University College London UMI Number: U591545 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Dissertation Publishing UMI U591545 Published by ProQuest LLC 2013. Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author. Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code. ProQuest LLC 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 Overview Within the psychological literature, the self-conscious emotion of shame is proving to be an area of growing interest. This thesis addresses the application of this emotion, as well as self and social evaluative processes, to our understanding of offenders, specifically those high in psychopathic traits. Part 1 reviews the literature concerning emotionality within psychopathy, in order to assess the capabilities, as well as the deficits that people with psychopathic traits demonstrate. Emotions classified as ‘moral’ or ‘self-conscious’, namely empathy, sympathy, guilt, remorse, shame, embarrassment and pride, are investigated. From the review it is clear that psychopaths are not the truly unemotional individuals that they are commonly portrayed as being, but instead experience many emotions to varying degrees. This paper concludes by highlighting possible areas for further exploration and research.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effect of Opioids on Emotional Reactivity
    The Effect of Opioids on Emotional Reactivity Steven M. Savvas, BHSc (Hons) Discipline of Pharmacology, School of Medical Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences University of Adelaide August, 2013 A thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy i Steven M. Savvas, PhD Thesis, 2013 TABLE OF CONTENTS Abstract .................................................................................................................................... xi Declaration ............................................................................................................................ xiii Acknowledgements ............................................................................................................... xiv CHAPTER 1 - INTRODUCTION ...................................................................................... 1 1.1 OPIOIDS AND OPIOID MAINTENANCE TREATMENT ...................................... 1 1.1.1 A BRIEF HISTORY OF OPIOIDS .......................................................................... 1 1.1.2 OPIOID RECEPTORS ............................................................................................ 1 1.1.3 ADAPTATION TO OPIOIDS.................................................................................. 3 1.1.3.1 Tolerance ........................................................................................................ 4 1.1.3.2 Withdrawal ...................................................................................................... 4 1.1.3.3 Dependence
    [Show full text]
  • Systematic Review of Symptom Assessment Measures for Use in Measurement-Based Care of Bipolar Disorders
    REVIEWS AND OVERVIEWS Systematic Review of Symptom Assessment Measures for Use in Measurement-Based Care of Bipolar Disorders Joseph M. Cerimele, M.D., M.P.H., Simon B. Goldberg, Ph.D., Christopher J. Miller, Ph.D., Stephen W. Gabrielson, M.S.L.I.S., John C. Fortney, Ph.D. Objective: Utilization of measurement-based care (MBC) for depressive symptoms, and six patient-reported and four bipolar disorders is limited, in part because of uncertainty clinician-observed measures of both symptom types. regarding the utility of available measures. The aim of this Patient-reported measures with the highest clinical utility study was to synthesize the literature on patient-reported included the Altman Self-Rating Mania Scale for assessment and clinician-observed measures of symptoms of bipolar of manic symptoms, the Quick Inventory of Depressive disorder and the potential use of these measures in MBC. Symptomatology–Self Report (QIDS-SR) (depressive symp- toms), and the Internal State Scale (both types). Highly rated Methods: A systematic review of multiple databases clinician (C)-observed scales were the Bech-Rafaelsen Ma- (PubMed, Embase, PsycINFO, Cochrane Library, and other nia Rating Scale (mania), the QIDS-C (depressive symptoms), gray literature) was conducted in June 2017 to identify val- and the Bipolar Inventory of Symptoms Scale (both types). idated measures. Data on the psychometric properties of each measure were extracted and used to assess the mea- Conclusions: Suitable choices are available for MBC of bi- sure’s clinical utility on the basis of established guidelines. polar disorders. The choice of a measure could be informed by clinical utility score and may also depend on how clini- Results: Twenty-eight unique measures were identified in cians or practices weigh each category of the clinical utility 39 studies, including four patient-reported and six clinician- scale and on the clinical setting and presenting problem.
    [Show full text]
  • Attitudes Toward Emotions
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology © 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 101, No. 6, 1332–1350 0022-3514/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0024951 Attitudes Toward Emotions Eddie Harmon-Jones and Cindy Harmon-Jones David M. Amodio Texas A&M University New York University Philip A. Gable University of Alabama The present work outlines a theory of attitudes toward emotions, provides a measure of attitudes toward emotions, and then tests several predictions concerning relationships between attitudes toward specific emotions and emotional situation selection, emotional traits, emotional reactivity, and emotion regula- tion. The present conceptualization of individual differences in attitudes toward emotions focuses on specific emotions and presents data indicating that 5 emotions (anger, sadness, joy, fear, and disgust) load on 5 separate attitude factors (Study 1). Attitudes toward emotions predicted emotional situation selection (Study 2). Moreover, attitudes toward approach emotions (e.g., anger, joy) correlated directly with the associated trait emotions, whereas attitudes toward withdrawal emotions (fear, disgust) correlated inversely with associated trait emotions (Study 3). Similar results occurred when attitudes toward emotions were used to predict state emotional reactivity (Study 4). Finally, attitudes toward emotions predicted specific forms of emotion regulation (Study 5). Keywords: discrete emotions, approach motivation, withdrawal motivation, emotion regulation, behav- ioral activation system (BAS) Emotions pervade subjective experience (Izard, 2009), and al- theory by assessing relationships between attitudes toward specific though often perceived as a single subjective state, emotional emotions and emotional traits, emotional reactivity, emotional experience is likely composed of many different elements. Tom- situation selection, and emotion regulation. kins (1962, 1963) and others (Ellsworth, 1994; Izard, 1971) sug- gested that the evaluation of an emotion is part of the experience of emotion.
    [Show full text]
  • The Reliability of Subjective Well-Being Measures by Alan B
    The Reliability of Subjective Well-Being Measures by Alan B. Krueger, Princeton University David A. Schkade, University of California, San Diego CEPS Working Paper No. 138 January 2007 The authors thank our colleagues Daniel Kahneman, Norbert Schwarz, and Arthur Stone for helpful comments and the Hewlett Foundation, the National Institute on Aging, and Princeton University’s Woodrow Wilson School and Center for Economic Policy Studies for financial support. Reliability of SWB Measures – 2 Introduction Economists are increasingly analyzing data on subjective well-being. Since 2000, 157 papers and numerous books have been published in the economics literature using data on life satisfaction or subjective well-being, according to a search of Econ Lit.1 Here we analyze the test-retest reliability of two measures of subjective well-being: a standard life satisfaction question and affective experience measures derived from the Day Reconstruction Method (DRM). Although economists have longstanding reservations about the feasibility of interpersonal comparisons of utility that we can only partially address here, another question concerns the reliability of such measurements for the same set of individuals over time. Overall life satisfaction should not change very much from week to week. Likewise, individuals who have similar routines from week to week should experience similar feelings over time. How persistent are individuals’ responses to subjective well-being questions? To anticipate our main findings, both measures of subjective well-being (life satisfaction and affective experience) display a serial correlation of about 0.60 when assessed two weeks apart, which is lower than the reliability ratios typically found for education, income and many other common micro economic variables (Bound, Brown, and Mathiowetz, 2001 and Angrist and Krueger, 1999), but high enough to support much of the research that has been undertaken on subjective well-being.
    [Show full text]
  • The Lived Experience of Being Born Into Grief
    The lived experience of being born into grief Michelle Holt A thesis submitted to Auckland University of Technology in partial fulfilment of the requirements of the degree of Doctor of Health Science (DHSc) 2018 School of Public Health and Psychosocial Studies Faculty of Health and Environmental Sciences Primary Supervisor: Dr Jacqueline Feather Abstract This study explores the meaning of the lived experience of being born into grief. Using a phenomenological hermeneutic methodology, informed by the writings of Martin Heidegger [1889-1976] and Hans-George Gadamer [1900-2002], this research provides an understanding of the lived experience of having been a baby when one or both parents were grieving (born into grief). The review of the literature identified physical effects of being born when a mother was stressed but no literature was found which discussed emotional effects that a baby may incur due to stress or grief of a parent. The notion of grief was explored and literature pertaining to early childhood adversity reviewed as a possible resource for bringing light to how it may be for babies born into grief. The literature indicated that possible long term complications such as rebellious behaviour, poor relationships, poor mental and physical health, could be a result of early adversity. The literature on understanding effects of grief from a conceptual perspective, rather than from the lived experience perspective, provided a platform for this study. In this study nine New Zealand participants told their stories about the grief situation they were born into and how they thought it had affected them. Data were gathered in the form of semi structured interviews which were audio recorded and transcribed verbatim.
    [Show full text]
  • Distinguishing Neurotic from Prosocial "Guilt": Evidence for the Conceptual
    Distinguishing Neurotic from Prosocial “Guilt”: Evidence for the Conceptual Distinctiveness of Checklist and Scenario Measures by Stefanie M. Tignor B.A. in Psychology, Binghamton University M.A. in Psychology, Northeastern University A dissertation submitted to The Faculty of the College of Science of Northeastern University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy July 28, 2016 Dissertation directed by C. Randall Colvin Associate Professor of Psychology ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS First, thank you to my advisor, Randy Colvin, for your ever-impressive expertise, guidance, and support. From my first day to my last you’ve made the Personality Lab feel like home, and I deeply appreciate all that you have taught me. Thanks for always being willing to chat about research or life, and thanks for always cheering me on. To Judy Hall, thank you for all of your support and mentorship over these past few years. Your immense knowledge of psychology and skills as a researcher are an inspiration to me. And of course, thank you for helping me fall in love with meta-analysis. To Nancy Kim, thank you for your insightful and helpful comments on this dissertation, in meetings and via email. It has been much improved thanks to your guidance, and I have greatly appreciated your help throughout the process. Thanks to the many research assistants who helped with this project: Emily Burke, David Kramer, Megan Pinaire, Christina Tebbe, Ronnie Lo, Simone Grant, Alexa Lambros, Fae Kayarian, Kristen Laws, Catherine Martin, Heather Offermann, and Sangjukta Sen Roy. Thank you to my research “brother” and “sister” Sun Park and Krista Hill; your welcoming nature and willingness to help a lost first-year will never be forgotten.
    [Show full text]
  • Reduced Emotional Empathy in Adults with Subclinical ADHD Groen, Y.; Den Heijer, A.E.; Fuermaier, A.B.M.; Althaus, M.; Tucha, O
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by University of Groningen University of Groningen Reduced emotional empathy in adults with subclinical ADHD Groen, Y.; den Heijer, A.E.; Fuermaier, A.B.M.; Althaus, M.; Tucha, O. Published in: ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders DOI: 10.1007/s12402-017-0236-7 IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below. Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Publication date: 2018 Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database Citation for published version (APA): Groen, Y., den Heijer, A. E., Fuermaier, A. B. M., Althaus, M., & Tucha, O. (2018). Reduced emotional empathy in adults with subclinical ADHD: Evidence from the empathy and systemizing quotient. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 10(2), 141–150. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-017-0236-7 Copyright Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Take-down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.
    [Show full text]
  • Stigma, Well-Being, Attitudes to Service Use and Transition to Schizophrenia: Longitudinal Findings Among Young People at Risk of Psychosis
    Stigma, well-being, attitudes to service use and transition to schizophrenia: Longitudinal findings among young people at risk of psychosis Nicolas Rüsch, Mario Müller, Karsten Heekeren, Ana Theodoridou, Wulf Rössler Dept of Psychiatry II, University of Ulm & BKH Günzburg, Germany Psychiatric University Hospital Zürich, Switzerland Background I: Stigma - an issue for at-risk states and intervention? • Labeling as ‚mentally ill‘ possible due to - emerging symptoms - early intervention • Concerns raised in the literature, cf. discussion about inclusion of at-risk syndrome in DSM-5 (Corcoran et al 2010; Yang et al 2010) • No quantitative data on stigma among people at risk of psychosis (qualitative: Judge et al 2008) Background II: Mechanisms Models to explain how public stigma and self-stigma / shame could negatively affect young people at risk: 1. Modified Labeling Theory (Bruce Link): After being labeled, public attitudes become self-relevant 2. Stress-coping models of stigma (Lazarus; Major & O‘Brien 2005): Stigma stress, if perc‘d harm exceeds perc‘d coping resources. Questions 1. Do labeling and stigma variables predict reduced well-being among people at risk of psychosis after one year? 2. Do stress-coping models explain how stigma affects this group? 3. Does stigma stress predict transition to schizophrenia? Cross-sectional findings Perceived public stigma + Stigma stress Well-being Shame + [perceived harm - [quality of life, about one’s > perceived self-esteem, mental illness coping self-efficacy] resources] + Self-labeling as ‘mentally ill’ Rüsch et al, Psych Serv 2014 Methods I: Participants • N=77, 13 to 35 years old (mean 20), 46% female (45% of baseline sample, n=172) • Recruitment via website, schools, GPs, counseling services, psychiatrists etc.
    [Show full text]
  • Trait Positive and Negative Emotionality Differentially Associate Withdiurnal Cortisol Activity Karissa G
    Bryn Mawr College Scholarship, Research, and Creative Work at Bryn Mawr College Psychology Faculty Research and Scholarship Psychology 2016 Trait positive and negative emotionality differentially associate withdiurnal cortisol activity Karissa G. Miller Aidan G.C. Wright Laurel M. Peterson Bryn Mawr College, [email protected] Thomas W. Kamarck Barbara A. Anderson See next page for additional authors Let us know how access to this document benefits ouy . Follow this and additional works at: http://repository.brynmawr.edu/psych_pubs Part of the Psychology Commons Custom Citation Miller et al. "Trait positive and negative emotionality differentially associate withdiurnal cortisol activity." Psychoneuroendocrinology 68 (2016); 177-185. This paper is posted at Scholarship, Research, and Creative Work at Bryn Mawr College. http://repository.brynmawr.edu/psych_pubs/47 For more information, please contact [email protected]. Authors Karissa G. Miller, Aidan G.C. Wright, Laurel M. Peterson, Thomas W. Kamarck, Barbara A. Anderson, Clemens Kirschbaum, Anna L. Marsland, and Matthew F. Muldoon This article is available at Scholarship, Research, and Creative Work at Bryn Mawr College: http://repository.brynmawr.edu/ psych_pubs/47 Psychoneuroendocrinology 68 (2016) 177–185 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Psychoneuroendocrinology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psyneuen Trait positive and negative emotionality differentially associate with diurnal cortisol activity a,∗ a b a Karissa G. Miller , Aidan G.C. Wright , Laurel
    [Show full text]