Anthropological Contributions to Cognitive Science

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Anthropological Contributions to Cognitive Science Anthropological Contributions to Cognitive Science Organizers Andrea Bender ([email protected] ) & Sieghard Beller ([email protected] ) Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, Norway Presenters Rita Astuti ([email protected] ) Department of Anthropology, London School of Economics (LSE), London, UK Olivier Le Guen ([email protected] ) Centro de Investigaciones y Estudios Superiores en Antropología Social, CIESAS, Mexico Ciudad, Mexico Karenleigh A. Overmann ([email protected] ) Center for Cognitive Archaeology, University of Colorado, Colorado Springs, US Peter Rácz ([email protected] ) & Fiona Jordan ([email protected] ) Department of Archaeology and Anthropology, University of Bristol, UK Mads Solberg ([email protected] ) Department of Social Anthropology, University of Bergen, Norway Keywords: anthropology; cognitive science; philosophy of tems as near-universal conceptual tools. With his back- science; cognition; culture; language; materiality. ground in (ethno-)linguistics and cognitive anthropology, Le Guen uses Yucatec Maya sign languages to illustrate the Anthropology was a founding member of cognitive science importance of cultural practices for shaping cognitive be- (Bender et al., 2010; Gardner, 1985), sharing with other havior. Based on Hutchins’ cognitive ecology approach, cognitive disciplines a deep interest in thinking and behav- Solberg speaks to questions at the intersection of anthropol- ior. With its unique expertise in the cultural content, con- ogy and philosophy of science by illuminating the cultural text, and constitution of cognition, it would still be essential framework of science production in a biology lab. And so- to any comprehensive endeavor to explore the human mind cial anthropologist Astuti concludes by taking a bird’s eye (Bloch, 2012), but rather has turned into cognitive science’s view on how efforts to understand the human mind crucially “missing discipline” (Boden, 2006), thus leaving important benefit from acknowledging its historical origins and from questions unanswered or even unasked. Given that substan- taking the specific sociocultural contexts into consideration. tial shares of knowledge are implicit and that cognition is Based on work some of which is published in high-quality situated, distributed, embodied, and grounded in various journals (such as Science , Nature , PNAS , BBS , TiCS, Cur- other ways, anthropological approaches provide privileged rent Anthropology, or Cognition ), these participants will access to investigation: for arriving at reasonable hypothe- offer invaluable contributions to a more diverse, more inclu- ses, ensuring ecological validity, and even for coming up sive, and hence more comprehensive cognitive science. with new research questions and paradigms (Astuti & Bloch, 2012; Hutchins, 2010; Nersessian, 2006). Archaeology and Cognitive Science In line with recent calls for rapprochement in Topics in Karenleigh A. Overmann Cognitive Science (Bender et al., 2012; Beller & Bender, Archaeology contributes to cognitive science in two key 2015), our symposium brings together scholars that repre- areas. First, in understanding human cognitive evolution, sent different branches of contemporary anthropology with archaeology furnishes critical data on the timing and context distinct perspectives—including ‘traditional’ social anthro- of developments (Wynn, 2002). This approach assumes pology, cognitive anthropology and ethno-linguistics, cogni- minds make tools : increasing complexity in material forms tive ecology, evolutionary anthropology, and archaeology— is an effect of, and thus signals, cognitive change related to to present what they consider to be indispensable contribu- neurological developments like encephalization. Second, tions to cognitive science. archaeology provides unique insight into the ways materiali- With our selection of authors, we hope to demonstrate the ty functions within the extended, enacted mind. This invert- value of anthropological approaches for cognitive science as ed approach—tools make minds (Malafouris, 2013)— well as the potential benefits of cross-disciplinary collabora- examines how material forms interact with body and brain tion. Cognitive archaeologist Overmann discusses a theo- to create meaning and experience and potentialize behavior- retical perspective on how mind, behavior, and material al and psychological change. In both contributions, archae- artifacts interact to shape human cognition. Combining their ology negotiates temporalities, centuries to millennia and expertise in linguistics and evolutionary anthropology, Rácz longer, that can be challenging for psychological theories and Jordan investigate the design principles of kinship sys- and methods to assimilate (e.g., Overmann, 2016). 21 Cognitive and Adaptive-Historical Explanations conceptual, social, and cultural infrastructures of laborato- for Kinship Diversity ries) set up divisions of cognitive labour and distribute cog- nition through time and space in ways that are critical to this Peter Rácz & Fiona Jordan process. This talk looks at how the alliance between anthro- Kinship systems are semantic systems whose forms can be pology, cognitive science, and adjacent fields like philoso- explained in terms of domain-general cognitive principles; phy and history of science, can contribute to further devel- kinship categories are optimised to be maximally distinct oping this research area. Such collaborations are necessary and as simple as possible. Kinship, then, is similar to other for adequately explaining cultural transmission and cultural universal semantic categories such as colour terms. Howev- evolution in scientific knowledge, and for describing inter- er, whereas colour terms broadly fit into one typological actions between mental representation, epistemic action, and hierarchy, kinship systems comprise a diverse typology. material culture in scientific experimentation. I draw on Alternatively, adaptive-historical explanations emphasise examples from a long-term cognitive-ethnographic case- how cultural traditions and social practices (particularly study in a community of molecular life-scientists. marriage and transfer of resources) place functional pres- sures on the shape of kinship systems (Jordan & Dunn, Anthropology as a Critical Friend 2010). Using a global ethnographic database of over a thou- Rita Astuti sand societies we show that marriage rules and ancestry have a significant influence on the type of kinship system Anthropology is commonly listed as one of the disciplines found in a society. This remains true if we control for the that make up cognitive science. But what exactly is the con- effect of lateral transmission and phylogeny. This, in turn, tribution that anthropology can make to the interdisciplinary means that kinship is best approached by combining cogni- study of human cognition? The paper will argue that an- tive and historic-anthropological explanations. These results thropology must take on the role of critical friend, constant- have broader implications for the understanding of lexical ly reminding other disciplines of the historical origins of all systems in particular and the mechanisms of human cogni- human phenomena and of the theoretical and methodologi- tion in general. cal challenges that come from recognising that all aspects of human cognition develop in specific social and cultural con- How Cultural Settings Frame Spatial Cognition: texts. The Example of Yucatec Maya and Yucatec Maya Sign Language References Olivier Le Guen Astuti, R., & Bloch, M. (2012). Anthropologists as cognitive scien- tists. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4, 453-461. On the Yucatec peninsula, the main native language is spo- Beller, S., & Bender, A. (Eds.) (2015). Exploring cognitive diversi- ken Yucatec Maya (YM). However, in villages where deaf ty: Anthropological perspectives on cognition. Topics in Cogni- people are born, a local sign language (YMSL) was created tive Science, 7(4) . both by deaf and their hearing kin. Although both languages Bender, A., Beller, S., & Medin, D. (Eds.) (2012). Does cognitive science need anthropology? Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(3) . are in intense contact, they are genetically different, and Bender, A., Hutchins, E., & Medin, D. L. (2010). Anthropology in YMSL is not a signed version of YM. In Le Guen (2011), I cognitive science. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 374-385. showed how gestures—in addition to linguistic structures Bloch, M. (2012). Anthropology and the cognitive challenge . (Levinson, 2003)—can support a geocentric frame of refer- Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ence. In this paper, I want to elaborate on how deaf signers Boden, M. A. (2006). Mind as Machine . Oxford: Clarendon Press. using YMSL still ‘inherit’ the same conception of space Gardner, H. (1985). The mind’s new science. NY: Basic Books. through cultural practices. Hutchins, E. (2010). Cognitive ecology. Topics in Cognitive Sci- ence, 2, 705-715. Jordan, F. M., & Dunn, M. (2010). Kin term diversity is the result Exploratory Experimentation in Experimental of multilevel, historical processes. Behavioral and Brain Scienc- Systems: Novel Directions for the Cognitive es , 33 , 388. Anthropology of Science Le Guen, O. (2011). Speech and gesture in spatial language and Mads Solberg cognition among the Yucatec Mayas. Cognitive Science, 35, 905-938. It is now widely recognised that progress in many scientific Levinson, S. C. (2003). Space in language and cognition . Cam-
Recommended publications
  • Evolutionary Psychology, Cognitive Archaeology, and Psychological Prehistory Course Description: This Course Is PSY 325, Evoluti
    Evolutionary Psychology, Cognitive Archaeology, and Psychological Prehistory Course Description: This course is PSY 325, Evolutionary Psychology, as offered in the Spring of 2020 Text: Henley, T., Rossano, M., & Kardas, E. (2020). Handbook of Cognitive Archaeology: Psychology in Prehistory. New York: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Note – I know this book is expensive, so there will be a copy placed on reserve in the library that you can use The Basics: Keep in mind that a syllabus sometimes shifts a little as a course unfolds. I say that by way of noting the importance of class attendance, as you will be held responsible for any change in plans announced in class. The other basic admonition every syllabus must include is that cheating (broadly defined) is not allowed. Or, as the university likes me to say: "All students enrolled at the University shall follow the tenets of common decency and acceptable behavior conducive to a positive learning environment (see Student's Guide Handbook, Policies and Procedures, Conduct).” And, note that “Students requesting accommodations for disabilities must go through the Academic Support Committee. For more information, please contact the Director of Disability Resources & Services, Gee Library, Room 132, (903) 886-5835.” Last: Only qualified law enforcement officers or those who are otherwise authorized to carry a concealed handgun in the State of Texas are permitted to do so. Pursuant to Penal Code (PC) 46.035 and A&M-Commerce Rule 34.06.02.R1, even license holders may not carry a concealed handgun in restricted locations. Readings: The readings are listed on the Schedule of Events as follows: key required; required; suggested; and optional.
    [Show full text]
  • Anthropology's Disenchantment with the Cognitive Revolution
    Topics in Cognitive Science 4 (2012) 354–361 Copyright Ó 2012 Cognitive Science Society, Inc. All rights reserved. ISSN: 1756-8757 print / 1756-8765 online DOI: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01199.x Anthropology’s Disenchantment With the Cognitive Revolution1 Richard A. Shweder Department of Comparative Human Development, University of Chicago Received 25 June 2011; received in revised form 3 November 2011; accepted 28 November 2011 Abstract Beller, Bender, and Medin should be congratulated for their generous attempt at expressive aca- demic therapy for troubled interdisciplinary relationships. In this essay, I suggest that a negative answer to the central question (‘‘Should anthropology be part of cognitive science?’’) is not necessar- ily distressing, that in retrospect the breakup seems fairly predictable, and that disenchantment with the cognitive revolution is nothing new. Keywords: Cognitive revolution; Behaviorism; Anthropology; Jerome Bruner; Roy D’Andrade; Clifford Geertz; Roger Shepard Some of the leaders of the cognitive revolution of the late 1950s and 1960s began parting from the cause almost as soon as it triumphed. Jerome Bruner, for example, who always enjoyed writing essays for both the left hand and the right hand, turned to hermeneutics, the study of law, and the interpretation of narratives (see Bruner, 1979, 1990). Even in the early days of the rebellion Bruner was attentive to language pragmatics, which may be one reason he named his 1960s big tent interdisciplinary center at Harvard University the ‘‘Center for Cognitive Studies’’ and not the ‘‘Center for Cognitive Science.’’ Bruner had just as much interest in the humanistic writings of E. H. Gombrich, Nelson Goodman, and Clifford Ge- ertz as in the latest claims about basic ⁄fundamental⁄universal cognitive processes coming out of experimental labs situated in Cambridge, London, or Geneva.
    [Show full text]
  • Epistemological Problems in Cognitive Archaeology: an Anti-Relativistic Proposal Towards Methodological Uniformity
    doi 10.4436/JASS.91003 JASs Invited Reviews e-pub ahead of print Journal of Anthropological Sciences Vol. 92 (2014), pp. 7-41 Epistemological problems in Cognitive Archaeology: an anti-relativistic proposal towards methodological uniformity Duilio Garofoli1 & Miriam Noël Haidle1,2 1) Zentrum für Naturwissenschaftliche Archäologie, Eberhard Karls Universität Tübingen, Rümelinstr. 23, 72070 Tübingen, Germany e-mail: [email protected] 2) Heidelberg Academy of Sciences and Humanities, Research Group “The Role of Culture in Early Expansions of Humans,” Research Institute Senckenberg, Senckenberganlage 25, D-60325 Frankfurt/ Main, Germany Summary - Cognitive archaeology (CA) has an inherent and major problem. The coupling between extinct minds, brains and behaviors cannot be investigated in a laboratory. Without direct testability, there is a risk that theories in CA will remain merely subjective opinions in which “anything goes”. To counter this risk, opponents of relativism originally argued that CA should adopt a method of validation based on “indirectly” testing inferences from the archaeological record. In this paper, we will offer a two-part analysis. In the first part, we will discuss problems with the original anti-relativistic agenda. While we agree with the necessity of developing a rational methodology for this discipline, in our view previous analyses have significant weak points that need to be strengthened. In particular, we will propose that “indirect testability” should be superseded by a methodology based upon deductive mappings from networks of theories, followed by a plausibility-selection stage. This methodology will be implemented by adopting an extension of Barnard´s (2010b) proposals for mapping hierarchical systems. In the second part, we will compare our methods with those currently adopted in the CA debate.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Archaeology
    Introduction to Archaeology: Class 16 Cognitive archaeology Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 − Cognitive archaeology : What people thought in the past, when they thought it, how they came to think it, and how that affected other things − Two broad foci: − Origins and development of modern human thinking abilities − When did people start thinking like we do, why, how, etc. − Content and influence of thought, as opposed to environment, economics, etc. − What religious ideas have people had, why, and how did that affect their lives and developments in their societies? − How did people understand and explain their world, both the physical world and the social (economic, political, etc.) world? − that is, what have people's ideologies been? − In part, this is a reaction to the excesses of the New Archaeology and the processual approach, which tended to emphasize − adaptation to the environment − economic or material determinism − a systemic view of society in which subsystems or subgroups acted in certain ways in response to certain conditions − reacting to this solely materialist focus, the cognitive archaeology approach emphasizes individuals and what they think as being of interest and having a causal role − in order to understand what people were doing in the past, cognitive archaeologists say you have to understand what they were thinking − their cognitive abilities (in the case of very early humans who may or may not have been fully modern in their psychology) − their ideology and cosmological framework for the world − which shapes their understanding
    [Show full text]
  • Toward a Phenomenological Cognitive Archaeology
    PHILIP TONNER The University of Glasgow [email protected] TOWARD A PHENOMENOLOGICAL COGNITIVE ARCHAEOLOGY abstract Archaeologists, neuroscientists and philosophers all aim to shed light on the holistic and co- constitutive role played by bodies and brains, objects and culture over the course of hominin cognitive evolution. Recent advances in neuroscience and brain imaging have enabled exploration of the foundation for tool using capacity in modern human brains. In tandem with this has been the development of cognitive archaeology, a perspective that seeks to uncover and engage with past ways of thought, as these can be inferred from surviving material remains. What I will suggest in this paper is that the phenomenological perspective can contribute to the methodological drive in cognitive archaeology. Phenomenology provides just the kind of access to consciousness and the mind required for an understanding of “ways of thought and action”, including past ways of thought and action, to emerge. I will argue that pragmatic meaning- bestowing agency is operative throughout the Palaeolithic and I will suggest how empirical evidence can be understood in the terms suggested by phenomenological philosophers. keywords Phenomenology; cognitive; archaeology; tools; manufacture; agency TOWARD A PHENOMENOLOGICAL COGNITIVE ARCHAEOLOGY PHILIP TONNER The University of Glasgow 1. Archaeologists, neuroscientists and philosophers all aim to shed light on the holistic and co-constitutive role played by bodies, brains, objects and worlds throughout hominin cognitive evolution. Hominins include modern humans (Homo sapiens sapiens) and all of our fossil ancestors: hominins are hominids, as are the great apes (Coward and Gamble 2009, p. 64). Archaeology can, with an increasing precision, tell us where and when modern humans emerged: Africa between 100.000 and 200.000 years ago.
    [Show full text]
  • Ethnomusicology a Very Short Introduction
    ETHNOMUSICOLOGY A VERY SHORT INTRODUCTION Thimoty Rice Sumário Chapter 1 – Defining ethnomusicology...........................................................................................4 Ethnos..........................................................................................................................................5 Mousikē.......................................................................................................................................5 Logos...........................................................................................................................................7 Chapter 2 A bit of history.................................................................................................................9 Ancient and medieval precursors................................................................................................9 Exploration and enlightenment.................................................................................................10 Nationalism, musical folklore, and ethnology..........................................................................10 Early ethnomusicology.............................................................................................................13 “Mature” ethnomusicology.......................................................................................................15 Chapter 3........................................................................................................................................17 Conducting
    [Show full text]
  • The Dissertation Committee for Richard Neill Hadder Certifies That This Is The
    The Dissertation Committee for Richard Neill Hadder certifies that this is the approved version of the following dissertation: Apparitions of Difference: Essays on the Vocation of Reflexive Anthropology Committee: ______________________________ Kathleen Stewart, Supervisor ______________________________ Richard Flores ______________________________ Ward Keeler ______________________________ Elizabeth Keating ______________________________ James Wilce Apparitions of Difference: Essays on the Vocation of Reflexive Anthropology by Richard Neill Hadder, B.A., M.A.I.S., M.A. Dissertation Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School of The University of Texas at Austin in Partial Fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy. The University of Texas at Austin May 2007 Preface Dear Reader(s), Hi, how are you? It has been suggested to me that a few pages of prefatory remarks of the form “dear reader” would help “prepare the reader to be disoriented” in the essays I’ve written. I’m very happy to do so, insofar as corresponding with you in this way somewhat lessens the natural readerly tendency to demand—intentionally or not—that I correspond “to” you. This distinction might be the most important cord running through the essays, which all in some way discuss coping strategies for how to deal with irrevocable, incommunicable difference. The maps by which we tend to conceptualize “diverse” people in familiar or categorical terms are inadequate. Disorienting? Yes, insofar as the reader holds to a protocol that does want some of its categories to remain unquestionable. Common sense tells us that one has to stand on solid ground to swing a hammer effectively; whereas my project has turned out to have more in common with Derrida’s, in that one can’t write about what underwrites meaning without either violating one’s own thesis or else finding some other technique for swinging the hammer—or wielding the scalpel, or unwinding the threads of a fabrication, or any metaphor you choose, so long as it means fundamental anti-foundationalist critique.
    [Show full text]
  • Archaeology: the Key Concepts Is the Ideal Reference Guide for Students, Teachers and Anyone with an Interest in Archaeology
    ARCHAEOLOGY: THE KEY CONCEPTS This invaluable resource provides an up-to-date and comprehensive survey of key ideas in archaeology and their impact on archaeological thinking and method. Featuring over fifty detailed entries by international experts, the book offers definitions of key terms, explaining their origin and development. Entries also feature guides to further reading and extensive cross-referencing. Subjects covered include: ● Thinking about landscape ● Cultural evolution ● Social archaeology ● Gender archaeology ● Experimental archaeology ● Archaeology of cult and religion ● Concepts of time ● The Antiquity of Man ● Feminist archaeology ● Multiregional evolution Archaeology: The Key Concepts is the ideal reference guide for students, teachers and anyone with an interest in archaeology. Colin Renfrew is Emeritus Disney Professor of Archaeology and Fellow of the McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge. Paul Bahn is a freelance writer, translator and broadcaster on archaeology. YOU MAY ALSO BE INTERESTED IN THE FOLLOWING ROUTLEDGE STUDENT REFERENCE TITLES: Archaeology: The Basics Clive Gamble Ancient History: Key Themes and Approaches Neville Morley Who’s Who in Ancient Egypt Michael Rice Who’s Who in the Ancient Near East Gwendolyn Leick Who’s Who in the Greek World John Hazel Who’s Who in the Roman World John Hazel ARCHAEOLOGY The Key Concepts Edited by Colin Renfrew and Paul Bahn LONDON AND NEW YORK First published 2005 by Routledge 2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX 14 4RN Simultaneously published in the USA and Canada by Routledge 270 Madison Ave., New York, NY 10016 Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group This edition published in the Taylor & Francis e-Library, 2005.
    [Show full text]
  • COGNITIVE ARCHAEOLOGY: in SEARCH of the EARLIEST SYNTACTIC LANGUAGE-USERS in HIMALAYA Maheshwar P
    COGNITIVE ARCHAEOLOGY: IN SEARCH OF THE EARLIEST SYNTACTIC LANGUAGE-USERS IN HIMALAYA Maheshwar P. Joshi Recent scientific studies unfold that neural structures bearing on intonation of speech have a deep evolutionary history traced to mammal-like reptiles called therapsids found in the Triassic period (∼252.17 mya, million years ago). Therefore, these structures were already present in the primates. It goes to the credit of Homo sapiens who developed it to the extent that humans are defined as symbolling animals, for language is the most articulated symbolism. Cognitive archaeology makes it clear that it took hominins millions of years to develop a syntactic language. Stratigraphically controlled and securely established artefact-bearing sites of the Middle Palaeolithic Arjun complex in the Deokhuri Valley, West Nepal, provide firm dates for the presence of the earliest syntactic language speakers in Himalaya from 100 ka to 70 ka (thousand years ago). Keywords: cognitive archaeology, neural structure, syntactic language, verbal communication, Arjun complex 1. Introduction Study of prehistory of language falls in the domain of cognitive archaeology and this discipline ‘is still in early development’ (Renfrew 2008: 67). To the best of my information, no scholar studying South Asian prehistory has addressed this issue despite the fact that South Asia is very rich in material culture vis-à-vis prehistory of language (see for details, Joshi 2014: in press-a; Joshi 2017: in press-b). Since my primary concern in this essay is Himalaya in general and Central Himalaya in particular, I will focus on the earliest syntactic language-users in the context of Central Himalaya.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Archaeology Abstract 1. Introduction
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by PhilSci Archive From Things to Thinking: Cognitive Archaeology Adrian Currie & Anton Killin Penultimate version, forthcoming in Mind & Language Acknowledgements This paper was read by Exeter University’s Culture & Cognition group, who provided invaluable feedback. Alexandra Ion, Kim Sterelny, Sarah Jackson and Greg Currie all provided useful comments on drafts. Adrian’s research was supported by a grant by the World Templeton Charity Foundation. Abstract Cognitive archaeologists infer from material remains to the cognitive features of past societies. We characterize cognitive archaeology in terms of trace-based reasoning, that is, cognitive archaeology involves inferences drawing upon background theory linking objects from the archaeological record to cognitive (including psychological, symbolic, and ideological) features. We analyse such practices, examining work on cognitive evolution, language, and musicality. We argue that the central epistemic challenge for cognitive archaeology is often not a paucity of material remains, but insufficient constraint from cognitive theories. However, we also argue that the success of cognitive archaeology doesn’t necessarily require well-developed cognitive theories: success might instead lead to them. 1. Introduction Human cognition and things are intimately linked: we build tools and manipulate our environment; we innovate, experiment, and probe. Regardless of cognition’s exact nature and its precise relationship with materiality, this suggests that much can be gleaned about mental and 1 cultural lives from material objects. This thought underwrites cognitive archaeology: using the material remains of past cultures as inferential windows to the development of human thought, past inner lives and social practices.
    [Show full text]
  • COGNITIVE ARCHAEOLOGY and HUMAN EVOLUTION This Book Presents New Directions in the Study of Cognitive Archaeology
    COGNITIVE ARCHAEOLOGY AND HUMAN EVOLUTION This book presents new directions in the study of cognitive archaeology. Seeking to understand the conditions that led to the development of a variety of cognitive processes during evolution, it uses evidence from empirical studies and offers theoretical speculations about the evolu- tion of modern thinking as well. The volume draws from the fields of archaeology and neuropsychology, which traditionally have shared little in the way of theories and methods, even though both disciplines provide crucial pieces to the puzzle of the emergence and evolution of human cognition. The twelve essays, written by an international team of scholars, represent an eclectic array of interests, methods, and theories about evolutionary cognitive archaeology. Collectively, they consider whether the processes in the development of human cognition sim- ply made use of anatomical and cerebral structures already in place at the beginning of hominization. They also consider the possibility of an active role of hominoids in their own development and query the impact of hominoid activity in the emergence of new cognitive abilities. Sophie A. de Beaune is Professor of Prehistory at Jean Moulin Univer- sity – Lyon 3 and Deputy Scientific Director at the French National Center for Scientific Research (CNRS). The author of eight books, most recently L’Homme et L’outil: L’invention Technique durant la Prehistoire´ , she is also director of a book series entitled “Le passe´ recom- pose”´ at CNRS Editions. Frederick L. Coolidge is Professor of Psychology at the University of Colorado at Colorado Springs. He has published extensively in behav- ioral genetics, neuropsychology, psychopathology assessment, and cog- nitive archaeology, with recent articles in Cambridge Archaeological Journal, the Journal of Human Evolution, and Journal of Archaeologi- cal Research, among others.
    [Show full text]
  • University of Colorado, Colorado Springs Center for Cognitive Archaeology ANTH 4515 / 5515: the Archaeology of Numbers
    University of Colorado, Colorado Springs Center for Cognitive Archaeology ANTH 4515 / 5515: The Archaeology of Numbers Professor: Karenleigh A. Overmann, DPhil, Oxon Email: [email protected] Phone: 719-332-7183 Office Hours: Coordinate appointments via email. OVERVIEW Numbers are integral to daily life: We use them while shopping, telling time, placing phone calls, and driving cars. This course will introduce you to the ABCs of numbers—their archaeological, biological, cultural, and linguistic aspects. We will look at how such cultural systems are invented, how and why numbers vary across systems and change with systems over time, how societies use numbers, how having numbers changes societies, and much more. This is not a math course! However, basic arithmetical competency (adding, subtracting, multiplying, and dividing small numbers) is recommended (and hoped-for). Calculators are permitted and encouraged, though most of you will find them unnecessary for the simple calculations involved in understanding what numbers are and how they vary and change. TOPICS AND ASSIGNMENTS Week 1 Numbers in a nutshell: What’s a “natural” number? Cardinality (how many of something there are) vs. ordinality (how numbers are ordered). What’s a number: zero and “non-being”; one and “the unity.” How high can you count? “Highest number counted” vs infinity. Highest number counted and numerical elaboration. Numerical change over time and historical cumulatively. An analytical framework for comparing number systems across cultures. • Reading − Bender, A., & Beller, S. (2011). Cultural variation in numeration systems and their mapping onto the mental number line. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(4), 579–597. − Chrisomalis, S.
    [Show full text]