Innate Capacity and Linguistic Input: Co- Constructing First Language Acquisition

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Innate Capacity and Linguistic Input: Co- Constructing First Language Acquisition 1 INNATE CAPACITY AND LINGUISTIC INPUT: CO- CONSTRUCTING FIRST LANGUAGE ACQUISITION Dra. Dian Rianita, MA (Universitas Lancang Kuning – Pekanbaru) Hanafi, PhD (Universitas Andalas – Padang) _____________________________________________________________________ Abstract: The issue of how children acquire their first language has become noticeable from time to time. There are two dominant mainstreams, which explore the significant background of first language acquisition such as the existence of language acquisition devices and the role of the linguistic input. Both have their arguments to clarify the process of language acquisition. This paper discusses those ideas in which the first language acquisition are explained by the collaboration of two influenced theories since both theories provide the comprehensive analysis regarding the complex process of first language acquisition by a child. 1. INTRODUCTION How children acquire their first language continues to be an interesting issue for years. Experts from multiple disciplines such as linguistics, psychology, education, etc., have been continuously exploring the development of the human being in acquiring language. As pointed out by Roeper (1986), the ultimate issue in linguistics theory is the explanation of how children are capable of acquiring human language because unfolding the process of first language acquisition help discover the key for opening up the secrecy of language origin and second language acquisition. From the extensive literature of first language acquisition, there emerge two contrasting views which define child's acquisition of a language. The first view claims that first acquisition will only be predominantly possible by the existing human-specific innate capacity, early known as the Language Acquisition Device (Chomsky, 1965). The latter sees first language acquisition as externally stimulated by sufficient linguistic 2 inputs. This paper argues for the joint contribution of both innate capacity and linguistic input in assisting the children to acquire their first language and considers that none is more dominant than the other is as both co-construct the acquisition. 2. THE INNATE CAPACITY HYPOTHESIS Innate capacity or innate linguistic knowledge firstly emerged in the spirit of cognitive revolution in language inquiries the 1950's and 1960's as the result of Chomsky’s (1965) criticism upon Skinner’s (1957) behavioristic view of language acquisition. Chomsky claimed that children would never acquire the tools needed for processing an infinite number of sentences if the language acquisition mechanism was dependent on language input alone. Chomsky gave his further arguments by providing a term "language faculty" as one of cognitive faculties that facilitate language learning and acquisition (1965, p.56). This innate ability allows children to creatively generate infinite linguistic outputs that were never introduced to them. This view contributes to the theoretical assumption of the existence of Universal Grammar (UG). This internalized grammar allows learners to go beyond particular sentences fed by the input, permitting generalization and generation of comprehensible and grammatical linguistic outputs. As language acquisition is well known as a selective process where the child sets the values of parameters based on the linguistic environment, acquiring a language is “...an effortless achievement for children because they get it without explicit teaching and on the basis of positive evidence (what they hear from the environment)... (Guasti, 2002, p.4).” In addition, children develop their language skills in a limited amount of time, under unpredictable circumstances, and in identical ways across different languages. One important thing about first language acquisition is the fact that there is an undeniable similarity in the early language of children all over the world (Klein, 1996) regarding the developmental stages of first language acquisition. 3 “Every child undergoes the similar developmental process in acquiring their first language, i.e., from the earliest vocalization or babbling, one-word sentence, phrasal expression, and so on (Clark, 2009, p.14)”. For the proponents of this naturalistic view of innateness, this similar development could be one evidence for the presence of universal grammar and the dominant role of language acquisition device that is inherently planted only in human brains. 3. THE LINGUISTIC INPUT HYPOTHESIS From the other side of the bench, another perspective for first language acquisition is drawn by considering the role of environment, i.e., nurture. Proponents of this view argue that despite the specificity of human capacity in acquiring and using language to interact with each other, the role of linguistic inputs in the process of language learning is more dominant and empirically evident in determining children first language acquisition. In other words, children need to receive sufficient linguistic inputs from their environment for them to develop their own grammatical system in their first language. Without this external input, such innate ability will never be fully functional (Cowie, 2008). Furthermore, Pinker (1995) and Klein (1994) also argue that human was not born with a specific language in their head. At birth, a child does not speak at all, and they can only speak a language because they get in touch with the environment. Therefore, the individual’s capacity to talk and understand a particular language depends not only on such hypothetical ability as the innate capacity but more importantly on other definite sources, namely linguistic input. A more accommodating argument is also suggested that despite strong theoretical argumentation for children's quick and independent learning of syntactic rules, other linguistic and sociolinguistic abilities cannot be acquired in the absence of sufficient input from the facilitative environment (Clark, 2009, p.2). 4 Such linguistic input can occur in the early stage when mothers and other caretakers give the stimulus by developing a special way of talking to the children, commonly known as baby talk or motherese. In this case, adults usually simplify their speech to refer to objects in the immediate here and now; they modify their speech patterns, using a higher overall pitch, often with a rising intonation at the end of sentences; their speech is slower and more precise; there are more instances of emphatic stress. They commonly repeat utterances frequently, expand, and elaborate on what it is the child has said. Sometimes, according to Snow (1976), there are grammatical modifications in speech addressed to young children as compared to speech addressed to older children or adult in order to let the children comprehend the language. Those modifications cover fewer verbs, modifiers, conjunctions, and prepositions, including less use of third person constructions, passive voice, and other more complicated constructions. This type of talking and communication may resemble Skinner’s (1957) behavioristic approach to language acquisition. In his term, language development is accounted by means of environmental influence. Correct utterances are positively reinforced when the child realizes the communicative value of words and phrases. In this view, children learn their first language because they are positively reinforced to produce correct verbal expressions and negatively reinforced when making errors. However, the linguistic input hypothesis is different from behaviorism in seeing the nature of child acquisition. The linguistic input hypothesis is still operating under the cognitive view. While behaviorism sees that behavioristic reinforcement as the primary tools for acquiring specific behavior in animals, it fails to explain the infinite range of linguistic creativity shown by normal children after the age of three, who are never taught the grammar lesson or given infinite number of sentence construction. These children can creatively produce new sentences or utterances that they never heard before (Pinker, 1995, p.108), which is beyond the descriptive coverage of behaviorism. 5 4. JOINT COLLABORATION IN FIRST LANGUAGE ACQUISITION: INNATE ABILITY AND LINGUISTIC INPUT These two hypotheses are not mutually exclusive to each other. They both are complementary because the absence of one will significantly inhibit the acquisitional process. On the one hand, the lack of human-specific mental capacity for generating grammatical rules in a child's brain will affect his/her ability in developing his linguistic and communicative skills. Many cases show the existence of this innate capacity in every normal human being. Cases, where normal adults lose their ability to speak after a stroke, prove that human brain is clearly equipped with such capacity that any other animals never have, as proven in Kanazi and Bonobo's case (Segerdahl, Fields, & Savage-Rumbaugh, 2005). On the other hand, the lack or absent of linguistic input (nurture) in the critical process of child first language acquisition also severely prevent children from succeeding in acquiring the first language. Cases of feral children like Genie (Curtis, 1977), the wild girl of Champagne, Prava (the bird boy), and many others are hard proofs of the importance of linguistic input in the process of children first language acquisition. Despite maintaining their perfect surviving ability and assumingly having the innate capacity, these socially isolated and abandoned children cannot develop their innate capacity due to
Recommended publications
  • Second Language Acquisition Through Neurolinguistic Programming: a Psychoanalytic Approach
    International Journal of Engineering & Technology, 7 (4.36) (2018) 624-629 International Journal of Engineering & Technology Website: www.sciencepubco.com/index.php/IJET Research paper Second Language Acquisition Through Neurolinguistic Programming: A Psychoanalytic Approach A. Delbio1*, M. Ilankumaran2 1Research Scholar in English, Noorul Islam Centre for Higher Education, Kumaracoil. 2Professor of English, Noorul Islam Centre for Higher Education, Kumaracoil, Thuckalay, Tamilnadu, India. E-mail:[email protected] *Corresponding author E-mail: [email protected] Abstract English is the only lingua-franca for the whole world in present age of globalization and liberalization. English language is considered as an important tool to acquire a new and technical information and knowledge. In this situation English learners and teachers face a lot of problems psychologically. Neuro linguistic studies the brain mechanism and the performance of the brain in linguistic competences. The brain plays a main role in controlling motor and sensory activities and in the process of thinking. Studies regarding development of brain bring some substantiation for psychological and anatomical way of language development. Neuro-Linguistic Programming (NLP) deals with psychological and neurological factors. It also deals with the mode of brain working and the way to train the brain to achieve the purpose. Many techniques are used in the NLP. It improves the fluency and accuracy in target language. It improves non-native speaker to improve the LSRW skills. This paper brings out the importance of the NLP in language learning and teaching. It also discusses the merits and demerits of the NLP in learning. It also gives the solution to overcome the problems and self-correction is motivated through neuro-linguistic programming.
    [Show full text]
  • Statistical Language Learning: Learning-Oriented Theories Is That Mechanisms and Constraints Such Accounts Seem at Odds with One of the Central Observations 1 Jenny R
    110 VOLUME 12, NUMBER 4, AUGUST 2003 most important arguments against Statistical Language Learning: learning-oriented theories is that Mechanisms and Constraints such accounts seem at odds with one of the central observations 1 Jenny R. Saffran about human languages. The lin- Department of Psychology and Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin–Madison, guistic systems of the world, de- Madison, Wisconsin spite surface differences, share deep similarities, and vary in non- arbitrary ways. Theories of language Abstract guage, it seems improbable that chil- acquisition that focus primarily on What types of mechanisms dren could ever discern its structure. preexisting knowledge of language underlie the acquisition of hu- The process of acquiring such a sys- do provide an elegant explanation man language? Recent evidence tem is likely to be nearly as com- for cross-linguistic similarities. suggests that learners, includ- plex as the system itself, so it is not Such theories, which are exempli- ing infants, can use statistical surprising that the mechanisms un- fied by the seminal work of Noam properties of linguistic input to derlying language acquisition are a Chomsky, suggest that linguistic uni- discover structure, including matter of long-standing debate. One versals are prespecified in the child’s sound patterns, words, and the of the central focuses of this debate linguistic endowment, and do not re- beginnings of grammar. These concerns the innate and environ- quire learning. Such accounts gener- abilities appear to be both pow- mental contributions to the lan- ate predictions about the types of erful and constrained, such that guage-acquisition process, and the patterns that should be observed some statistical patterns are degree to which these components cross-linguistically, and lead to im- more readily detected and used draw on information and abilities portant claims regarding the evolu- than others.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Acquisition Device and the Origin of Language Briana Sobecks
    Language Acquisition Device and the Origin of Language Briana Sobecks In the early twentieth century, psychologists re- a particular language, the universal grammar can be refined alized that language is not just understanding words, but to fit a specific language. Even if some children may hear a also requires learning grammar, syntax, and semantics. specific language pattern more than others, the fact that all Modern language is incredibly complex, but young chil- children know it indicates a poss ble innate language sense. dren can understand it remarkably well. This idea supports One of Chomsky’s main tenants in his LAD theory Chomsky’s idea that language learning is innate. According is the Poverty of Stimulus argument. Though children do to his hypothesis, young children receive “primary linguistic collect data to learn a language, it is unlikely that the data data” from what is spoken around them, which helps them they are exposed to is enough to master an entire lan- develop knowledge of that specific language (Cowie 2008). guage. Instead, they must infer grammatical rules through Children passively absorb language from adults, peers, an internal sense. There are several cognitive factors that and exposure to media. However, this data is not sufficient support this argument. Underdetermination states that the to explain how children can learn unique constructions finite data is applicable in infinite situations. In context, of words and grammar patterns. Previously structuralists this means that children utilize the finite amount of data created a list of “phrase structure rules” to generate all they hear to generate any possible sentence.
    [Show full text]
  • A Linguistic Perspective on the Acquisition of German As an L2
    i A Linguistic Perspective on the Acquisition of German as an L2 A thesis submitted to the Miami University Honors Program in partial fulfillment of the requirements for University Honors with Distinction by Nicholas D. Stoller (December 2006) Oxford, Ohio ii ABSTRACT A LINGUISTIC PERSPECTIVE ON THE ACQUISITION OF GERMAN AS AN L2 by Nicholas D. Stoller It is obvious that the setting of acquisition, the amount and type of input, and the motivation of learners play a large role in adult second language (L2) acquisition. Many of the theories of L2 acquisition unfortunately fail to adequately take these variables into account. This thesis gives an overview of the current and past theories, including evidence for and against each theory. This is supplemented by an error analysis of second year Miami University students to see if this can give support to any of the current theories. Once that is completed, I examine the relation between input and the possibility of a language learning device such as UG and then move on to pedagogical application of my findings. iii Contents Chapter Page 1 Introduction 1 2 2 The Basis of the Study of L2 Acquisition 2 3 Linguistic Theories of L2 Acquisition 7 3.1 Theories without UG 7 3.1.1 Contrastive Analysis Hypothesis 7 3.1.2 Markedness Difference Hypothesis 8 3.1.3 Fundamental Difference Hypothesis 9 3.1.4 Information Processing Approach 10 3.2 Theories with Partial UG 13 3.2.1 Transfer Hypothesis 13 3.2.2 Krashen’s Comprehension Hypothesis 14 3.3 Theories with Full UG use 19 3.3.1 Identity Hypothesis 19 3.3.2 Full Transfer/Full Access Hypothesis 20 3.4 Overview of the Theories 21 4 Error Analysis and Miami University 2nd 22 Year Students 4.1 Errors of Cases Following Verbs 23 4.2 Errors of Gender of Nouns 25 4.3 Errors of Verb Form 26 4.4 Errors of Umlaut Usage 29 5 Relation of UG and Input 30 6.1 Problems with Input in Classroom Instruction 33 6.2 Pedagogy and L2 Acquisition 35 7 Conclusion 40 Bibliography 42 iv 1 A Linguistic Perspective on the Acquisition of German as an L2 1.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 the Story of Language Acquisition: from Words to World and Back
    The story of language acquisition: From words to world and back again Amy Pace Temple University Dani F. Levine Temple University Giovanna Morini University of Delaware Kathy Hirsh-Pasek Temple University Roberta Michnick Golinkoff University of Delaware Pace, A., Levine, D., Morini, G., Hirsh-Pasek, K. & Golinkoff, R.M. (2016) The story of language acquisition: From words to world and back again. In L. Balter and C. Tamis-LeMonda C. (Eds.), Child Psychology: A Handbook of Contemporary Issues, 3rd Edition. 43-79. 1 Introduction Imagine an infant visiting the zoo with her mother. From her stroller, she observes a troop of capuchins on a nearby tree. Her mother points to the scene and says, “Look! The monkeys are grooming each other!” How might she come to understand that her mother’s arbitrary auditory signals represent something about a scene that she is witnessing? How does she parse the continuous actions of the apes to derive appropriate units of meaning such as agents or actions from this complex, dynamic event? And how might she make the correct assumptions about how the words relate to the unfolding events before her? Despite recent advances, much of the current debate centers on the classical questions of how infants map words onto the dazzling array of sights and sounds in their world and how this process is guided by development and experience. Indeed, the field is still pondering the classic gavagai story that was introduced in 1960 by Quine. Given the complexity of the world, how is a language learner to know that the foreign word gavagai uttered while a rabbit scurries by, refers to the entire rabbit rather than to the fur, ears, or ground on which it thumps.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurolinguistic Applications to SLA Classroom Instruction: a Review of the Issues with a Focus on Danesi's Bimodality
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 423 677 FL 025 478 AUTHOR Kim-Rivera, E. G. TITLE Neurolinguistic Applications to SLA Classroom Instruction: A Review of the Issues with a Focus on Danesi's Bimodality. PUB DATE 1998-00-00 NOTE 14p.; For complete volume of working papers, see FL 025 473. PUB TYPE Journal Articles (080) Reports Research (143) JOURNAL CIT Texas Papers in Foreign Language Education; v3 n2 p91-103 Spr 1998 EDRS PRICE MF01/PC01 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS *Brain Hemisphere Functions; *Language Processing; *Linguistic Theory; *Neurolinguistics; Second Language Instruction; Second Language Learning; Second Languages IDENTIFIERS *Bimodal Theory; *Danesi (Marcel) ABSTRACT Few studies have approached second language teaching from a neurolinguistic perspective. An exception is Marcel Danesi's educational construct of neurological bimodality, an attempt to find a neurological foundation for classroom language instruction. The underlying hypothesis is that there is a natural flow of information processing from the right to the left hemispheres of the brain during language learning; therefore, language instruction should reflect that flow direction by providing concrete forms of instruction at early language learning stages and more formal and abstract instruction at later stages. However, the hypothesis raises questions; for example, the evidence for riaht-hemisphere functions in second language learning is contradictory, yet those functions are an important elemenc in bimodality. In addition, there have been few empirical studies supporting the hypothesis.
    [Show full text]
  • First and Second Language Acquisition: a Brief Comparison
    Raymond Hickey English Linguistics Campus Essen First and second language acquisition: A brief comparison First language acquisition Second language acquisition An instinct, triggered by birth A personal choice, required motivation Very rapid Varies, but never as quick as FLA Complete Never as good as a native speaker, though good competence can be achieved Natural (no instruction) Natural or guided (for synthetic languages grammatical instruction is required Note the following facts concerning first and second language acquisition 1) The level of competence acquired with SLA (second language acquisition) depends not so much on the time spent learning a language as the time at which one begins. 2) In general linguists maintain that a first language is acquired, i.e. that knowledge is stored unconsciously, and that a second language is learned, i.e. that knowledge is gained by conscious study of the second language’s structure. However, this distinction is not watertight and SLA can involve acquisition to a certain degree. FLA (first language acquisition) does not, however, learned as no instruction is required. Remember that you know your first language before you start school. 3) There is a critical period, that of puberty, around 12 or 13 years of age, after which it is difficult, if not impossible to acquire a second language with the same degree of competence as the first language. The decline in ability to acquire a second language may also be connected with the lateralisation of the brain just before puberty, i.e. with the fixing of functions in one or other of the two halves of the brain.
    [Show full text]
  • Experimental Methods in Studying Child Language Acquisition Ben Ambridge∗ and Caroline F
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by MPG.PuRe Advanced Review Experimental methods in studying child language acquisition Ben Ambridge∗ and Caroline F. Rowland This article reviews the some of the most widely used methods used for studying children’s language acquisition including (1) spontaneous/naturalistic, diary, parental report data, (2) production methods (elicited production, repeti- tion/elicited imitation, syntactic priming/weird word order), (3) comprehension methods (act-out, pointing, intermodal preferential looking, looking while lis- tening, conditioned head turn preference procedure, functional neuroimaging) and (4) judgment methods (grammaticality/acceptability judgments, yes-no/truth- value judgments). The review outlines the types of studies and age-groups to which each method is most suited, as well as the advantage and disadvantages of each. We conclude by summarising the particular methodological considerations that apply to each paradigm and to experimental design more generally. These include (1) choosing an age-appropriate task that makes communicative sense (2) motivating children to co-operate, (3) choosing a between-/within-subjects design, (4) the use of novel items (e.g., novel verbs), (5) fillers, (6) blocked, coun- terbalanced and random presentation, (7) the appropriate number of trials and participants, (8) drop-out rates (9) the importance of control conditions, (10) choos- ing a sensitive dependent measure (11) classification of responses, and (12) using an appropriate statistical test. © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. How to cite this article: WIREs Cogn Sci 2013, 4:149–168. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1215 INTRODUCTION them to select those that are most useful for studying particular phenomena.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Language.Pdf
    PREFINAL DRAFT: Kirby, S. (2007). The evolution of language. In Dunbar, R. and Barrett, L., editors, Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, pages 669–681. OUP Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology 25 November 2005 The Evolution of Language Simon Kirby Language Evolution and Computation Research Unit University of Edinburgh www.ling.ed.ac.uk/~simon 1. Introduction: language and the evolution of life Maynard Smith & Szathmáry (1997) set out 8 “major transitions” in the evolution of life. These are events in the history of our planet that signal radical changes in the way evolution works. They start with a change in the way molecules replicate in the very earliest stages of the origins of life, through the emergence of DNA, and go on to include larger-scale later phenomena like the evolu- tion of colonies where once there were only solitary individuals (see gure 1). What makes the work of these two eminent evolutionary biologists so interesting for us is their inclusion of the most recent evolutionary transition: the emergence of language. Why is the emergence of language such a signicant event? What does it have in common with the other major evolutionary transitions? One of Maynard Smith & Szathmáry’s interesting obser- vations is that, despite their diversity, these transitions have some features in common. In particu- lar, many of the transitions give rise to a new mechanism for the transmission of information. Lan- guage, they argue, provides just such a novel mechanism - essentially enabling a system of cultural transmission with unlimited heredity. It is clearly true that language enables the transmission and storage of very complex cultural in- formation.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Acquisition and Language Development of Children
    Jurnal Ar-Rihlah Inovasi Pengembangan Pendidikan Islam Volume. 4. No.2. 2019 Fakultas Tarbiyah IAINU Kebumen ISSN: 2541-402X ISSN: 2851-0197 http://ejournal.iainu-kebumen.ac.id/index.php/Ar-rihlah/index LANGUAGE ACQUISITION AND LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT OF CHILDREN Kartika S UIN Raden Intan Lampung [email protected] Nadia Raifah Nawa Kartika Iinstitut Agama Islam Nahdlatul Ulama Kebumen nadiaranaka22@gmailcom Abstract: General Psycholinguistics is the study of how the observation or perception adults of the language and how it produces language also regarding underlying cognitive processes when a person uses language. This article focused on language acquisition and language development of children. There is problem that faced in language teaching Psycholinguistics. As psycholinguistics is a core of language, knowing the language acquisition and language development of children is one of solutions to solve the problems in language teaching. It is divided two kinds of languages, Language Acquisition of Children and Language Development of Children. Keywords: psycholinguistics, language acquisition, language development 239 Jurnal Ar-Rihlah Inovasi Pengembangan Pendidikan Islam Volume. 4. No.2. 2019 Fakultas Tarbiyah IAINU Kebumen ISSN: 2541-402X ISSN: 2851-0197 http://ejournal.iainu-kebumen.ac.id/index.php/Ar-rihlah/index A. INTRODUCTION Psycholinguistics is a study of the use of language and language acquisition by humans ( Levelt , 1975) . According to Levelt, there are three the main study areas of psycholinguistics, namely: general psycholinguistics, psycholinguistic development and applied psycholinguistics. General Psycholinguistics is the study of how the observation or perception adults of the language and how it produces language also regarding underlying cognitive processes when a person uses language.
    [Show full text]
  • What Neurolinguistic Methodologies (Might) Tell Us About Linguistic And
    Evidence from neurolinguistic methodologies: Can it actually inform linguistic/language acquisition theories and translate to evidence-based applications? Leah Roberts1, Jorge González Alonso2,3, Christos Pliatsikas3 and Jason Rothman3,2 1. University of York, UK 2. UiT The Arctic University of Norway 3. University of Reading, UK Abstract This special issue is a testament to the recent burgeoning interest by theoretical linguists, language acquisitionists and teaching practitioners in the neuroscience of language. It offers a highly valuable, state-of-the-art overview of the neurophysiological methods that are currently being applied to questions in the field of second language (L2) acquisition, teaching and processing. Research in the area of neurolinguistics has developed dramatically in the past twenty years, providing a wealth of exciting findings, many of which are discussed in the papers in this volume. The goal of this commentary is twofold. The first is to critically assess the current state of neurolinguistic data from the point of view of language acquisition and processing—informed by the papers that comprise this special issue and the literature as a whole—pondering how the neuroscience of language/processing might inform us with respect to linguistic and language acquisition theories. The second goal is to offer some links from implications of exploring the first goal towards informing language teachers and the creation of linguistically and neurolinguistically-informed evidence-based pedagogies for non-native language teaching. Introduction Research into linguistic and language acquisition theories and teaching spans a number of theoretical approaches, and with researchers working from within a range of different of disciplines.
    [Show full text]
  • On Linguistic Environment for Foreign Language Acquisition
    Vol. 1, No. 1 Asian Culture and History On Linguistic Environment for Foreign Language Acquisition Chengjun Wang School of Foreign Languages Yangtze Normal University 98 Julong Road, Lidu, Fuling, Chongqing 408100, China Tel: 86-23-7279-0023 E-mail: [email protected] The research is financed by the Project of Advanced English of Yangtze Normal University. No. 2006-B022 (Sponsoring information) Abstract It is clear that children acquire their first language without explicit learning. A foreign or second language is usually learned but to some degree may also be acquired or “picked up” depending on the environmental setting. So, this article mainly discusses the linguistic environmental setting for foreign language acquisition. It suggested that we should make an effective linguistic environment for foreign language acquisition in foreign language classroom. Keywords: Acquisition, Linguistic environment, Linguistic input, Foreign language teaching 1. Introduction Language acquisition refers to the learning and development of a person’s language. The learning of a native or first language is called first Language Acquisition, and of a second or foreign language, Second Language Acquisition (Richards Jack C.John Platt and Heidi Platt, 2000). The term “acquisition” is often preferred to “learning” because the latter term is sometimes linked to a behaviorist theory of learning. Language acquisition is studied by linguists, psychologists and applied linguists to enable them to understand the process used in learning a language, to help identify stages in the developmental process, and to give a better understand of the nature of language. Techniques used include longitudinal studies of language learners as well as experimental approaches, and focus on the study of the development of phonology, grammar, vocabulary, and communicative competence.
    [Show full text]