8 Using Natural Experiments to Provide “Arguably Exogenous” Treatment Variability

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

8 Using Natural Experiments to Provide “Arguably Exogenous” Treatment Variability 8 Using Natural Experiments to Provide “Arguably Exogenous” Treatment Variability The cost to families of investing in their children’s education is of concern the world over. In some developing countries, the question is whether charging fees for enrollment in secondary school reduces access to education markedly for low-income families. Recently, some countries, beginning with Brazil and Mexico, have turned this question on its head and examined whether charging negative prices (that is, making cash pay- ments to low-income families that enroll their children in secondary school) will increase enrollment (Fiszbein, Schady, & Ferreira, 2009 ). In the United States and other industrialized countries, the focus is on the impact of college cost on decisions to enroll in post-secondary educa- tion. In every country, knowledge of the sensitivity of families’ educational decisions to the cost of education is critical to sound educational policy- making. A challenge, then, is to provide compelling evidence about the causal impact of a change in cost on families’ educational decisions. As we have argued in Chapter 4, a randomized experiment provides the most persuasive strategy for answering educational policy questions about the causal impact of school fees or scholarships on families’ educa- tional enrollment decisions. Indeed, as we explain in Chapter 14, a number of recent randomized experiments have shed new light on the impact of costs on school-enrollment decisions. Researcher-designed ran- domized experiments on this topic are often diffi cult to carry out, however, due to cost and diffi culty in obtaining the requisite cooperation of par- ticipants and educational institutions. As a result, researchers often try to learn from experiments that occur naturally. These “natural” experiments are situations in which some external agency, perhaps a natural disaster, or an idiosyncrasy of geography or 135 136 Methods Matter birth date, or a sudden unexpected change in a longstanding educational policy “assigns” participants randomly to potential “treatment” and “control” groups. The challenge for the researcher is to recognize such natural experiments when they occur and to be prepared to deal with the oppor- tunities and challenges they present. In the next section of this chapter, we explain the respects in which investigator-designed experiments and natural experiments are similar and the respects in which they are different. We then use data from two excellent studies to illustrate how researchers have taken advantage of natural experiments to address important policy questions about cause and effect. In the subsequent section, we point out sources of natural experiments that have proven productive in the past, and we sum- marize their common features. Then, we describe two important threats to internal validity that are an integral part of working with data from a special kind of natural experiment that has what we will refer to as a discontinuity design . We then explain how a novel analytic approach known as difference-in-differences estimation responds sensibly to one of these valid- ity threats. Natural- and Investigator-Designed Experiments: Similarities and Differences Central to the internal validity of an experiment is the assignment of par- ticipants to the experimental conditions. When we say that experimental assignment is exogenous, we mean that it is beyond any possible manipu- lation by the participants themselves, so that membership in either a treatment or a control group is totally independent of the participants’ own motivations and decisions. So, when investigators assign participants in an experiment randomly to experimental conditions, the assignment is exogenous because, by defi nition, participants in a fair lottery cannot infl uence the outcome. Randomized assignment, in turn, renders mem- bers of the treatment and control groups equal in expectation prior to the intervention. Consequently, any between-group difference detected in the average value of the outcome, post-treatment, must be a causal consequence of the intervention itself. Participants are sometimes randomized to different program options, to innovative practices, or to different incentives by exogenous mecha- nisms that are not under the direct control of an investigator, but still provide the equality in expectation prior to treatment that supports causal inference. Provided that we can argue persuasively that those partici- pants who are then subject to the contrasting and naturally occurring Natural Experiments 137 “experimental” conditions are indeed equal in expectation prior to treat- ment, we have a logical basis for making unbiased inferences about the causal impact of the treatment. However, although data from natural experiments can sometimes be analyzed in the same way as data from investigator-designed experiments, you may need to modify your analytic strategy to respond to additional threats to internal validity that may occur when an experiment arises naturally. Two Examples of Natural Experiments We begin by describing examples of two prototypical natural experiments. The fi rst occurred when the U.S . Department of Defense introduced mil- itary draft lotteries during the Vietnam War era. Each lottery created two experimental groups of young males who were arguably equal in expecta- tion and differed only in that one group was offered— that is, could be drafted into — military service, whereas the second could not. Our second example occurred when the federal government ended, in 1982, a pro- gram that had previously provided college fi nancial aid to children who were the survivors of Social Security benefi ciaries. This abrupt policy shift meant that high-school seniors in and before 1981, whose fathers were deceased Social Security recipients, were “assigned” effectively to a treatment group that received an offer of college fi nancial aid. High- school seniors immediately after 1981, whose fathers were deceased Social Security recipients, were not made this aid offer. So long as these groups of high-school seniors were otherwise equal in expectation, we are pre- sented with naturally formed experimental groups that differed only in the offer of aid. The Vietnam-Era Draft Lottery The question of whether military service affects long-term labor-market outcomes for participants is a question many governments ask in the pro- cess of designing manpower policies. As Joshua Angrist ( 1990 ) pointed out, this question cannot be answered by using census data to compare the long-term earnings of men who served in the military and those who did not serve. The reason is that men are not usually assigned randomly to military service. Instead, men who have relatively unattractive employ- ment opportunities in the civilian sphere may tend to enter the military. Unobserved differences between those who volunteer to serve and those who do not may therefore create bias in the estimation of the long-term labor market consequences of military service. 138 Methods Matter A natural experiment that took place during the Vietnam War era provided Angrist with an opportunity to obtain unbiased estimates of the impact of military draft eligibility on long-term labor market outcomes. Between 1970 and 1975, the U.S. Department of Defense conducted fi ve draft lotteries that determined which American males in a particular age group were eligible to be drafted into military service. The 1970 lottery included men aged 19 through 26, and the lotteries in the four subse- quent years included men aged 19 to 20. In each lottery, a random-sequence number (RSN), ranging from 1 through 365, was assigned to each birth date. Then, only men in the relevant age cohorts whose birthdays had RSNs less than an exogenously determined ceiling, which was specifi ed by the Department of Defense each year, were subject to induction. Angrist called such men “draft-eligible.” A simple comparison of the annual earnings in the early 1980s for the group of men in a particular cohort who were draft-eligible with those who were not provides an unbi- ased estimate of the impact on earnings of being draft-eligible. Note that the treatment being administered in this natural experiment is “eligibility for the draft,” not the actual experience of military service. This is because it is only the assignment of young men to draft eligibility that was randomized, not military service itself. Indeed, some of those in the draft-eligible “treatment group” avoided military service by enrolling in college, by being declared unfi t for military service due to physical lim- itations, or by having been arrested prior to the draft. Among white males born in 1950, 35 % of those declared draft-eligible as a result of having a low draft number actually served in the military, compared to 19% of those declared draft-ineligible (Angrist, 1990 ; Table 2, p. 321). Of course, the fact that many draft-eligible men did not serve in the military, and some men who were not draft-eligible did serve, does not threaten the unbiased estimation of the impact of being “draft-eligible” on later labor- market outcomes. In fact, this natural experiment resembles the New York Scholarship Program (NYSP), the investigator-designed experiment in which the treatment was the randomized offer of a scholarship to help pay private-school tuition. As explained in Chapter 4, not all families that received the scholarship offer sent their child to a private school. As we will see in Chapter 11, it is possible to use a more sophisticated technique, called instrumental-variables estimation , to tease out the causal impact of actual military service (or actual private-school attendance), using the original randomly assigned offer as an “instrument.” In this chapter, how- ever, we focus only on estimating the impact of draft eligibility on later labor-market earnings. Combining information from the draft lotteries with information on subsequent earnings from the Social Security Administration, Angrist Natural Experiments 139 estimated the impact of draft-eligibility status on future annual earnings, separately for white and non-white men, in the several birth cohorts in which the draft lotteries were conducted.
Recommended publications
  • Natural Experiments
    Natural Experiments Jason Seawright [email protected] August 11, 2010 J. Seawright (PolSci) Essex 2010 August 11, 2010 1 / 31 Typology of Natural Experiments J. Seawright (PolSci) Essex 2010 August 11, 2010 2 / 31 Typology of Natural Experiments Classic Natural Experiment J. Seawright (PolSci) Essex 2010 August 11, 2010 2 / 31 Typology of Natural Experiments Classic Natural Experiment Instrumental Variables-Type Natural Experiment J. Seawright (PolSci) Essex 2010 August 11, 2010 2 / 31 Typology of Natural Experiments Classic Natural Experiment Instrumental Variables-Type Natural Experiment Regression-Discontinuity Design J. Seawright (PolSci) Essex 2010 August 11, 2010 2 / 31 Bolstering Understand assumptions. J. Seawright (PolSci) Essex 2010 August 11, 2010 3 / 31 Bolstering Understand assumptions. Explore role of qualitative evidence. J. Seawright (PolSci) Essex 2010 August 11, 2010 3 / 31 Classic Natural Experiment J. Seawright (PolSci) Essex 2010 August 11, 2010 4 / 31 Classic Natural Experiment 1 “Nature” randomizes the treatment. J. Seawright (PolSci) Essex 2010 August 11, 2010 4 / 31 Classic Natural Experiment 1 “Nature” randomizes the treatment. 2 All (observable and unobservable) confounding variables are balanced between treatment and control groups. J. Seawright (PolSci) Essex 2010 August 11, 2010 4 / 31 Classic Natural Experiment 1 “Nature” randomizes the treatment. 2 All (observable and unobservable) confounding variables are balanced between treatment and control groups. 3 No discretion is involved in assigning treatments, or the relevant information is unavailable or unused. J. Seawright (PolSci) Essex 2010 August 11, 2010 4 / 31 Classic Natural Experiment 1 “Nature” randomizes the treatment. 2 All (observable and unobservable) confounding variables are balanced between treatment and control groups.
    [Show full text]
  • The Distribution of Information in Speculative Markets: a Natural Experiment∗
    The Distribution of Information in Speculative Markets: A Natural Experiment∗ Alasdair Brown† University of East Anglia June 9, 2014 ∗I would like to thank an associate editor, two anonymous referees, Fuyu Yang, and seminar participants at UEA for comments on this work. All remaining errors are my own. †School of Economics, University of East Anglia, Norwich, NR4 7TJ. Email: [email protected] 1 1 Introduction Abstract We use a unique natural experiment to shed light on the distribution of information in speculative markets. In June 2011, Betfair - a U.K. betting exchange - levied a tax of up to 60% on all future profits accrued by the top 0.1% of profitable traders. Such a move appears to have driven at least some of these traders off the exchange, taking their information with them. We investigate the effect of the new tax on the forecasting capacity of the exchange (our measure of the market’s incorporation of information into the price). We find that there was scant decline in the forecasting capacity of the exchange - relative to a control market - suggesting that the bulk of information had hitherto been held by the majority of traders, rather than the select few affected by the rule change. This result is robust to the choice of forecasting measure, the choice of forecasting interval, and the choice of race type. This provides evidence that more than a few traders are typically involved in the price discovery process in speculative markets. JEL Classification: G14, G19 Keywords: informed trading, natural experiment, betting markets, horse racing 1 Introduction There is a common perception that the benefits of trading in speculative assets are skewed towards those on the inside of corporations, or those that move in the same circles as the corporate elite.
    [Show full text]
  • Synthetic Control Method: Inference, Sensitivity Analysis and Confidence Sets∗
    Synthetic Control Method: Inference, Sensitivity Analysis and Confidence Sets∗ Sergio Firpo† Vitor Possebom‡ Insper Yale University September 1st, 2017 Abstract We extend the inference procedure for the synthetic control method in two ways. First, we impose a parametric form to the treatment assignment probabilities that includes, as a particular case, the uniform distribution that is assumed by Abadie et al.(2015). By changing the value of this parameter, we can analyze the sensitiveness of the test's decision to deviations from the usual uniform distribution assumption. Second, we modify the RMSPE statistic to test any sharp null hypothesis, including, as a specific case, the null hypothesis of no effect whatsoever analyzed by Abadie et al.(2015). Based on this last extension, we invert the test statistic to estimate confidence sets that quickly show the point-estimates' precision, and the test's significance and robustness. We also extend these two tools to other test statistics and to problems with multiple outcome variables or multiple treated units. Furthermore, in a Monte Carlo experiment, we find that the RMSPE statistic has good properties with respect to size, power and robustness. Finally, we illustrate the usefulness of our proposed tools by reanalyzing the economic impact of ETA's terrorism in the Basque Country, studied first by Abadie & Gardeazabal(2003) and Abadie et al.(2011). Keywords: Synthetic Control Estimator, Hypothesis Testing, Sensitivity Analysis, Con- fidence Sets JEL Codes: C21, C23, C33 ∗We are grateful to FAPESP that provided financial aid through grant number 2014/23731-3. We are in debt to the editor and two anonymous referees for useful suggestions.
    [Show full text]
  • Randomized Controlled Trials, Development Economics and Policy Making in Developing Countries
    Randomized Controlled Trials, Development Economics and Policy Making in Developing Countries Esther Duflo Department of Economics, MIT Co-Director J-PAL [Joint work with Abhijit Banerjee and Michael Kremer] Randomized controlled trials have greatly expanded in the last two decades • Randomized controlled Trials were progressively accepted as a tool for policy evaluation in the US through many battles from the 1970s to the 1990s. • In development, the rapid growth starts after the mid 1990s – Kremer et al, studies on Kenya (1994) – PROGRESA experiment (1997) • Since 2000, the growth have been very rapid. J-PAL | THE ROLE OF RANDOMIZED EVALUATIONS IN INFORMING POLICY 2 Cameron et al (2016): RCT in development Figure 1: Number of Published RCTs 300 250 200 150 100 50 0 1975 1980 1985 1990 1995 2000 2005 2010 2015 Publication Year J-PAL | THE ROLE OF RANDOMIZED EVALUATIONS IN INFORMING POLICY 3 BREAD Affiliates doing RCT Figure 4. Fraction of BREAD Affiliates & Fellows with 1 or more RCTs 100% 90% 80% 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% 1980 or earlier 1981-1990 1991-2000 2001-2005 2006-today * Total Number of Fellows and Affiliates is 166. PhD Year J-PAL | THE ROLE OF RANDOMIZED EVALUATIONS IN INFORMING POLICY 4 Top Journals J-PAL | THE ROLE OF RANDOMIZED EVALUATIONS IN INFORMING POLICY 5 Many sectors, many countries J-PAL | THE ROLE OF RANDOMIZED EVALUATIONS IN INFORMING POLICY 6 Why have RCT had so much impact? • Focus on identification of causal effects (across the board) • Assessing External Validity • Observing Unobservables • Data collection • Iterative Experimentation • Unpack impacts J-PAL | THE ROLE OF RANDOMIZED EVALUATIONS IN INFORMING POLICY 7 Focus on Identification… across the board! • The key advantage of RCT was perceived to be a clear identification advantage • With RCT, since those who received a treatment are randomly selected in a relevant sample, any difference between treatment and control must be due to the treatment • Most criticisms of experiment also focus on limits to identification (imperfect randomization, attrition, etc.
    [Show full text]
  • Reflections on Experimental Techniques in the Law*
    University of Chicago Law School Chicago Unbound Journal Articles Faculty Scholarship 1974 Reflections on Experimental echniquesT in the Law Hans Zeisel Follow this and additional works at: https://chicagounbound.uchicago.edu/journal_articles Part of the Law Commons Recommended Citation Hans Zeisel, "Reflections on Experimental echniquesT in the Law," 15 Jurimetrics Journal 256 (1974). This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Faculty Scholarship at Chicago Unbound. It has been accepted for inclusion in Journal Articles by an authorized administrator of Chicago Unbound. For more information, please contact [email protected]. REFLECTIONS ON EXPERIMENTAL TECHNIQUES IN THE LAW* Hans Zeiselt Much of the law's reasoning turns on the effects of the rules it creates or enforces. Normally these effects are alleged or implied, but rarely proven. Yet during the last decades, first the natural sciences and, learn- ing from them, the social sciences have developed techniques of ek- perimentation that would allow firmer determinations of what a rule of law does or does not accomplish.' The basic tool (and also the paradigm for retrospective analysis) is the controlled experiment. Its essence is simple: The objects or people on which the experiment is to be performed are divided randomly, that is, by some lottery device, into two groups; the experimental treatment is then applied to the one group and withheld from the other, the control group. If an effect is found in the experimental group that is absent from the control group, it can be attributed to the experimental treatment, since at the time of the prior random selection the two groups had been strictly comparable, and in fact interchangeable.' And randomization assures that whatever the outcome of the experiment, it is possible to calculate the degree of certainty that can be attached to it.
    [Show full text]
  • Evidence of False Positives in Research Clearinghouses and Influential Journals: an Application of P-Curve to Policy Research
    Evidence of False Positives in Research Clearinghouses and Influential Journals: An Application of P-Curve to Policy Research Sean Tanner Selective reporting of results within studies, a practice known as “p-hacking,” presents a pernicious threat to the validity of published social science research. This form of publication bias is particularly troubling in research that influences public policy debates and programmatic funding. While recent analyses of political science, psychology, economic, and sociology research have identified considerable publication bias, the methods used are unable to distinguish p-hacking from accidents of random sampling. Using the novel “p-curve” estimation framework, which can distinguish p-hacking from other forms of false positives, I find little evidence of p-hacking in federal research clearinghouses and three rigorous, policy-oriented journals: the Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, The Journal of Human Resources, and Education Evaluation and Policy Analysis. The results demonstrate that most of the findings in this literature are likely to be true positives. I discuss methodological and substantive divergence from the prior literature and suggest future research that can more accurately assess the cross-disciplinary veracity of social science research. I. Introduction Recent work has uncovered a troubling pattern in social science research: statistically significant results can often be attributed to selective reporting of results within studies, or “p-hacking” (Ioannidis, 2005; Simonsohn, Nelson,
    [Show full text]
  • Using Natural Experiments to Study the Impact of Media on the Family
    JOSEPH PRICE Brigham Young University GORDON B. DAHL University of California, San Diego* Using Natural Experiments to Study the Impact of Media on the Family The randomized trial is the gold standard in including television, movies, print and digital scientific research and is used by several fields advertisements, and the Internet (Pergrams & to study the effects of media. Although useful Zaradic, 2006). Some of these media may have for studying the immediate response to media negative effects, whereas other types of media exposure, the experimental approach is not well may have positive effects. Social scientists from suited to studying long-term effects or behavior a variety of disciplines have used different outside the laboratory. The ‘‘natural experi- techniques to model and estimate these impacts. ment’’ approach, a quasi-experimental method In this paper, we discuss what economists commonly used by economists, exploits settings have learned about these questions through in which there is naturally occurring variation the use of ‘‘natural experiments.’’ We view that ‘‘randomly’’ influences the amount and type the economist’s approach as complementary to of media that individuals are exposed to. We dis- those in other disciplines (and also recognize cuss the methodological issues of natural exper- its occasional use by other disciplines). A iments and the ways in which findings based primary aim of this article is to encourage a on this approach can complement the findings more interdisciplinary approach to evaluating from laboratory or other experimental settings. the impact of the media on the family. The examples we discuss highlight the value of The gold standard in scientific research is interdisciplinary work in better understanding the randomized controlled trial.
    [Show full text]
  • Nber Working Paper Series Regression Discontinuity
    NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES REGRESSION DISCONTINUITY DESIGNS IN ECONOMICS David S. Lee Thomas Lemieux Working Paper 14723 http://www.nber.org/papers/w14723 NATIONAL BUREAU OF ECONOMIC RESEARCH 1050 Massachusetts Avenue Cambridge, MA 02138 February 2009 We thank David Autor, David Card, John DiNardo, Guido Imbens, and Justin McCrary for suggestions for this article, as well as for numerous illuminating discussions on the various topics we cover in this review. We also thank two anonymous referees for their helpful suggestions and comments, and Mike Geruso, Andrew Marder, and Zhuan Pei for their careful reading of earlier drafts. Diane Alexander, Emily Buchsbaum, Elizabeth Debraggio, Enkeleda Gjeci, Ashley Hodgson, Yan Lau, Pauline Leung, and Xiaotong Niu provided excellent research assistance. The views expressed herein are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Bureau of Economic Research. © 2009 by David S. Lee and Thomas Lemieux. All rights reserved. Short sections of text, not to exceed two paragraphs, may be quoted without explicit permission provided that full credit, including © notice, is given to the source. Regression Discontinuity Designs in Economics David S. Lee and Thomas Lemieux NBER Working Paper No. 14723 February 2009, Revised October 2009 JEL No. C1,H0,I0,J0 ABSTRACT This paper provides an introduction and "user guide" to Regression Discontinuity (RD) designs for empirical researchers. It presents the basic theory behind the research design, details when RD is likely to be valid or invalid given economic incentives, explains why it is considered a "quasi-experimental" design, and summarizes different ways (with their advantages and disadvantages) of estimating RD designs and the limitations of interpreting these estimates.
    [Show full text]
  • Methods to Address Self-Selection and Reverse Causation in Studies of Neighborhood Environments and Brain Health
    International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health Review Methods to Address Self-Selection and Reverse Causation in Studies of Neighborhood Environments and Brain Health Lilah M. Besser 1,*, Willa D. Brenowitz 2, Oanh L. Meyer 3, Serena Hoermann 4 and John Renne 4 1 Department of Urban and Regional Planning, Institute for Human Health and Disease Intervention (I-HEALTH), Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, FL 33431, USA 2 Departments of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and Epidemiology & Biostatistics, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA 94121, USA; [email protected] 3 Department of Neurology, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA; [email protected] 4 Center for Urban and Environmental Solutions (CUES), Department of Urban and Regional Planning, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, FL 33431, USA; [email protected] (S.H.); [email protected] (J.R.) * Correspondence: [email protected] Abstract: Preliminary evidence suggests that neighborhood environments, such as socioeconomic disadvantage, pedestrian and physical activity infrastructure, and availability of neighborhood desti- nations (e.g., parks), may be associated with late-life cognitive functioning and risk of Alzheimer’s disease and related disorders (ADRD). The supposition is that these neighborhood characteristics are associated with factors such as mental health, environmental exposures, health behaviors, and social determinants of health that in turn promote or diminish cognitive reserve and resilience in later life. However, observed associations may be biased by self-selection or reverse causation, such as when individuals with better cognition move to denser neighborhoods because they prefer many Citation: Besser, L.M.; Brenowitz, destinations within walking distance of home, or when individuals with deteriorating health choose W.D.; Meyer, O.L.; Hoermann, S.; residences offering health services in neighborhoods in rural or suburban areas (e.g., assisted living).
    [Show full text]
  • Natural Experiment Methods and COVID-19 Research
    Natural Experiment Methods and COVID-19 Research Bernie Black 2020.09 Next-D2 Stakeholder Virtual Meeting (Sept. 2020) 1 Underlying goal in any observational study • Observe only treated outcome y1 for treated units • Observe only control outcome y0 for control units • Use control units to predict missing potential outcome for treated units • Compute ATT (average treatment effect for the treated) • y1 (observed) – y0 (if had been control), for the treated • Sometimes also study ATE (average effect for everyone) • Goal: Approximate a randomized experiment • Then can use control mean to predict missing mean potential outcome for treated: • E[y0|treated] (unobserved) = E[y0|control] (observed) • Try to acheive: Random assignment to treatment, conditional on x • Different names: Unconfounded assignment, strongly ignorable assignment, SOO, CIA • E[y0|treated, x] (unobserved) = E[y0|control, x] (observed) • If goal achieved: Randomized block experiment [block on x] 2 Exogenous Shock • “Natural experiment” provides an exogenous shock • Hopefully: divides sample into treated vs. control units in as-if- random manner. Several possible designs, in different settings: • Difference-in-differences (DiD) • Shock-based instrumental variables (shock-IV) • Regression discontinuity (RD) • Combined methods • Sometimes (if shock is large and clearly exogenous): • Before vs. after the shock: Interrupted time series (ITS) • COVID-19 is a large shock, so ITS can be credible • ITS with fast onset of treatment effect RD, which can be highly credible 3 To move
    [Show full text]
  • Estimation of Treatment Effects Under Endogenous Heteroskedasticity*
    ESTIMATION OF TREATMENT EFFECTS UNDER ENDOGENOUS HETEROSKEDASTICITY* JASON ABREVAYAy AND HAIQING XUz ABSTRACT. This paper considers a treatment effects model in which individual treatment effects may be heterogeneous, even among observationally identical individuals. Specifically, by extend- ing the classical instrumental-variables (IV) model with an endogenous binary treatment, the heteroskedasticity of the error disturbance is allowed to depend upon the treatment variable so that treatment generates both mean and variance effects on the outcome. In this endogenous heteroskedasticity IV (EHIV) model, the standard IV estimator can be inconsistent for the average treatment effects (ATE) and lead to incorrect inference. After nonparametric identification is established, closed-form estimators are provided for the linear EHIV of the mean and variance treatment effects, and the average treatment effect on the treated (ATT). Asymptotic properties of the estimators are derived. A Monte Carlo simulation investigates the performance of the proposed approach. An empirical application regarding the effects of fertility on female labor supply is considered, and the findings demonstrate the importance of accounting for endogenous heteroskedasticity. Keywords: Endogenous heteroskedasticity, individual treatment effects, average treatment effects, local average treatment effects, instrumental variable Date: Friday 23rd August, 2019. ∗We thank Daniel Ackerberg, Sandra Black, Ivan Canay, Salvador Navarro, Max Stinchcombe, Quang Vuong, and Ed Vytlacil for useful comments. We also thank seminar participants at University of Iowa, University of Hong Kong, McMaster University, Western University, University of Texas at Austin, Xiamen University, Monash University, University of Melbourne, USC, the 2017 Shanghai workshop of econometrics at SUFE, the 2018 Texas Econometrics Camp, and the 2018 CEME conference at Duke University.
    [Show full text]
  • A Natural Experiment of the EU Enlargement to the East
    A Service of Leibniz-Informationszentrum econstor Wirtschaft Leibniz Information Centre Make Your Publications Visible. zbw for Economics Martínez-Zarzoso, Inmaculada; Voicu, Anca M.; Vidovic, Martina Working Paper Production networks in Europe: A natural experiment of the EU enlargement to the east cege Discussion Papers, No. 390 Provided in Cooperation with: Georg August University of Göttingen, cege - Center for European, Governance and Economic Development Research Suggested Citation: Martínez-Zarzoso, Inmaculada; Voicu, Anca M.; Vidovic, Martina (2020) : Production networks in Europe: A natural experiment of the EU enlargement to the east, cege Discussion Papers, No. 390, University of Göttingen, Center for European, Governance and Economic Development Research (cege), Göttingen This Version is available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10419/215744 Standard-Nutzungsbedingungen: Terms of use: Die Dokumente auf EconStor dürfen zu eigenen wissenschaftlichen Documents in EconStor may be saved and copied for your Zwecken und zum Privatgebrauch gespeichert und kopiert werden. personal and scholarly purposes. Sie dürfen die Dokumente nicht für öffentliche oder kommerzielle You are not to copy documents for public or commercial Zwecke vervielfältigen, öffentlich ausstellen, öffentlich zugänglich purposes, to exhibit the documents publicly, to make them machen, vertreiben oder anderweitig nutzen. publicly available on the internet, or to distribute or otherwise use the documents in public. Sofern die Verfasser die Dokumente unter Open-Content-Lizenzen (insbesondere CC-Lizenzen) zur Verfügung gestellt haben sollten, If the documents have been made available under an Open gelten abweichend von diesen Nutzungsbedingungen die in der dort Content Licence (especially Creative Commons Licences), you genannten Lizenz gewährten Nutzungsrechte. may exercise further usage rights as specified in the indicated licence.
    [Show full text]