Chapter 13: Social Psychology

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Chapter 13: Social Psychology Psyc 100 – Introduction to Psychology Other Social Influence: Overview • How does the presence of others affect Chapter 13: Social performance? Psychology • Social influences on altruism Module 13.3: Social Influence: Conformity, • The power of the group Compliance, Obedience •Conformity Psychology 100 • Group decision-making Introduction to Psychology • The power of authority: Obedience • Milgram’s obedience experiment Listen to the audio lecture while viewing these slides • The role of culture 1 2 Psyc 100 – Introduction to Psychology Psyc 100 – Introduction to Psychology How Does the Presence of Others Affect Performance? Social Influences on Altruism • In the presence of others, performance can be • Altruism: Acting in a way that shows unselfish affected in several ways: concern for the welfare of others • Social facilitation: Performance enhanced • Bystander effect: Reluctance to come to • Social interference: Performance impaired someone’s aid when others are present • Interacts with task difficulty • Famous example: Kitty Genovese case • Others help spur performance of easy tasks, but • Diffusion of responsibility: Allowing sense of they hinder performance of new or difficult ones responsibility to spread out among those • May happen becaus e pres ence o f ot hers rais es present arousal • Derives from belief that others have already • Recall: Performance is best when arousal is not too done something to help or soon will low (as for easy tasks) or too high (as for difficult • The more witnesses, the less likely any single one ones) will help 3 4 Psyc 100 – Introduction to Psychology Psyc 100 – Introduction to Psychology Conformity The Power of the Group • Social loafing: Tendency to put in less • Tendency to comply with the wishes of the group effort when working in a group than • Opinions, feelings, behaviors when working alone generally move toward the • May be connected to the bystander norm • Especially likely when pressure effect, diffusion of responsibility comes from in-group, or • Deindividuation: Loss of individuality individuals with whom you that comes from being in a group identify • Can be extreme, as in Asch’s • Example: People might do things when in (1951, 1955) studies: a large, rowdy group that they would • Confederate subjects lied never do alone about easy visual judgments • Many subjects conformed to • May also relate to diffusion of these wrong judgments responsibility 5 6 1 Psyc 100 – Introduction to Psychology Psyc 100 – Introduction to Psychology Fig. 13.7 Group Decision-Making • Group polarization: Tendency for a group’s dominant point of view to become stronger, more extreme with time • May relate to conformity, wish to be liked by other group members • Groupthink: Tendency for members to become so interested in seeking consensus that they ignore, suppress, dissenting views • Can be countered by encouraging 7 dissent, awareness of the tendency 8 Psyc 100 – Introduction to Psychology Psyc 100 – Introduction to Psychology Compliance: Getting Others to Respond to Requests The Power of Authority: Obedience • foot–in–door (small req. followed by • Form of compliance that occurs when people respond to orders of larger one; self-perception) an authority figure • Why do people often carry out • door–in–face (large req. followed by extreme or horrifying orders, for moderate one; reciprocity) example during war? • Milgram (1963) designed an • attraction/liking (thus similarity, experiment to test the resistance attractiveness, etc.) of average people to authority when demands are extreme • low–ball (changing the riles of the game • Experimenter ordered participants after it starts; similar to “bait and to administer increasing levels of shock to an unwilling “victim” switch”) •IMPORTANT: “Victim”was a confederate; all shocks were fake 9 10 PsycObedience 100 – Introduction to Psychology Psyc 100 – Introduction to Psychology E = Experimenter (originally, Milgram himself) Fig. 13.9 S = The Subject L = The Learner (actually a confederate of E) E SL Fig. 13.8 11 12 2 Psyc 100 – Introduction to Psychology Psyc 100 – Introduction to Psychology Results of the Milgram Experiment The Role of Culture • Over half delivered “shocks” up to the • Western cultures promote an maximum level, despite realistic protests independent view of the self and screams of the “victim” • Leads to devaluing conformity, • Ethical concerns: obedience, altruism • Participants clearly distressed • Many non-Western cultures promote an interdependent view • Were debriefed, however, and long-term effects were minor • Example: Japanese culture strongly emphasizes belonging to, contributing to • What we learn from it: Under certain a collective circumstances, average people will obey extreme orders 13 14 3.
Recommended publications
  • Being an Active Bystander
    “…DID THEY REALLY JUST SAY THAT?!” BEING AN ACTIVE BYSTANDER Lena Tenney, MPA, MEd. | Researcher & Facilitation Specialist The Ohio State University | August 29, 2017 YOU ARE NOT ALONE! • In the last 2 months, have you experienced or observed a comment that made you uncomfortable or was inappropriate? – 62% yes – 38% no • Did anyone intervene? – 20% yes – 70% no YOU ARE NOT ALONE! • If you chose not to intervene, please select why: – 11% fear for safety – 6% fear of judgement – 46% did not know what to do or say – 31% did not feel comfortable intervening Outcomes for Student all students engagement NCLUSIVE I NVIRONMENTS OF E Innovation in Cognitive problem complexity in solving in problem team ENEFITS solving environments B EARNING L Milem and Hakuta, 2000; Gurin et al., 2002; Hurtado et al., 2003; Milem, 2003; Antonio et al., 2004; Page, 2007; Page, 2010 GOALS FOR THIS SESSION • Gain an understanding of the bystander effect and what it means to be an active bystander • Illustrate the importance and benefits of committing to being an active bystander • Learn how to effectively communicate in challenging situations through strategies that can be tailored to the particular situation • Practice being an active bystander ABOUT THE KIRWAN INSTITUTE Rigorous & Robust Applied Community Research Engagement Image Credit WHAT DOES IT MEAN TO BE AN ACTIVE BYSTANDER? THE BYSTANDER EFFECT • Diffusion of responsibility – The more people who are present, the less likely that someone will take action during a situation • Social influence – Groups monitor behavior, individuals don’t want to violate norms Darley, J.M., Latane, B. (1968).
    [Show full text]
  • Exploring the Perception of Nationalism in the United States and Saudi Arabia Reem Mohammed Alhethail Eastern Washington University
    Eastern Washington University EWU Digital Commons EWU Masters Thesis Collection Student Research and Creative Works 2015 Exploring the perception of nationalism in the United States and Saudi Arabia Reem Mohammed Alhethail Eastern Washington University Follow this and additional works at: http://dc.ewu.edu/theses Part of the Islamic World and Near East History Commons, and the United States History Commons Recommended Citation Alhethail, Reem Mohammed, "Exploring the perception of nationalism in the United States and Saudi Arabia" (2015). EWU Masters Thesis Collection. 330. http://dc.ewu.edu/theses/330 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Research and Creative Works at EWU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in EWU Masters Thesis Collection by an authorized administrator of EWU Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. EXPLORING THE PERCEPTION OF NATIONALISM IN THE UNITED STATES AND SAUDI ARABIA A Thesis Presented To Eastern Washington University Cheney, WA In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements For the Degree Master of Arts in History By Reem Mohammed Alhethail Fall 2015 ii THESIS OF REEM MOHAMMED ALHETHAIL APPROVED BY DATE ROBERT SAUDERS, GRADUATE STUDY COMMITTEE DATE MICHAEL CONLIN, GRADUATE STUDY COMMITTEE DATE CHADRON HAZELBAKER, GRADUATE STUDY COMMITTEE iii MASTER’S THESIS In presenting this thesis in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a master’s degree at Eastern Washington University, I agree that (your library) shall make copies freely available for inspection. I further agree that copying of this project in whole or in part is allowable only for scholarly purposes.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Destructiveness and Authority: the Milgram Experiments and the Perpetration of Genocide
    W&M ScholarWorks Dissertations, Theses, and Masters Projects Theses, Dissertations, & Master Projects 1995 Human Destructiveness and Authority: The Milgram Experiments and the Perpetration of Genocide Steven Lee Lobb College of William & Mary - Arts & Sciences Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wm.edu/etd Part of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Commons, Political Science Commons, and the Social Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Lobb, Steven Lee, "Human Destructiveness and Authority: The Milgram Experiments and the Perpetration of Genocide" (1995). Dissertations, Theses, and Masters Projects. Paper 1539625988. https://dx.doi.org/doi:10.21220/s2-yeze-bv41 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Theses, Dissertations, & Master Projects at W&M ScholarWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations, Theses, and Masters Projects by an authorized administrator of W&M ScholarWorks. For more information, please contact [email protected]. HUMAN DESTRUCTIVENESS AND AUTHORITY: THE MILGRAM EXPERIMENTS AND THE PERPETRATION OF GENOCIDE A Thesis Presented to The Faculty of the Department of Government The College of William and Mary in Virginia In Partial Fulfillment Of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts by Steve Lobb 1995 APPROVAL SHEET This thesis is submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts Steve Lobb Approved, November 1995 L )\•y ^ . Roger Smith .onald Rapi limes Miclot i i TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Wrong Tomorrow, Wrong Yesterday, but Not Today: on Sliding Into Evil with Zeal but Without Understanding
    Symposium Wrong Tomorrow, Wrong Yesterday, but not Today: On Sliding into Evil with Zeal but without Understanding Robert A. Burt* Collaboration with systems of evil-the overall topic of this Symposium-is a problem as fresh as contemporary news ac­ counts. The New York Times recently carried a front-page story about the dilemmas described by young Serbs who were evading military conscription because they understood the evil ofSlobodan Milosevic's genocidal policies against the Kosovar Albanians but nonetheless felt a patriotic duty to protect their country against foreign assaults. As one young man put it, '''we'd like to see [Milosevic] hanging.' But ... '[i]fthe guys from NATO come here, we will shoot them.'"! As wrenching as this kind of dilemma may be, it is not the problem ofcollaboration that I want to discuss. My concern is with a more difficult problem, as I see it-a problem that is more insidious and difficult to identify as such. The young Ser­ bian resister knows that his President is an evil man, and he feels the moral conflict between his revulsion at this evil and his patri­ otic impulses. My concern is for circumstances where the evil is not understood as such by its perpetrators, where they are una­ ware at the time they are acting of the wrongfulness of the actions in which they are engaged. * Alexander M. Bickel Professor of Law, Yale University School of Law. 1. Steven Erlanger, Serb Conscripts Drift into Hiding: Assert Patriotism While Using Safe Houses to AvoidArmy, N.Y. Times, Apr. 5, 1999, at A1, available in LEXIS, News Library, New York Times File.
    [Show full text]
  • Aversive Racism and the Responsibility of Bystanders Audrey J
    The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available on Emerald Insight at: https://www.emerald.com/insight/2040-7149.htm Aversive Why someone did not stop them? racism Aversive racism and the responsibility of bystanders Audrey J. Murrell College of Business Administration, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, USA Received 8 July 2020 Revised 31 July 2020 Accepted 2 August 2020 Abstract Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to examine whether the impact of persistent racial bias, discrimination and racial violence is facilitated by otherwise well-intentioned individuals who fail to act or intercede. Utilizing the aversive racism framework, the need to move beyond awareness raising to facilitate behavioral changes is discussed. Examining the unique lens provided by the aversive racism framework and existing research, the bystander effect provides important insights on recent acts of racial violence such as the murder of Mr. George Floyd. Some promise is shown by the work on effective bystander behavior training and highlights the need for shared responsibility in preventing the outcomes of racial violence and discrimination to create meaningful and long-lasting social change. Design/methodology/approach – This paper uses literature based on the aversive racism framework together with the literature on the bystander effect to understand the factors, conditions and consequences for lack of intervention when the victim is African American. This paper also provides evidence and theory-based recommendations for strategies to change passive bystanders into active allies. Findings – The use of the aversive racism framework provides a powerful lens to help explain the inconsistencies in the bystander effect based on the race of the victim.
    [Show full text]
  • MILGRAM, METHOD and MORALITY By
    1 MILGRAM, METHOD AND MORALITY By Charles R. Pigden and Grant R. Gillet1 1. The Milgram Experiments In the early 1960s, Stanley Milgram, a social psychologist at Yale, recruited a set of experimental subjects, by advertising in the New Haven papers. They came from all walks of life, though in the first series of experiments all of them were male2. The subjects were promised a modest fee ($4.00 - not a princely sum even in those days) plus a 50c carfare. It is important to remember what a modest fee this was. It was the sort of sum most of Milgram's subjects could have returned without a qualm. (It is not as if they were coerced by economic necessity into carrying on with the experiment to the bitter end.) The experiment was officially about learning and memory. Each recruit was introduced to another man, ostensibly a recruit like himself, but in fact an accomplice of the experimenter. One of them was to be a 'learner', the other a 'teacher', the roles to be determined by lot. In fact the procedure was rigged so that the real recruit always ended up as the 'teacher'. The 'learner' was wired up to a fake electric chair and set to perform a series of mnemonic tasks. He soon began to make 'mistakes'. Every time the 'learner' made a mistake, the 'teacher' was supposed to deliver an 'electric shock'. These were increased in power as the number of errors increased. The teacher experienced a real shock at the outset just so as he would know how the lowest intensity felt - unpleasant but not really painful.
    [Show full text]
  • A Comprehensive Study About the Bystander Effect
    Volume 10 Issue 1 (2021) Why People Stand By: A Comprehensive Study About the Bystander Effect Nitya Ganti1,2 and Sori Baek2 1Basis Independent Silicon Valley, San Jose, CA, USA 2Horizon Academics, Jersey City, NJ, USA ABSTRACT Bystander effect is the phenomenon that describes how, when more people are around, each individual is less likely to intervene. While the bystander effect is an integral part of studying social behaviors and group thinking, the many caveats it presents itself with must be considered. Every situation differs based on location, people, and circumstance, so the idea of the bystander effect is not valid in every scenario, as evidenced by the various counterexamples and contradictory findings researchers have discovered. However, the bystander effect is still very important to study because understanding what encourages/prevents people from helping is critical to decrease the effect of the bystander effect to promote helping behavior. In this paper, we discuss the various factors that affect the prevalence of the bystander effect: perceived physical and social harm to the helper, responsibility diffusion, and perceived helpfulness. Introduction In Queens, New York in 1964, 28-year-old Kitty Genovese was raped and murdered in front of 38 nearby-apartment dwellers (Geyer-Schulz, Ovelgönne, & Sonnenbichler, 2010). Though facts are disputed, it is largely agreed that the first person to call the police as they witnessed this gruesome attack did so over 45 minutes after the incident began. By then, Kitty was already dead. The question of why or how these events could have happened is a timeless one. With so many witnesses, how could such a brutal criminal act go unreported? The answer lies in three words: the bystander effect.
    [Show full text]
  • Bandwagon Effect: Lesson Plan
    Bandwagon Effect: Hop On! Bandwagon Effect: Lesson Plan Topic The bandwagon effect is the psychological phenomenon in which an individual does something because others, particularly a large group of others, are doing it. This action or behavior is done regardless of whether it aligns with the individual’s personal belief or even factual evidence. Possible subjects/classes Time needed ● Psychology ● English 30-45 minutes ● Economics/Marketing ● Politics Video link: https://academy4sc.org/topic/bandwagon-effect-hop-on/ Objective: What will students know/be able to do at the end of class? By the end of this lesson, students will be able to ● Define the bandwagon effect. ● Explain how and why the bandwagon effect functions in every social field. ● Combat the bandwagon effect in their daily lives. Key Concepts & Vocabulary Conformity, bandwagaon Materials Needed A popular song and device to play music for the introductory activity Before you watch If you can get student confidants in on your plan, play popular, well-known music in your classroom. Encourage students to sing along. You’ll need student confidants because students are highly unlikely to actually sing in your classroom without outside motivation. Your confidants should be able to attract students not in on the plan to join in. Bandwagon Effect: Hop On! If you fail to get students to sing, whether your confidants did not attract students or you decided against using confidants, this can also be learning experience. Regardless of the results, question students about what just happened. Why did no one or few people sing at the beginning? Encourage students to think beyond simple answers like “I didn’t want to” or “I didn’t know the lyrics” to deeper responses.
    [Show full text]
  • 11 Social Influence
    9781405124003_4_011.qxd 10/31/07 3:09 PM Page 216 Social Influence 11 Miles Hewstone and Robin Martin KEY CONCEPTS autokinetic effect compliance consistency conversion deindividuation door-in-the-face technique evaluation apprehension foot-in-the-door technique group polarization groupthink informational influence lowballing technique majority influence (conformity) minority influence (innovation) norms normative influence obedience to authority referent informational influence self-categorization theory social comparison social facilitation social influence whistleblowing 9781405124003_4_011.qxd 10/31/07 3:09 PM Page 217 CHAPTER OUTLINE This chapter considers two main types of social influence, both of which can be understood in terms of fundamental motives. First, we discuss ‘incidental’ social influence, where people are influenced by the presence or implied presence of others, although there has been no explicit attempt to influence them. We consider the impact of the mere presence of other people on task performance, and the impact of social norms. In the second part of the chapter, we ask why people succumb to social influence, highlighting types of social influence and motives underlying influence on the part of the target of influence. In the third part of the chapter, we turn to ‘deliberate’ social influence. We introduce theory and research on compliance, the influence of numerical majorities and minorities, group decision-making and obedience. Throughout we will see that social influence is an ambivalent concept. On the one hand, it is the glue of society: it makes things work, and society would be utterly chaotic without it. But on the other hand it can be a dark force, underlying some of the most extreme, even immoral, forms of human social behaviour.
    [Show full text]
  • Conformity and the Groupthink Mentality
    Conformity and the Groupthink Mentality What is “groupthink?” Groupthink, a term coined by social psychologist Irving Janis (1972), occurs when a group makes faulty decisions because group pressures lead to a deterioration of “mental efficiency, reality testing, and moral judgment” (9). Groups affected by groupthink ignore alternatives and tend to take irrational actions that dehumanize other groups. A group is especially vulnerable to groupthink when its members are similar in background, when the group is insulated from outside opinions, and when there are no clear rules for decision making. (emphasis added) Janis, Irving L. (1972). Victims of Groupthink. New York: Houghton Mifflin. Question 1: How do the underlined portions of the text above contribute to oppression, discrimination or the mistreatment of others? Explain in the space below. Groupthink is one of the most troublesome downfalls of organized society. Today, it manifests itself on a sliding scale of severity, ranging from genocide to bullying to superstition to fashion fads to the “Digg mentality” of news reporting. Still, most of us refuse to believe that our opinions, perception and worldview are being in any way shaped by those of others. And yet they are. Even subcultures, the very essence of which is to stand out, are founded on group conformity — or, as James Thurber famously puts it, “why do you have to be a nonconformist like everyone else?” What’s perhaps most interesting about conformity is how our own relationship to it changes throughout the course of our lives. We spend our teenage years trying, desperately, to fit in, only to mature into trying, just as desperately, to stand out — a point eloquently echoed by one Etsy employee in his recent contribution to the tremendously important It Gets Better Project.
    [Show full text]
  • The Neurobiology of Groupthink: a Qeeg Approach to the Study of Followership
    Pepperdine University Pepperdine Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations 2016 The neurobiology of groupthink: a qEEG approach to the study of followership Angela A. Deulen Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.pepperdine.edu/etd Recommended Citation Deulen, Angela A., "The neurobiology of groupthink: a qEEG approach to the study of followership" (2016). Theses and Dissertations. 628. https://digitalcommons.pepperdine.edu/etd/628 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by Pepperdine Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Pepperdine Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected], [email protected]. Pepperdine University Graduate School of Education and Psychology THE NEUROBIOLOGY OF GROUPTHINK: A qEEG APPROACH TO THE STUDY OF FOLLOWERSHIP A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Education in Organizational Leadership by Angela A. Deulen April, 2016 Kent Rhodes, Ed.D. – Dissertation Chairperson This dissertation, written by Angela A Deulen under the guidance of a Faculty Committee and approved by its members, has been submitted to and accepted by the Graduate Faculty in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF EDUCATION Doctoral Committee: Kent Rhodes, Ed.D., Chairperson John Tobin, J.D. Joseph Pelletier, Ph.D. © Copyright by Angela A. Deulen (2016) All Rights Reserved
    [Show full text]
  • Irving L. Janis' Victims of Groupthink Author(S): Paul't Hart Reviewed Work(S): Source: Political Psychology, Vol
    Irving L. Janis' Victims of Groupthink Author(s): Paul't Hart Reviewed work(s): Source: Political Psychology, Vol. 12, No. 2 (Jun., 1991), pp. 247-278 Published by: International Society of Political Psychology Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3791464 . Accessed: 17/02/2012 11:40 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. International Society of Political Psychology is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Political Psychology. http://www.jstor.org Political Psychology, Vol. 12, No. 2, 1991 Classics in Political Psychology Irving L. Janis' Victims of Groupthink Paul 't Hart1 INTRODUCTION Victims of Groupthink:A Psychological Study of Foreign Policy Decisions and Fiascoes by Irving L. Janis was published for the first time in 1972. In an unprecedentedway, Janisapplied ideas from small-groupanalysis to the explana- tion of policy fiascoes. He made plausible the hypothesis that each of these events can, to a considerable extent, be attributedto the occurrence of a very specific and obviously detrimentalphenomenon within the groups of decision- makers involved in their making. He called this phenomenon "Groupthink," cleverly picking this highly suggestive Orwellian mode of expression ("dou- blethink" in Orwell's novel 1984).
    [Show full text]