City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works

All Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects

9-2016

Physiological Regulation, Psychosocial Adversity, and Proactive Versus Reactive Aggression: A Longitudinal Study

Wei Zhang The Graduate Center, City University of New York

How does access to this work benefit ou?y Let us know!

More information about this work at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/gc_etds/1563 Discover additional works at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu

This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY). Contact: [email protected] PHYSIOLOGICAL REGULATION, PSYCHOSOCIAL ADVERSITY, AND PROACTIVE

VERSUS REACTIVE AGGRESSION: A LONGITUDINAL STUDY

by

WEI ZHANG

A dissertation submitted to the Graduate Faculty in Psychology in partial fulfillment of the

requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy,

The City University of New York

2016

© 2016

WEI ZHANG

All Rights Reserved

ii

Physiological Regulation, Psychosocial Adversity, and Proactive Versus Reactive Aggression: A Longitudinal Study

By

Wei Zhang

This manuscript has been read and accepted for the Graduate Faculty in Psychology in satisfaction of the dissertation requirement for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.

______Yu Gao, PhD

Date Chair of Examining Committee

______Richard Bodnar, PhD

Date Executive Officer, Psychology

Supervisory Committee:

Deborah J. Walder, PhD

Jennifer E. Drake, PhD

Elisabeth Brauner, PhD

Erika Y. Niwa, PhD

The City University of New Yok iii

ABSTRACT

Physiological Regulation, Psychosocial Adversity, and Proactive Versus Reactive Aggression: A

Longitudinal Study

By

Wei Zhang

Advisor: Yu Gao, PhD

Two types of aggression in children and adolescents have been identified: reactive aggression

(RA) and proactive aggression (PA). Despite the accumulating evidence suggesting differential

temperamental, behavioral, cognitive, social-environmental, and neurobiological correlates in re- lation to the two types of aggression, no study has examined emotion regulation in children with

RA vs. PA using psychophysiological approaches. In this study a sample of eight to 10 years old

children participated in an emotion regulation task in which they were required to either induce

or inhibit their emotions. They also reported their aggressive behavior using the Reactive-Proac- tive Aggression questionnaire (RPQ; Raine et al., 2006). Electrocardiogram and respiration were assessed continuously during a 2-min rest period and the emotion regulation task and were used to derive the baseline respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) and RSA reactivity, respectively. Both

RSA measures and aggressive behavior were assessed again during the one-year follow-up visit.

The aims of the study were threefold: First, we aimed to examine if emotion dysregulation, as reflected by low baseline RSA and/or reduced RSA reactivity, would be differentially associated with RA and PA. Second, the study examined the interaction effects between baseline RSA and

RSA reactivity in relation to the two types of aggression. Finally, we explored the moderating effects of psychosocial risk factors on the RSA-aggression associations. Concurrent correlations

iv

between RSA, aggression, and social adversity measures at each time point were first examined, and the predictive relations between these measures from Time 1 to Time 2 were then deter- mined using multiple regressions and ANCOVAs. Results showed that after PA was controlled for, high RA was associated with autonomic hyperarousal, i.e., higher baseline RSA and in- creased RSA reactivity, in boys at Time 1 only. No such effect was found in girls. By contrast, high PA was associated with hypoarousal, i.e., reduced RSA reactivity, only in the conditions of low social adversity and no moderating effect of gender was found. Together, these findings pro- vide initial evidence that the RA and PA are characterized by different patterns of the parasym- pathetic–mediated emotional regulation processes and that both neurobiological and psychoso- cial influences are important in understanding the etiology of aggressive behavior in children and adolescents.

Keywords: emotion regulation, respiratory sinus arrhythmia, social adversity, reactive ag- gression, proactive aggression

v

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Completion of this doctoral dissertation has been one of the most remarkable academic accomplishments of my life. This work could not have been possible without constant support

from certain people. I owe my deepest gratitude to them.

First, I would like to sincerely thank my advisor, Dr. Yu Gao. It has been an honor for me to be her first PhD student. I deeply appreciate her generosity to provide me with time, knowledge, and funding to make my PhD experience creative, focused and productive. I remain

very grateful to her for the excellent example she set before me as a successful woman and pro-

fessor.

My gratitude must go to all current and prior members of Gao’s Psychophysiology Lab

with whom I have interacted during my graduate study. They have always been the important

source of friendship as well as support and collaboration. I am especially gratefully to Sarah Ba-

kanosky, Medina Mishiyeva, Shawn Fagan, and Yonglin Huang for their contribution to various

domains of my dissertation project.

I am very grateful to Dr. Deborah Walder and Dr. Jennifer Drake for their insightful com-

ments and constructive criticism during the development of this dissertation. I would also like to

thank the other two members of my oral defense committee, Dr. Elisabeth Brauner and Dr. Erika

Niwa for their time and valuable advice. Special thanks go to Dr. David Owen for his continuous

encouragement and guidance.

I am deeply indebted to the Department of Psychology at Brooklyn College and the Grad-

uate Center, CUNY. I would like to express my full appreciation to their staff for their whole-

hearted support.

vi

Moreover, I wish to thank my best friend Sixuan Liu for her care, support, and encour-

agement that helped me to overcome obstacles and to stay strong during these years. I would also like to thank Beliz Hazan for being a staunch and fierce friend who stood by me during these years.

Last but not least, I am extremely grateful to my father Bingrong Zhang and my mother

Huilan Yi, who have always put faith in my endeavors. None of this would have been possible without their unconditional love and confidence in me.

vii

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ABSTRACT ...... IV

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ...... VI

TABLE OF CONTENTS ...... VIII

LIST OF TABLES ...... X

LIST OF FIGURES ...... XI

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS ...... XII

CHAPTER 1 - REACTIVE AND PROACTIVE AGGRESSION ...... 1

Introduction ...... 1

Reactive and Proactive Aggression ...... 2

Psychosocial Differences of RA and PA ...... 4

Neurobiological Differences of RA and PA ...... 10

Chapter Summary ...... 18

CHAPTER 2 - EMOTION REGULATION ...... 20

Emotion Regulation ...... 20

Measurements of Emotion Regulation...... 21

Neurobiological Mechanisms of Emotion Regulation ...... 24

Chapter Summary ...... 33

CHAPTER 3 - PHYSIOLOGICAL REGULATION AND SUBTYPES OF AGGRESSION .....35

PNS Activity, Emotion Regulation and Subtypes of Aggression ...... 35

Joint Effects of Baseline RSA and RSA Reactivity...... 37

Biosocial Interaction ...... 37

viii

Moderating Effect of Gender ...... 39

Chapter Summary ...... 40

CHAPTER 4 – CURRENT STUDY ...... 41

Method ...... 42

Results ...... 50

Discussion ...... 61

Limitations and Future Direction ...... 65

APPENDIX A: RPQ ...... 73

APPENDIX B: SOCIAL ADVERSITY INDEX ...... 75

REFERENCES ...... 77

ix

List of Tables

Table 1. Psychosocial Differences between RA and PA ...... 9

Table 2. Neurobiological Differences between RA and PA ...... 18

Table 3. Missing Data and Outliers for Each Time Point Separately and for the Combined

Dataset...... 47

Table 4. Correlations between Main Study Variables at Each Time Point ...... 51

Table 5. Summary of Hierarchical Regressions for RA and PA: Predicting Effects of Baseline

RSA, RSA Reactivity, Social Adversity and Their Interactions ...... 52

Table 6. Correlation between Time 1 and Time 2 Variables ...... 55

Table 7. Baseline RSA Group Comparisons on RSA, Sociodemographic and Aggression

Measures ...... 57

Table 8. Summary of Hierarchical Regression for T2 RA/PA: Predicting Effects of T1 and T2

Baseline RSA, and SA and Their Interactions ...... 58

Table 9. RSA Reactivity Group Comparisons in RSA, Sociodemographic Data, and Aggression

Measures ...... 59

Table 10. Summary of Hierarchical Regression for T2 RA/PA: Predicting Effects of T1 and T2

RSA reactivity, and SA and Their Interactions ...... 60

x

List of Figures

Figure 1. Main Cortical (e.g., MFC, DLFC, ACC, vmPFC, OFC) and Subcortical Regions (e.g., amygdala) Involved in Emotion Regulation ...... 26

Figure 2. The Interaction Effect between Baseline RSA and RSA Reactivity in Predicting Time 1

RA (controlling for PA) in Boys Only ...... 53

Figure 3. The Interaction Effect between RSA Reactivity and Social Adversity in Predicting

Time 2 PA (Combining Boys and Girls) ...... 54

Figure 4. Individual Differences in the Change Pattern of Baseline RSA from Time 1 to Time 2

(r =.50, p < .01) ...... 55

Figure 5. Individual Differences in the Change Pattern of RSA Reactivity from Time 1 to Time 2

(r = .14, p >.05)...... 56

xi

List of Abbreviations

ACC...... Anterior Cingulate Cortex

ACTH...... Adrenocorticotrophic Hormone

ADHD...... Attention-Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder

ANS...... Autonomic Nervous System

ASD...... Autism Spectrum Disorder

CRH...... Corticotrophin-releasing Hormone

CU...... Callous-Unemotional

DLFC...... Dorsolateral Frontal Cortex

EEG...... Electroencephalography

ERN...... Error-Related Negativity

ERP...... Event-Related Potential

HPA...... Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal

HR...... Heart Rate

HRV...... Heart Rate Variability

MFC...... Medial Frontal Cortex

OFC...... Orbitofrontal Cortex

PA...... Proactive Aggression

PAG...... Periaqueductal Gray

PEP...... Pre-ejection Period

PFC…...... Prefrontal Cortex

PNS…...... Parasympathetic Nervous System

xii

RA…...... …Reactive Aggression

RSA…...... Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia

RPQ…………...Reactive-Proactive Aggression Questionnaire

SC…...... …Skin Conductance

SNS…...... Sympathetic Nervous System

tDCS...... Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation

vmPFC...... Ventral Medial Prefrontal Cortex

xiii

CHAPTER 1 - REACTIVE AND PROACTIVE AGGRESSION

Introduction

Aggression in children and adolescents can have detrimental effects on school perfor-

mance, parent and peer relationship, and future adjustment problems, and it constitutes a signifi-

cant public concern for the society (Hubbard, McAuliffe, Morrow, & Romano, 2010). Prior re-

search has suggested that aggression in children accounts for 50% of child referrals for psycho-

logical services as well as 25% of all special services in school settings (Nelson & Finch, 2000).

In addition, children and adolescents with more aggressive behavior are more likely to commit

crimes when they grow up as compared with children with or without other adjustment problems

(Snyder & Sickmund, 1999). Since the 20th century, research in human aggression has flour-

ished. Besides psychosocial risk factors, recent research has also examined psychophysiological abnormality, as a potential endophenotype, to better understand the etiology of aggressive and antisocial behavior (Hinnant & El-Sheikh, 2009; Patrick, 2008; Raine, 2002). However, the ab-

normality observed in adult population may be the consequences of the aggressive behavior itself

or the manifest of environmental influences. Investigations of these abnormalities during the

childhood may maximize the chance of identifying its genetics thus further minimizing the ag- gressive behavior itself (Sterzer & Stadler, 2009). In order to provide a more holistic picture, re- cent studies have started to examine the interaction effects between psychophysiological and psychosocial influences in predicting aggression (Raine, 2002; Wilson & Scarpa, 2012).

Although research on the associations between emotion dysregulation and externalizing

behavioral problems has increased exponentially (Beauchaine, 2015a), no study has in particular

differentiated the two subtypes of aggression: one is reactive and the other is proactive. Reactive

1

Aggression (RA) is characterized by impulsive and hostile responses to provocation and frustra-

tion, and is associated with higher levels of body arousal. Conversely, Proactive Aggression (PA)

is instrumental, predatory, and goal directed, and has been linked to lower levels of arousal

(Dodge & Coie, 1987; Scarpa & Raine, 1997). The main aim of the current study was to examine the effects of parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) - mediated emotion regulation process and psychosocial risk factors in predisposing children to either reactive or proactive subtypes of ag- gression using a longitudinal (repeated-measures) design.

Reactive and Proactive Aggression

Researchers have classified two subtypes of aggression in children and adolescents: RA

(also called affective or impulsive aggression) and PA (also called instrumental or predatory ag- gression). According to the frustration-aggression theory, RA is the consequence of hostile at- tributional bias when an individual attends selectively to a hostile or threatening environmental cue during social interactions (Berkowitz, 1962; Crick, Dodge, Crick, & Dodge, 1996; Dodge &

Coie, 1987; Dodge, Murphy, & Buchsbaum, 1984; Dodge, Pettit, McClaskey, Brown, &

Gottman, 1986; Dollard, Miller, Doob, Mowrer, & Sears, 1939). Conversely, the social learning theory posits that PA is pulled by an anticipation of external rewards after the execution of vio- lent behavior; thus it occurs mostly without provocation (Bandura, 1973; Crick et al., 1996;

Crick & Dodge, 1994).

The social information-processing model of aggression (Crick et al., 1996; Crick &

Dodge, 1994; Dodge et al., 1986) further suggests that RA and PA are associated with deficits related to different steps during the information-processing process. In particular, RA is con-

2

structed to relate to early stage problems; that is, reactively aggressive individuals “encode” en-

vironmental cues in a selective and inaccurate manner, attend to fewer and selectively to threat-

ening cues, and/or display a hostile attributional bias when “interpreting” others’ behaviors (de

Castro, Merk, Koops, Veerman, & Bosch, 2005; Dodge & Frame, 1982; Milich & Dodge, 1984).

In contrast, PA is associated with later stage problems; that is, proactively aggressive individuals

select the most positively evaluated responses to enact in the situation, expect more positive out-

comes for aggressive behaviors, feel more positively about their ability to perform aggressive be-

haviors, and make less negative evaluations of aggressive responses during the response deci-

sion–making process (Crick et al., 1996).

Multiple assessment tools have been developed to assess the two types of aggression and

most of the empirical studies have unitized one of the following two instruments. Dodge and

Coie (1987) first developed a 12-item teacher-rating instrument to measure RA and PA in ele-

mentary school students. Respondents indicate how frequently each item applies to the child on a

1-to 5-point Likert scale, ranging from “never” to “almost always”. The scale contains three items indexing RA (i.e., “when teased, strikes back”, “blames others in fights”, and “overreacts angrily to accidents”), three items indexing PA (i.e., “use physical force to dominate”, “gets oth- ers to gang up on a peer” and “threatens and bullies others”), and six items indexing unclassified aggression (e.g., “teases and name-calls”, “gets into verbal arguments”). Multiple informant ver- sions (i.e., teacher, parent, and self-report) of the Reactive-Proactive Aggression Questionnaire

(RPQ) that can be used in youth aged nine to 16 was later developed by Raine et al. (2006). This scale contains 11 items of RA (e.g., “hit others to defend yourself”), and 12 items of PA (e.g.,

“used force to get money or things from others). Respondent rates each statement as 0 (never), 1

3

(sometimes), or 2 (often) for the frequency of occurrence (See Appendix A). Both of these scales

had good psychometric characteristics, and factor analyses of these scales have identified RA

and PA as two correlated but distinct subtypes of aggression (Crick & Dodge, 1994; Day, Bream,

& Pal, 1992; Dodge & Coie, 1987; Poulin & Boivin, 2000a; Price & Dodge, 1989; Raine et al.,

2006).

Psychosocial Differences of RA and PA

Although some argue against the distinctions between these subtypes of aggression given

that aggressive individuals tend to show high levels of both RA and PA (Bobadilla, Wampler, &

Taylor, 2012; Miller & Lynam, 2006), there is growing evidence suggesting that there are mean-

ingful distinctions between RA and PA in the temperamental, behavioral, cognitive, social-envi-

ronmental and developmental domains (Hubbard et al., 2010) (see Table 1).

Temperamental and Behavioral Correlates

Research has in general demonstrated that there are differential temperamental and be-

havioral correlates for RA and PA. For example, RA has been associated with anger rumination (

Vitaro, Brendgen, & Tremblay, 2002; White & Turner, 2014), poor emotion regulation (Marsee

& Frick, 2007), anxiety, and (Card & Little, 2006; McAuliffe, Hubbard, Rubin,

Morrow, & Dearing, 2007; Raine et al., 2006). Conversely, PA has been associated with psycho-

pathic personality (Raine et al., 2006) and callous-unemotional traits (Fanti, Frick, & Georgiou,

2008; Frick et al., 2003), referring to a specific set of affective and interpersonal styles (e.g., lack of guilt, fail to show empathy, and using others to get what they want; Frick et al., 2003; Kruh,

2005).

In addition, some researchers have found that RA is uniquely associated with attention

4

and impulsivity problems (Atkins & Stoff, 1993; Crick et al., 1996; Dodge & Coie, 1987; Raine

et al., 2006), while others have linked both RA and PA to the attention-deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Card & Little, 2006; Connor, Steingard, Cunningham, Anderson, & Melloni,

2004). PA has been uniquely linked to delinquency and serious antisocial problems (Atkins &

Stoff, 1993; Pulkkinen, 1996; Raine et al., 2006; Vitaro et al., 2002; Vitaro, Gendreau,

Tremblay, & Oligny, 1998), and specifically to later externalizing and antisocial behaviors. For example, one longitudinal study showed that proactively, but not reactively aggressive children at age 12 years showed increased conduct problems in middle adolescence (Vitaro et al., 1998).

In another study, PA at age 14 years was uniquely associated with adult criminality and external- izing behaviors in males, and internalizing behaviors and neuroticism in females, whereas RA was associated with better adjustment in adulthood (Pulkkinen, 1996).

Cognitive Correlates

Prior research has established the relationship between impaired intellectual functioning

(i.e., IQ) and aggression and antisocial behaviors in children and adolescents (Archwamety &

Katsiyannis, 2000; Dougherty et al., 2007; Loney, Frick, Ellis, & McCoy, 1998; McHale,

Obrzut, & Sabers, 2003; Moffitt, Lynam, & Silva, 1994). Lower verbal IQ has been thought to impede an individuals’ ability to engage in private speech, which is important for behavioral reg- ulation, and has also been associated with lower academic achievement and increased school re- jection (Loney et al., 1998; Luria, 1980; Moffitt et al., 1994).

With regard to RA and PA, researchers have found that children with higher RA showed poor verbal skills, whereas those with higher PA were characterized by intact or better verbal abilities (Arsenio, Adams, & Gold, 2009; Loney et al., 1998; Luria, 1980; Moffitt et al., 1994). It

5

has been proposed that poor verbal skills found in reactively aggressive children may lead to fail-

ure in addressing potential interpersonal misunderstandings, thus further lead to an increase of

provoked aggressive behaviors. In contrast, greater verbal abilities and thus better social skills

may enable proactively aggressive youths to use aggression as a strategy to obtain external goals

during social interactions (Arsenio et al., 2009). Similarly, although impaired executive function-

ing (EF) have often been found in the general antisocial population, RA, but not PA, has been

uniquely related to deficits in EF and social information-processing (Crick et al., 1996; Dodge &

Coie, 1987; Giancola, Moss, Martin, Kirisci, & Tarter, 1996; Raine et al., 2006).

Social and Environmental Correlates

Several social and environmental factors, including parenting styles and peer relation-

ships, have been found to be predictive of the development of aggression and antisocial behav-

iors. With regard to RA and PA, research has shown that RA is associated with histories of phys- ical abuse, harsh parenting, and peer rejection and victimization (Dodge, Lochman, Harnish,

Bates, & al, 1997; Poulin & Boivin, 2000b). Conversely, PA is associated with parental sub- stance abuse, family violence, and tolerance or even positive relationships with peers (Connor et al., 2004; Dodge et al., 1997; Poulin & Boivin, 2000b; Raine et al., 2006). These findings pro- vide the empirical support for Dodge’s (Dodge, 1991) model, which proposes that RA and PA may originate from different causes. Briefly, it has been proposed that while RA develops in re- action to an abusive and harsh parenting or a negative and threatening environment (e.g., peer rejection and victimization), PA thrives in the “supportive” environments that foster the use of aggression as a means to achieve one’s goals (e.g., family violence and positive peer relation- ship). Furthermore, these different social and environmental correlates of RA and PA have

6

yielded great insights into learning about the developmental progression of the two subtypes of

aggression.

Developmental Trajectories

Past research has examined the developmental course of the two subtypes of aggression

(Fite & Colder, 2007; Fite, Colder, Lochman, & Wells, 2008; Lansford, Dodge, Bates, & Pettit,

2002; Vitaro et al., 2006). Fite and Colder (2007) have reported the 1-year stability of RA and

PA to be .72 and .55, in a group of children between nine and 12 years old. Furthermore, find-

ings have suggested that RA and PA both decline as children age (Fite et al., 2008; Lansford et

al., 2002). In one study, Lansford et al. (2002) examined RA and PA using teacher’s report and

found that both types of aggression declined from kindergarten through the 3rd grade, and from

the 6th grade through the 7th grade. In a group of aggressive children, Fite et al. (2008) found that both RA and PA declined from 5th to 9th grade, and that the developmental trajectories were simi-

lar for both forms of aggression.

With regard to the long-term outcomes of children with the two subtypes of aggression,

as previously reviewed, findings have suggested that children with higher RA will show rela-

tively better adulthood adjustment (Pulkkinen, 1996), whereas those with higher PA are more

likely to show severe later antisocial behavior problems (Atkins & Stoff, 1993; Pulkkinen, 1996;

Raine et al., 2006). Researchers have proposed that the decreases in RA during late childhood

and early adolescence may be due to improvement in self-regulation and temperament manage-

ment. In contrast, PA has been proposed to remain stable or even increase throughout adoles-

cence, as reinforced by the family and peer environment in using force to solve conflicts (Vitaro,

Brendgen, & Barker, 2006; Vitaro et al., 2002).

7

Finally, there is some evidence showing that while RA is predictive of later violence dur- ing romantic relationships, PA is predictive of delinquency-related violence towards strangers

(Brendgen, Vitaro, Tremblay, & Lavoie, 2001). According to the proposition by Hubbard et al.

(2010), the progression from early RA to later violence towards the intimate partner may be due to negative parenting (e.g., lack of warmth and emotional involvement with the child), whereas the progression from early PA to later violence towards strangers may be due to a lack of paren- tal monitoring, which in turn fosters violent behaviors.

In sum, a limited number of longitudinal studies have shown that the level of RA declines whereas PA remains stable or even increases from early childhood to adolescence, and their long-term outcomes may be qualitatively different.

Gender Differences

Studies in aggressive behavior have often shown that boys are generally more aggressive than girls. Of note, boys are found to be three times more likely to be diagnosed with conduct problems than girls (Zoccolillo, 1993). Relatively less research has examined gender differences in terms of RA and PA. Salmivalli and Nieminen (2002) found that boys scored higher on both

RA and PA as compared to girls in a sample of 1,062 children aged 10 to 12 years. In contrast,

Connor, Steingard, Anderson, and Melloni (2003) found no gender differences in RA or PA in a clinical sample of seriously emotional disturbed children and adolescents. Gender differences in correlates of RA and PA have also been reported. For example, in one study by Connor et al.

(2003), RA was associated with hyperactivity/impulsivity in boys and early abuse experience and lower verbal IQ in girls, whereas PA was found to be associated with drug use, hostility, and negative parenting in both genders. In a recent study by Stickle, Marini, and Thomas (2012), it

8

was found that girls with both RA and PA showed more callous-unemotional (CU) traits com- pared with boys with both subtypes of aggression in a sample of adjudicated adolescents. Moreo- ver, RA was found to be uniquely associated with poor emotion regulation and anger reaction, whereas PA was uniquely associated with CU traits in a sample of detained girls aged 12 to 18

(Marsee & Frick, 2007). Understanding the distinct correlates of RA and PA in boys and girls may shed light on future prevention and treatment efforts that target on each gender group.

Table 1. Psychosocial Differences between RA and PA RA PA Temperamental/ Behavioral Anger rumination, emotion Psychopathic personality, CU dysregulation, anxiety, de- traits, and hyperactivity features pression, attention, and im- pulsivity problems

Cognitive Lower verbal IQ and poor ex- Better verbal skills and intact ecutive functioning executive functioning

Social/ Environmental Negative environment, harsh “Supportive environment”, pa- parenting, peer rejection, and rental substance abuse, lack of parental monitoring, family vio- lence, tolerant or even positive relationships with peers

Developmental Decreases in late childhood Chronic antisocial behaviors and early adolescence

Gender In boys: RA was associated In girls: PA was associated with with hyperactivity and impul- CU traits. sivity. In girls: RA was associated with lower verbal IQ, CU traits, and emotion dysregula- tion.

9

Neurobiological Differences of RA and PA

In addition, there is growing literature documenting the differences between RA and PA at the neurobiological levels (see Table 2).

Genetics

Findings from the behavioral genetic research have shown that genetic effects account for up to 50% of the variations in shaping aggressive behavior (Miles & Carey, 1997; Plomin, Nitz,

& Rowe, 1990). Studies examining the genetic structure of RA and PA have suggested that herit- ability is slightly higher for PA than for RA and that there may be gender differences. For in- stance, in 172 male and female twin pairs who were six years old, Brendgen et al. (2006) found that genetic influences explained 39% and 41% of the variances of RA and PA, respectively, and that the genetic influences are highly correlated (r = .87) in both gender groups. In a sample of nine-to 10-year-old twins (N = 596 pairs), Baker et al. (2008) reported that in boys, the genetic influences on PA were higher than those on RA (50% and 38% for PA and RA, respectively), whereas in girls, environmental but not genetic influences almost entirely accounted for both subtypes of aggression. Furthermore, there is evidence that while the stability of RA may be due to both genetic and non-shared environmental influences (i.e., experiences that make siblings dissimilar), the stability of PA is primarily mediated by the genetic influences (Tuvblad, Raine,

Zheng, & Baker, 2009). In this study, participants included nine- to 10-year-old twins (N =

1,241) who were assessed again at ages 11 to 14 years (N = 874). Taken together, findings sug- gest that the genetic influences may play a more significant role in PA and its development, in particular in males.

10

Neuroimaging

Very few neuroimaging studies have examined the structural and functional brain abnor- mality in children and adolescents with aggressive behavior (Connor, 2002). However, findings from the adult literature have in general shown the structural and functional brain deficits promi- nently in the prefrontal cortex (PFC; involved in inhibitory control, decision-making, and emo- tional processing), the temporal cortex (language and memory), the amygdala (fear conditioning and emotion), the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; emotion regulation and autonomic function- ing), and the hippocampus (emotion regulation and fear conditioning) are associated with higher antisocial and aggressive behaviors (Blair, 2003; Blair, 2001; Kiehl, 2006; Yang & Raine, 2009).

Concerning RA and PA, relatively less neuroimaging evidence has been documented, alt- hough several neurocognitive models have been proposed to conceptualize the neurobiological distinctions between the two subtypes of aggression. For example, White, Meffert, and Blair

(2015) formulated the cognitive neuroscience models of aggression in the legal context. Specifi- cally, their presumptions of the different neurological substrates of the two subtypes of aggres- sion include: a) RA is associated with increased amygdala and periaqueductal gray (PAG) acti- vation, and also with ventral medial PFC and striatum deficiency, implying an over-activated basic-threat system in response to social provocation and difficulties in solving frustration; and b) PA is associated with functional deficits in amygdala and ventral medial PFC, implying a lack of empathy and problems with moral decision-making. Similarly, Blair (2001) proposed a neu- rocognitive model of aggression, in which RA is postulated to be associated with impairments in the executive emotional system and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) in particular, whereas PA is

11

linked to dysfunctions in the systems (e.g., amygdala) involved in socialization, which is partly

reflected by deficits in fear conditioning.

Indeed, several preliminary empirical studies in adults have supported these conceptual neurocognitive models. For example, it was found that prefrontal dysfunction was specific to the affective (impulsive/reactive) but not the predatory (premeditated/proactive) subgroup of con- victed murderers (Raine et al., 1998). Dolan, Deakin, Roberts, and Anderson (2002) found that

impulsive–aggressive patients showed significant volume reduction in the temporal lobe.

Coccaro, McCloskey, Fitzgerald, and Phan (2007) found an amygdala-OFC dysfunction in re- sponse to angry faces in individuals with impulsive/reactive aggression. Although neuroimaging

research on PA is limited, researchers have found that individuals with psychopathy, a cluster of

personality traits that are highly correlated with PA, showed reduced activity in the amygdala

and the rostral ACC/ventral medial PFC (vmPFC) in response to facial emotional expressions

(Kiehl et al., 2001) and during a fear conditioning task (Birbaumer et al., 2005). Similarly,

among non-clinical individuals, researchers have found that people high on psychopathic traits

showed reduced activation in the amygdala in response to emotional expressions (Gordon, Baird,

& End, 2004).

In aggregate, prior theoretical and empirical literature has suggested that RA is associated

with impairments in the brain areas related to information processing, emotion regulation and in-

hibitory control (i.e., prefrontal areas, temporal lobe, amygdala, and OFC). In contrast, PA is as-

sociated with impairment in the areas related to emotion responses and fear conditioning (i.e.,

amygdala, ACC, and vmPFC). Given that the empirical findings are exclusively from the adults,

it will be important for future studies to replicate these findings in younger populations.

12

Psychophysiology

Studies using psychophysiological methods have been flourishing since the last decade

and been used widely in the area of research in aggression and antisocial behaviors in children

and adolescents.

Autonomic nervous system activity. In general, lower baseline or resting heart rate

(HR) is one of the most robust biological correlates of aggressive and antisocial behaviors in

youth (Lorber, 2004; Ortiz & Raine, 2004; Pitts, 1997). Lower baseline HR, indicating lower

baseline levels of autonomic nervous system (ANS) activity, has been associated with both RA

and PA (Raine, Fung, Portnoy, Choy, & Spring, 2014). In terms of ANS reactivity to social or

cognitive stimuli, researchers have found RA to be associated with larger HR reactivity and

higher skin conductance (SC) responses to stressful or threatening stimuli (see Lorber, 2004 for a

review). For example, Pitts (1997) found that RA but not PA was associated with increased HR

reactivity during a challenging situation in boys from the 3rd to 6th grades. Hubbard et al. (2002) found RA but not PA to be related to increased SC reactivity to provocation in a group of 2nd

graders. In contrast, PA but not RA have been associated with reduced SC responses to punish- ments in youths (Gao, Tuvblad, Schell, Baker, & Raine, 2015).

Findings on the associations between PNS activity (i.e., vagal activity) and aggression, however, are less consistent. For example, prior research has shown that conduct problems are associated with either low (Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, & Mead, 2007; Calkins & Dedmon, 2000)

or high levels of baseline PNS activity (Dietrich et al., 2007; Scarpa, Fikretoglu, & Luscher,

2000). Low baseline PNS activity has been posited to relate to poor emotion regulation

13

(Beauchaine, 2001; Porges, Doussard-Roosevelt, & Maiti, 1994), though high baseline PNS ac- tivity has been related to a passive coping style to stressor or vagal sensitivity (Raine &

Venables, 1984; Venables, 1988). With regard to RA and PA, low baseline PNS activity has been found in children with high RA, indicating their poor emotion regulation skills (Scarpa,

Haden, & Tanaka, 2010; Xu, Raine, Yu, & Krieg, 2013). Conversely, high baseline PNS has been found in proactively aggressive children (Scarpa et al., 2010), which may reflect their better emotional control or regulation skills, and/or their insensitivity to negative effects (Fabes &

Eisenberg, 1997; Scarpa et al., 2010). Relatively less is known about the direct effects of PNS reactivity in response to threats/stressors on the two subtypes of aggressive behavior. Evidence from adult populations has indicated in women with a sexual abuse history, blunted PNS reactiv- ity during a stressful interview to be uniquely associated with proactively

(Murray-Close & Rellini, 2012). Zhang and Gao (2015) found that PNS reactivity during a moral decision-making task to be positively related to RA but negatively related to PA in college stu- dents from benign home environments.

Taken together, empirical evidence suggests that the two subtypes of aggression may be characterized by different ANS activity profiles. While hyperarousal may be more relevant to the reactive form of aggression, reflecting exaggerated responses to stress, provocation, and negative affect, autonomic hypoarousal seems more relevant to the proactive form aggression (Eysenck,

1997; Raine, Venables, & Mednick, 1997; Scarpa & Raine, 1997), indicating their fearlessness, callousness, and the tendency towards sensation seeking in attempt to maintain arousal to an op- timal level .

14

EEG and ERP correlates. A large number of studies using electrocortical measures has

shown that enhanced slow wave electroencephalographic (EEG) activity is associated with ag- gressive and antisocial behaviors in children and adolescents (Raine, Venables, & Williams,

1990; Scarpa & Raine, 1997). This predominance of the slow wave activities may suggest corti- cal immaturity that is linked to impaired inhibitive ability (Volavka, 1990).

Research has also examined the associations between left versus right frontal EEG asym- metry and aggressive behaviors. Based on the motivational direction model of frontal asym- metry, relatively greater left frontal activity is related to approach motivation, whereas greater right frontal activity is linked to withdrawal motivation (Harmon-Jones, 2004). Results from the line of work by Harmon-Jones and colleagues (Harmon-Jones & Allen, 1998; Harmon-Jones &

Sigelman, 2001; Harmon-Jones, Vaughn-Scott, Mohr, Sigelman, & Harmon-Jones, 2004;

Harmon-Jones, 2003, 2004; Peterson, Shackman, & Harmon-Jones, 2008) have shown that rela- tively increased left vs. right frontal activation during rest and anger induction is related to higher levels of anger and aggressive behaviors in adults. In addition, frontal EEG activity asymmetry has been found in children and adolescents with mood and disruptive behavior disorders (Rybak,

Crayton, Young, Herba, & Konopka, 2006).

In terms of RA and PA, however, it is still unclear to what degree the frontal asymmetry findings may apply to the two subtypes of aggression. Only one recent study by Dambacher et al.

(2015) has shown that proactively aggressive behavior could be reduced by inducing right hemi- spheric dominance through transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) in men. Given that

RA was more related to anger, hostility, and impulsivity (Raine et al., 2006; White & Turner,

15

2014), and that PA was thought to be predatory and goal oriented (Dodge & Coie, 1987), the en-

hancement of the right hemisphere activity may theoretically inhibit the approach motivation as

mediated by the left hemisphere activity. Therefore, these preliminary data have provided evi-

dence to support the notion that the increased left frontal activity may be more significantly asso-

ciated with PA but not RA. Future studies are needed to replicate these findings.

Another line of research has focused on event-related potentials (ERPs) in relation to ag-

gressive behaviors. Among these studies, abnormality in P3 (i.e., P300), a rather broad positivity

that is maximal along the midline at the parietal recording sites and between 300 and 500 milli-

seconds following stimulus presentation (Sutton, Braren, Zubin, & John, 1965), has been found

to be a robust correlate of aggressive and violent behavior. P3 component is a well-known and reliable marker of brain functioning related to information processing. In particular, lower ampli- tude and longer latency of P3 indicate limited neural resources and inefficient cognitive function- ing (Donchin, 1979). Research on the P3 component in response to a variety of tasks (e.g., the visual and auditory oddball tasks, the Stroop task) has demonstrated that antisocial behaviors are associated with reduced P3 amplitudes and lengthened P3 latencies, reflecting their information processing impairments (Gao & Raine, 2009).

Further to this point, research has shown that P3 deficits are uniquely associated with the impulsive/reactive (Barratt, Stanford, Kent, Felthous, & Alan, 1997; Gerstle, Mathias, &

Stanford, 1998) but not the instrumental/proactive form of aggression (Barratt et al., 1997;

Stanford, Houston, Villemarette-Pittman, & Greve, 2003). Thus, impaired cognitive functioning, such as inefficient attention allocation, maybe specific to RA. Finally, since most studies have focused on P3, it is important for future studies to investigate other ERP components, such as P1

16

(reflecting selective attention ability; Mangun & Hillyard, 1996) and N2 (response inhibition;

Bekker, Kenemans, & Verbaten, 2005), in relation to the two subtypes of aggression.

Endocrinology

Cortisol is a type of hormone that indexes the arousal of the hypothalamic-pituitary-ad-

renal (HPA) system, which controls the neurophysiological processes in response to stressors (de

Kloet, 1998; Johnson, Kamilaris, Chrousos, & Gold, 1992). The exposure to a stressor triggers

the hypothalamus to communicate with the pituitary gland by secreting corticotrophin-releasing

hormone (CRH), and the pituitary gland responds by secreting adrenocorticotrophic hormone

(ACTH), which further activates the adrenal gland to release more cortisol (Lopez-Duran, Hajal,

Olson, Felt, & Vazquez, 2009). Cortisol levels have been reported to be reduced in aggressive

children and adolescents (McBurnett, Lahey, Rathouz, & Loeber, 2000; Shoal, Giancola, &

Kirillova, 2003), suggesting lower HPA arousal in these individuals. The associations between

cortisol activity and aggression have not been reported consistently, however. For example,

Mcburnett et al. (1991) found elevated cortisol levels in conduct disordered children with a

comorbid anxiety disorder.

In relation to RA and PA, researchers have found that higher levels of baseline cortisol

and increased cortisol reactivity to stress are associated with RA, whereas the HPA-axis profile

(both baseline cortisol and cortisol reactivity) of proactively aggressive children is no different from that of non-aggressive children (Lopez-Duran, Olson, Hajal, Felt, & Vazquez, 2009; van

Bokhoven et al., 2005). Together, evidence suggests that RA is characterized by a more active

HPA system associated with enhanced stress reactivity, whereas the HPA-axis profile of proac- tively aggressive children was equivalent to that of non-aggressive children.

17

Table 2. Neurobiological Differences between RA and PA RA PA Neuroimaging Impairments in brain regions Impairments in brain regions linked to information processing, linked to negative emotion respon- emotion regulation, and inhibi- sivity and fear conditioning tory control EEG N/A Greater left vs. right frontal EEG asymmetry

ERP Lower P3 amplitude and longer Normal P3 P3 latency

ANS ANS hyper-arousal ANS hypo-arousal

HPA More active HPA system Normal HPA system

Chapter Summary

Taken together, prior studies have demonstrated that RA and PA are different in the psy- chological, behavioral, cognitive, developmental, psychosocial, and neurobiological domains.

While RA has been associated with anger rumination, anxiety and depression, PA has been linked to callous-unemotional traits and psychopathic personality traits. Additionally, while RA has been related to poor verbal ability and executive functioning, PA is characterized by good verbal skills and intact cognitive functioning. Furthermore, RA has been thought to develop from harsh parenting or a threatening environment. In contrast, PA may develop in reaction to the

“supportive” environments that promote the use of aggressive behavior. Moreover, gender differ- ences have been observed in terms of the differential correlates of RA and PA. The two types of aggression seem to follow different developmental trajectories, and RA seems to be associated

18

with better adulthood adjustment than PA. Finally, while RA predicts adulthood violence to- wards romantic partner, PA is predictive of the later violent behaviors towards strangers.

Most of the neurobiological research, in particular the brain imaging and EEG/ERP stud- ies, has focused on adult populations and relatively little has been done with children and adoles- cents. While the neurobiological abnormality observed in adults may be the consequences of the aggressive behavior itself or the manifest of the environmental influences, in children an early investigation of the neurobiological abnormalities may maximize the chance of identifying its etiology and minimize the effect of aggressive behavior itself (Sterzer & Stadler, 2009). Dozens of studies have suggested that both youths and adults displaying antisocial and aggressive behav- iors show abnormally low autonomic system activity (Sterzer & Stadler, 2009). However, very few studies have systematically investigated the neurobiological differences or similarities be- tween young and old populations. In terms of RA and PA, empirical evidence has suggested that

RA is associated with brain abnormalities in regions linked to information processing, emotion regulation and control of inhibition, higher ANS arousal, reduced P3 amplitude, and a higher level of HPA activity. In contrast, PA is associated with impairments in the brain areas related to responsivity to negative emotions and fear conditioning, lower ANS arousal, greater left vs. right hemisphere activation, relatively intact cognitive functioning (i.e., normal P3), and a normal level of HPA activity. Future studies incorporating and comparing multiple measures of physio- logical responses (i.e., autonomic arousal, EEG, ERP, and brain imaging) within the same task will help further our understanding of the etiology of aggression.

19

CHAPTER 2 - EMOTION REGULATION

Emotion Regulation

Emotion regulation has emerged as a popular research topic as it plays an important role in understanding both typical and atypical childhood development (Beauchaine, 2015a; Cole,

Marin, & Dennis, 2004; Keenan, 2006). It has been generally agreed that the process of emotion regulation is distinct from that of emotion generation (the formation of an emotional response)

(Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010; Cole et al., 2004; Gross & Thompson, 2007).

Furthermore, although the definition and research methodologies of emotion regulation often vary across studies (Adrian, Zeman, & Veits, 2011; Cole et al., 2004; Eisenberg et al., 2001;

Morris, Silk, Steinberg, & Robinson, 2009), there seems to be a common intuitive understanding of what is meant by emotion regulation (Cole et al., 2004; Morris et al., 2009; Thompson, 2008).

Broadly speaking, emotion regulation refers to the extrinsic and intrinsic processes that an indi- vidual makes to monitor, evaluate, and modify emotional responses and experiences as to ac- complish one’s goals (Thompson, 1990). An extended model of emotion regulation includes three major regulatory stages: identification (whether or not to regulate), selection (which gen- eral regulation categories to use), and implementation (which specific regulation tactic to imple- ment). Meanwhile, each stage has three basic elements: perception, valuation, and action (Gross,

2015; Sheppes, Suri, & Gross, 2015). Emotion dysregulation can occur in any of the basic ele- ments of the three major regulatory stages (Sheppes et al., 2015).

Effective emotion regulation is essential for a child’s development of adaptive function- ing (Cole et al., 2004), and emotion dysregulation has been associated with various forms of psy- chopathology (e.g., depression, anxiety, bipolar disorders, ADHD) (Beauchaine, 2015b; Nancy

20

Eisenberg et al., 2001; Leibenluft, 2011; Shaw et al., 2012; Silk, Steinberg, & Morris, 2003;

Sobanski et al., 2010; Suveg & Zeman, 2004; Zeman, Shipman, & Suveg, 2002), and aggressive

and violent behaviors in particular (Rothbart, Ahadi, & Hershey, 1994; Zeman et al., 2002). In-

deed, children who are unable to regulate emotion efficiently are more likely to show hostile re-

action consequent to frustration and provocation, whereas individuals with superior emotion reg- ulation skills show fewer problematic behaviors via better impulse or anger control (Rothbart et al., 1994; Zeman et al., 2002).

Measurements of Emotion Regulation

Many of the prior studies have used self- or informant-report (i.e., teachers and parents),

laboratory observation, or behavioral paradigms to assess emotion regulation. Recent studies

with implementation of a variety of neurophysiological methodologies (e.g., neuroimaging, vagal

activity, EEG/ERPs) have helped us to disentangle the important mechanisms underlying the

emotion regulation processes (Adrian et al., 2011; Beauchaine, 2015a; Davidson, 2000). In fact,

multiple levels of analyses including at least one physiological measure have often been used in

emotion regulation research papers published in the top tier journals (Adrian et al., 2011).

Behavioral Approaches

Self-report. Self-report measures are important instruments for assessing emotion regula- tion even in young children (Durbin, 2010). Several instruments, including the Difficulties with

Emotion Regulation Scale (Gratz & Roemer, 2004; Weinberg & Klonsky, 2009), the Emotion

Regulation Questionnaire for Children and Adolescents (Gross & John, 2003; Gullone,

MacDermott, & Hughes, 2009), and the Emotion/Affect Regulation Interview (Zeman & Garber,

1996), have often been used to assess emotion regulation in children beyond 13 years, between

21

10 and 18 years, and as young as 1st grade, respectively. For example, the Difficulties with Emo-

tion Scale consists of 36 items assessing a variety of problems associated with regulation of neg-

ative affect, including the Non-Acceptance, Goals, Impulse, Awareness, Strategies, and Clarity

subscales. The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire for Children and Adolescents is a 10-item

questionnaire designed to assess the emotion regulation strategies of Reappraisal (e.g., “When I

want to feel happier, I think about something different”) and Suppression (e.g., “I keep my feel-

ings to myself.”). Finally, the Emotion/Affect Regulation Interview assesses Emotion Regulation

Decision, Display Rules, Reasons and Methods of Regulation, and Emotion Self-Efficacy from the semi-structure interview using vignettes and open-ended questions.

Informant-report. Informant reports (e.g., parent and teacher) are sometimes used to measure emotion regulation in youth, as they usually demonstrate better reliability and validity

(Morris, Robinson, & Eisenberg, 2006). The most frequently used parent or teacher reports are the Emotion Regulation Checklist (Shields & Cicchetti, 1997) and the California Q-sort (Block

& Block, 1980; Shields & Cicchetti, 1997). The Emotion Regulation Checklist is a 24-item par- ent- or teacher-report assessing Emotion Liability/Negativity (i.e., dysregulated negative affect) and Emotion Regulation (i.e., appropriate emotion expression) in children and adolescents be- tween six and 18 years old. The California Q-sort scale is a 100-item parent-report, which as- sesses different types of personalities with 10 emotion regulation items in children aged six to 12 years old.

Observational methods. Observational methods are often used in measuring emotion regulation during child development, due to the fact that very young children may have difficul- ties in understanding and reporting their own emotional experiences (Cole et al., 2004). Research

22

of child temperament has yielded important evidence on how emotions are regulated in young populations (e.g., infants and toddlers). For example, the Infant-Toddler Frustration Paradigm

(e.g., Stifter & Braungart, 1995) has been adopted by researchers to assess the ability to self- sooth, avoid, communicate, orientate to others, and redirect in infants to 18-month-old toddlers in a variety of tasks such as arm restraints and toy removal. Moreover, emotion regulation has been examined during early mother-and-child interaction (Cohn & Tronick, 1987; Field, 2009;

Tronick, 1989), because the quality of this emotional exchange between mother and child is pre- dictive of later emotion regulation development (Cole et al., 2004). In this paradigm, the emo- tional exchange between mother and child is recorded, coded, and analyzed in terms of the tim- ing and sequencing of the changes in each partner of the dyad (Field, 2009). Finally, many stud- ies focus on child’s self-initiated attempts and skills to regulate negative emotions under chal- lenging conditions. For example, the Anger Induction: Interaction Paradigm (Cummings, 1987;

Cummings, Pellegrini, Notarius, & Cummings, 1989; Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002) is designed to examine child’s emotion behavioral responses and emotion regulation strategies (i.e., appropri- ately concerned, undercontrol, and overcontrol) in response to the interadult conflicts between the research assistant and the mother. Children’s emotion regulation has also been examined dur- ing the frustration task in which they must tolerate waiting to get a cookie when there is little of interest in the surrounding environment (Gilliom, Shaw, Beck, Schonberg, & Lukon, 2002). Sim- ilarly, children’s attempts to regulate their emotional behaviors have been assessed in the disap- pointing situation in which the researcher creates positive expectations for receiving a desirable reward for children but in fact giving an undesirable one (Cole, 1986; Saarni, 1984).

23

Neurobiological Approaches

Various behavioral measures, although widely used and reliable, do not tap into the re-

lated internal neurobiological processes of emotion regulation (Beauchaine, 2015a; Lewis,

Granic, & Lamm, 2006). The most compelling evidence for emotion regulation has emerged

from studies that use neurophysiological methodologies (e.g., neuroimaging, EEG/ERPs, psy-

chophysiology), which are critically important for understanding the underlying mechanism of

emotion regulation (Beauchaine, 2015a; Jackson, Malmstadt, Larson, & Davidson, 2000).

Neuroimaging techniques can provide insights into the neural circuitries implicated in

emotion processing and regulation (e.g., Davidson, 2000). Although as noted previously, neu-

roimaging research in children and adolescents has been far less than that of adults (Connor,

2002). EEG/ERPs studies are well-suited to identify possible neural markers of emotion dysregu-

lation (Dennis, 2010; Lewis & Stieben, 2004). Autonomic activity, in particular the PNS activity,

has been studied as the peripheral biomarker of emotion regulation (Beauchaine, 2001, 2015a,

2015b). Of note, autonomic activity is used routinely in emotion regulation research in children

and adolescents. Compared to the ERP measures that capture the short-term self-regulation of emotion and require a very limited amount of active control, the ANS activity targets a relative long-term regulation process.

Neurobiological Mechanisms of Emotion Regulation

Cortical and Subcortical Neural Network

Evidence from neurobiological research indicates that emotion regulation is a top-down process where the cortical neural network regulates the emotion that is processed from the sub-

24

cortical neural network (Beauchaine, 2015a; Davidson, 2000; See Figure 1). The cortical net- work mediates the top-down deliberated regulation of emotion; in contrast, the subcortical net- work mediates the bottom-up autonomic process of generating emotion. The cortical network in- cludes brain regions such as the medial frontal cortex (MFC), dorsolateral frontal cortex (DLFC),

ACC, vmPFC and OFC, whereas the subcortical network includes brain regions such as the amygdala (Davidson, 2000). For example, the ACC, located in the MFC, is an essential cortical structure in modulation of cognition and emotion responses (Bush, Luu, & Posner, 2000; Casey et al., 1997; Dennis, 2010; Reed & Warner-Rogers, 2009). The amygdala, located in the medial temporal lobes of the brain, is a crucial subcortical structure for emotional perception and gener- ation, including determining the motivational significance of the emotional cues (e.g., threat, fear, and anger), sending messages to the lower brain regions involved in initiating motor move- ment and release of neurochemicals, and receiving feedback from the PFC for either prompting or inhibiting emotion (Zeman, Cassano, Perry-Parrish, & Stegall, 2006).

Effective communications between the cortical and the subcortical areas are important for successful emotion regulation. In contrast, deficiencies in emotion regulation have been related to reduced functional connectivity between the PFC and amygdala, implying deficient top-down modulation of the amygdala by the PFC regions (Beauchaine, 2015a). In addition, it is found that the subcortical networks mature during early development, while the cortical networks continue to develop in the 20s (Beauchaine & McNulty, 2013; Beauchaine, 2015a). Thus, deficits in regu- lating emotion often found in children and adolescents may be partly due to the competition be- tween the heightened activity in the subcortical network involved in processing emotional infor- mation and the immature cortical network involved in emotion regulation (Hare et al., 2008).

25

Researchers have also examined the cortical-subcortical activity during emotion regula-

tion in relation to a child’s internalizing/externalizing behaviors (Davidson, 2000; Hare et al.,

2008). Neuroimaging studies have revealed that children with anxiety disorders and trait of anxi-

ety show less functional connectivity in the amygdala-ventrolateral PFC connections in response to negative facial expressions in a variety of tasks (e.g., attention task, Go/No-Go task) (Hare et al., 2008; Monk et al., 2008). Shannon, Sauder, Beauchaine, and Gatzke-Kopp (2009) found that adolescent males with higher externalizing behaviors showed reduced functional connectivity in the striatal-prefrontal connections during a Reward/No-Reward task as compared to the controls.

Note: OFC= orbitofrontal cortex; vmPFC = ventral medial prefrontal cortex; MFC = medial frontal cortex; DLFC = dorsolateral frontal cortex; ACC = anteriro cingulate cortex.

Figure 1. Main Cortical (e.g., MFC, DLFC, ACC, vmPFC, OFC) and Subcortical Regions (e.g., amygdala) Involved in Emotion Regulation

26

Ventral and Dorsal Systems

Two distinct neural systems, ventral and dorsal, have been identified to be involved in regulating emotion automatically and effortfully, respectively (Dennis, Buss, & Hastings, 2012;

Dennis, 2010; Derryberry & Rothbart, 1997; Drevets & Raichle, 1998; Henderson & Wachs,

2007; Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, & Gabrieli, 2002; Reed & Warner-Rogers, 2009). The ventral sys- tem, consisting of brain areas such as the limbic, brainstem region, and the orbitofrontal and me- dial structure of the PFC (Dennis, 2010; Reed & Warner-Rogers, 2009), is sensitive to emotion- ally significant information, and is responsible for executing autonomic evaluation and regulat- ing emotion responses rapidly and defensively (Critchley, 2005; Dolan, 2002; Rolls, 2004). In contrast, the dorsal system, including the dorsolateral and the medial frontal areas (Dennis,

2010), utilizes motivationally relevant information from the ventral system to consciously formu- late decisions among a number of courses of actions based on the anticipated consequents so as to regulate emotion in an effortful, proactive and deliberate way (Dennis, 2010; Ochsner et al.,

2004; Reed & Warner-Rogers, 2009).

Abnormalities in the ventral and dorsal systems have been associated with maladaptive functioning related to emotional dysregulation. For example, neuroimaging evidence has shown that anxious and depressed individuals from seven to 32 year old have greater ventral activation of the PFC as compared to the non-depressed/anxious controls (Hare et al., 2008), implying highly and rigidly regulating/responding to the urging emotional value of the stimuli in the envi- ronment. In contrast, researchers have found that aggressive adults show deficits in both the OFC and other inter-connected core structures underlying emotion regulation, such as the ACC and the amygdala (Davidson, 2000; Hoptman, 2003), implying under-regulation of emotion with

27

both the ventral and dorsal systems. In his review article on the neurobiological bases of emotion

regulation and violence, Davidson (2000) proposed that deficits in the core structures underlying

emotion regulation, such as the OFC, the ACC, and the amygdala, would increase individuals’

vulnerability towards aggressive behavior (impulsive and affective aggression in particular).

Furthermore, the ACC, located in the medial frontal cortex, is an essential structure of the

dorsal system, and plays a significant role in regulating cognition and emotion behaviors (Bush

et al., 2000; Casey et al., 1997; Dennis, 2010; Reed & Warner-Rogers, 2009). Research into the

effortful regulatory control of emotion behaviors as generated by the ACC has focused on sev-

eral ERP components. For example, the Nc component is a frontocentral negative deflection

peaking around 200-700 milliseconds following the presentation of the stimulus. It is generated from the areas of the medial frontal cortex, including the ACC, and is sensitive to facial emotions

(Batty & Taylor, 2006; Lewis, Todd, & Honsberger, 2007; Nelson & Nugent, 1990; Todd,

Lewis, Meusel, & Zelazo, 2008). In a normative sample, Dennis, Malone, and Chen (2009) found that larger Nc amplitudes in response to facial expressions are associated with greater at- tention performance and better parent-reported emotion regulation. In addition, N2 and Error- related negativity (ERN) have also been examined, and both reflect the cognitive control pro- cessing over emotion as mediated by the dorsal ACC. Specifically, the N2 reflects conflict moni- toring and response inhibition, whereas the ERN reflects recognition of a mismatch error follow- ing incorrect responses (Bekker et al., 2005; Bokura, Yamaguchi, & Kobayashi, 2001). Prelimi- nary findings using ERP components in relation to emotion regulation as regulated by the ACC have demonstrated that anxious children show larger N2 amplitudes towards both positive and

28

negative facial cues and greater ERN in the Go/No-Go task, indicating increased deliberate con- trol of the proponent responses and more error monitoring (Hum, Manassis, & Lewis, 2013).

Autonomic Network

There is growing evidence for the significant role of the autonomic nervous systems in a wide variety of cognitive and emotional functions. In general, there are two branches of the

ANS: the parasympathetic and the sympathetic nervous systems (PNS and SNS), which function to mediate the direction and the magnitude of HR change. In general, HR decreases and in- creases in response to the activation of PNS and SNS influences, respectively. In addition, inves- tigators have examined respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) to measure PNS activity and pre- ejection period (PEP) to measure SNS activity (Berntson, Cacioppo, Binkley, et al., 1994;

Cacioppo, Uchino, & Berntson, 1994). Specifically, PNS-linked cardiac activity, indexed by

RSA or heart rate variability (HRV), has been linked with emotion regulation, whereas SNS- linked cardiac activity is associated with the behavioral approach system and reflects reward re- activity (Beauchaine et al., 2007; Beauchaine, 2001, 2015b; Brenner, Beauchaine, & Sylvers,

2005).

PNS activity. PNS-linked cardiac activity is a critical psychophysiological component used to reflect the emotion regulation process (Beauchaine et al., 2007; Beauchaine, 2001,

2015b; Brenner et al., 2005). PNS activity (i.e., vagal activity), as indexed by RSA or HRV, re- flects the periodic fluctuations in the HR that is related to respiration, and is determined largely by the vagal influences on the heart (Beauchaine, 2001; Grossman, Beek, & Wientjes, 1990).

Many studies have demonstrated that low baseline RSA and abnormal RSA reactivity are related to emotion dysregulation (Beauchaine, 2015b).

29

According to the neurovisceral integration theory (Thayer, Hansen, Saus-Rose, &

Johnsen, 2009), the PNS branch of the cardiovascular activity serves as a peripheral index of the medial PFC function that governs goal-oriented behavior. As stated by Thayer et al. (2009), the medial prefrontal, the insular, and the cingulate cortices form the central neural network that me- diates activation of the amygdala and in turn provides inhibitory input to the heart via the PNS connection with the sinoatrial node (Porges, 1995). Given the existence of inhibitory neural ef- ferent pathways from the medial PFC to the PNS (Barbas et al., 2003; Lane et al., 2009a; Ter

Horst et al., 1997; Wong et al., 2007), efficient prefrontal function produces higher baseline

RSA, and moderate levels of the RSA reactivity (Crowell et al., 2006). Consistent with this prop- osition, emotion regulation deficit has been related to the deficiency in the prefrontal brain re- gions (Davidson, 2000; Hare et al., 2008), which in turn is associated with lower RSA and an ab-

normal RSA reactivity (excessive RSA withdrawal or RSA augmentation) (Beauchaine, 2015b).

In addition, more convincing evidence that RSA is related to PFC has been found in le-

sion studies (e.g., Buchanan et al., 2010), and studies using positron emission tomography (e.g.,

Lane et al., 2009) or fMRI (see a review by Thayer et al., 2012). For example, Buchanan et al.

(2010) found that medial PFC damage is related to altered PNS activity. Similarly, Lane et al.

(2009a) found that blood flow in the medial PFC and the ACC during emotion induction is asso-

ciated with RSA activity. Together, these findings demonstrate that the PFC and the ACC con-

tribute, at least in part, to RSA activities.

PNS activity and emotion regulation. Many researchers have examined baseline RSA

and RSA reactivity in attempts to understand the neurobiological mechanisms underlying emo-

tion regulation. Higher baseline RSA is believed to reflect adaption to the environment and better

30

emotion regulation capacity (Beauchaine, 2015b; Gyurak & Ayduk, 2008; Marcovitch et al.,

2010). In the context of threat or challenge, however, vagal influence of the heart is expected to

withdraw, thus HR and the sympathetic responses (fight or flight) may increase (Berntson,

Cacioppo, Quigley, & Fabro, 1994). The removal of the vagal influence is referred to as vagal

withdrawal (i.e., decline in RSA from baseline to task, or increased RSA reactivity), whereas in-

creased vagal influence is referred to as vagal augmentation (increase in RSA from baseline to

task or reduced RSA reactivity). Higher baseline RSA under normal condition as well as moder-

ate RSA reactivity (i.e., efficient vagal withdrawal) when regulation is required have been pos-

ited to relate to effective PFC functioning and adaptive coping and self-regulation (Beauchaine,

2001, 2015b). Conversely, dysregulation of emotion and behaviors is characterized by lower

baseline RSA and excessive RSA reactivity. Furthermore, vagal augmentation in response to

challenges may deaccelerate HR, suggesting immobilization responses and a failure to cope effi-

ciently with environmental demands (Calkins & Dedmon, 2000; Calkins, 1997).

Empirical studies have documented the relationship between reduced baseline RSA and

abnormal RSA reactivity and increased youth psychopathology, including autism spectrum dis-

order (ASD) (Patriquin, Scarpa, Friedman, & Porges, 2013), depression (Rottenberg, 2007), anx-

iety (Hastings, Sullivan, et al., 2008), ADHD (Musser, Galloway-Long, Frick, & Nigg, 2013),

and elevated aggressive and antisocial behavior (Beauchaine et al., 2007; Beauchaine, Katkin,

Strassberg, & Snarr, 2001; Boyce et al., 2001; Calkins & Dedmon, 2000; Calkins, Graziano, &

Keane, 2007; de Wied, van Boxtel, Matthys, & Meeus, 2011). For example, de Wied, van

Boxtel, Matthys, and Meeus (2011) found the most severe subtype of aggressive behavior (in-

dexed by higher callous-unemotional traits) was related to low baseline RSA in 12- to 15-year-

31

old boys recruited from the special school for adolescents with behavior problems. Beauchaine,

Katkin, Strassberg, and Snarr (2001) found that 12- to 17-year-old boys with both comorbid con- duct problems and ADHD showed significant lower baseline RSA than both the ADHD only and the control groups. Pang and Beauchaine (2012) found that children aged eight and 12 years old with concurrent depression and conduct problems showed excessive RSA reactivity while watch- ing movie clips. Boyce et al. (2001) and Calkins et al. (2007) found that higher externalizing be- havior was related to reduced RSA reactivity or even RSA augmentation in a variety of cognitive and stress paradigms in children between five and seven years old from the community.

However, findings in this field are mixed. Some studies have shown that higher but not lower RSA activity is associated with more aggressive behavioral problems (Dietrich et al.,

2007; Scarpa et al., 2000). For example, Dietrich et al. (2007) found that 10- to 13-year-old chil- dren with higher vagal tone (and lower HR) during a rest period in the supine position exhibited elevated externalizing problems. This result is not surprising given that PNS activity is nega- tively associated with HR, and lower HR is robustly associated with antisocial and aggressive behaviors (Ortiz & Raine, 2004). Theoretically, a predominant PNS over SNS activity or exces- sive vagal sensitivity has been argued to reflect a passive vagal coping response to stress, which may further contribute to both lower HR and more antisocial behaviors (Raine & Venables,

1984; Venables, 1988). In addition, some studies have found that reduced PNS reactivity (i.e., vagal augmentation) in response to a number of challenges were associated with fewer external- izing problems and better self-regulation (Crowell et al., 2006; Hastings, Nuselovici, et al.,

2008).

32

Finally, other research has failed to establish a significant direct relationship between the

PNS activity and externalizing behaviors (El-Sheikh, Harger, & Whitson, 2001; El-Sheikh &

Whitson, 2006; Gordis, Feres, Olezeski, Rabkin, & Trickett, 2010). For example, El-Sheikh et al.

(2001) did not find significant correlations between externalizing behavior and baseline RSA ac- tivity/RSA reactivity. Instead they found that higher baseline RSA may buffer children against developing externalizing behaviors from exposure to marital conflicts in a community sample of children aged between eight and 12 years old. Similarly, in a sample of nine- to 16-year-old chil- dren and adolescents, Gordis et al. (2010) found that higher baseline RSA (suggesting better emotion regulation) protected against the effects of maltreatment on developing aggressive be- havior, although this effect was moderated by skin conductance reactivity among girls.

Chapter Summary

Emotion regulation has been broadly studied in the area of child development. Difficul- ties in regulating emotional behavior have been related to a variety of youth psychopathology and externalizing behavioral problems in particular. Multiple methods, including structured and semi-structured interviews, self-administered scales, behavioral observation, and neurobiological assessments, have been designed to assess emotion regulation in the young population. Specifi- cally, neurobiological methodologies (e.g., imaging, EEG/ERP, and ANS measures) have made tremendous contributions for the understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying emotion regulation deficits in youths with emotional and behavioral disorders.

Neurobiological studies have revealed that emotion regulation is a top-down process, in which the cortical neural network regulates the emotion that is generated from the subcortical neural network. Moreover, efficient emotion regulation requires both autonomic and effortful

33

regulatory control of emotion as mediated by the brain regions involved in the ventral and dorsal neural systems. Lower baseline PNS activity and abnormal PNS reactivity, reflecting emotion dysregulation and partly contributed by deficits in brain regions including the PFC and the ACC, have been related to aggressive and antisocial behavior.

34

CHAPTER 3 - PHYSIOLOGICAL REGULATION AND SUBTYPES OF AGGRESSION

PNS Activity, Emotion Regulation and Subtypes of Aggression

One possible reason for the mixed findings regarding the relations between PNS activity

and aggression is that most of the prior studies have not differentiated subtypes of aggressive be-

haviors; that is, instead of measuring RA and PA separately, researchers assessed aggressive be-

havior in a more general manner (e.g., the aggression subscale of the Child Behavior Checklist;

Achenbach, 1991). Considerable findings from prior literature have shown that the two subtypes

of aggressive behavior have distinct neurobiological correlates, as described previously. Moreo-

ver, understanding the relationship between emotion dysregulation and reactive-proactive ag- gression is crucial for better identification and treatment of youth with aggressive behaviors. In- deed, teaching better emotion regulation skills have been the essential elements of the cognitive- behavioral therapy used in youth with aggressive behaviors (Kazdin, Bass, Siegel, & Thomas,

1989; Sukhodolsky, Kassinove, & Gorman, 2004).

Although researchers have found RA to be negatively related to baseline RSA (Scarpa et al., 2010; Xu et al., 2013), and PA to be positively associated with baseline RSA (Scarpa et al.,

2010), direct evidence for the relations between reactive-proactive aggression and the PNS-medi- ated emotion regulation process, as indexed by RSA reactivity, is limited. Based on theoretical and empirical evidence mentioned in prior sections, we expect that RA would be related to lower baseline RSA and increased RSA reactivity, whereas PA is related to higher baseline RSA and reduced RSA reactivity. This hypothesis is formulated based on the following reasons.

Firstly, prior research has demonstrated that RA is related to increased ANS activity such as elevated HR and skin conductance reactivity to stress (Hubbard et al., 2002; Pitts, 1997). It

35

has been proposed that RA is “hot-headed” and characterized by autonomic over-arousal

(Dodge, 1991; Scarpa & Raine, 1997). It is therefore possible that RA is characterized by hyper-

active ANS activity, as reflected by higher RSA reactivity. Secondly, prior studies have shown

that low baseline RSA (Dietrich et al., 2007; Forbes et al., 2006) and excessive RSA reactivity

(Boyce et al., 2001; Calkins et al., 2007), or both (Hinnant & El-Sheikh, 2009), are associated

with internalizing symptoms in children and adolescents. Given that RA rather than PA shows

stronger correlations with internalizing behaviors (Card & Little, 2006; Scarpa et al.,2010;

Dodge et al., 1997; Raine et al., 2006), it is conceivable that RA would be related to low baseline

RSA and increased RSA reactivity, which reflect limited capacity of emotion regulation and the

tendency of vigilance and over-sensitivity to emotional stimulation when confronted.

In contrast, previous studies have shown that PA is related to reduced ANS activity such

as blunted skin conductance response to punishment (Gao et al., 2015; Lorber, 2004; Scarpa et

al., 2010). Individuals with higher PA are “cold-blooded” and are characterized by autonomic

under-arousal (Dodge, 1991; Scarpa & Raine, 1997). Therefore, it is possible that PA is related

to a hypoactive ANS, as indicted by lower RSA reactivity. In addition, PA has been associated

with psychopathic and callous-unemotional traits that are characterized by lack of guilt and em-

pathy (Fanti et al., 2008; Raine et al., 2006), and reduced empathy and lower level of prosocial behavior are associated with reduced RSA reactivity (Gill & Calkins, 2003; Liew et al., 2011).

Taken together, it is expected that PA would be associated with reduced RSA reactivity, reflect- ing a failure to engage with the environmental demands and immobilization responses in a chal- lenging situation (Calkins, 1997). Evidence from adult literature has provided some indirect evi- dence for this proposition. For example, in women with a history of sexual abuse, proactive but

36

not reactive relationship aggression was found to be related to blunted RSA reactivity in re-

sponse to a stressor (Murray-Close & Rellini, 2012).

Joint Effects of Baseline RSA and RSA Reactivity

Previous studies have focused mainly on the baseline RSA or RSA reactivity as inde- pendent predictor of aggressive behavior (e.g., de Wied et al., 2011; Pang & Beauchaine, 2013;

Scarpa et al., 2008), as both measures reflect important aspects of the PNS-mediated emotional processes (Porges, 2007). However, one study has suggested that considering the joint effects of baseline RSA and RSA reactivity is important in predicting behavioral problems (Hinnant & El-

Sheikh, 2009). In this study, researchers found that internalizing behavior is highest among chil- dren who showed low baseline RSA in conjunction with excessive RSA reactivity and that exter- nalizing behavior is highest among those showing both low baseline RSA in conjunction with reduced RSA reactivity in a group of eight- to nine-year-old children from the community. It is suggested that future studies examine the independent effect of baseline RSA and RSA reactivity as well as their joint effects to further understand the etiology of aggressive and antisocial behav- ior (Hinnant & El-Sheikh, 2009).

Biosocial Interaction

Both neurobiological and psychosocial risk factors are important for understanding the etiologies of aggressive and antisocial behaviors (Gao, Baker, Raine, Wu, & Bezdjian, 2009;

Obradović, Bush, Stamperdahl, Adler, & Boyce, 2010; Raine et al., 2014). There is growing evi- dence that the relationship between physiological activity and antisocial behaviors can be moder- ated by psychosocial factors, such as family adversity and history of abuse (Gao et al., 2009;

Maliphant, Hume, & Furnham, 1990; Raine et al., 1997; Raine & Venables, 1984; Scarpa &

37

Ollendick, 2003; Zhang & Gao, 2015). One line of this work has shown that the relationship be-

tween neurobiological risk factors and antisocial behaviors is particularly stronger in individuals without the psychosocial adversity background. For example, Raine and Venables (1984) found that low baseline HR is a risk factor for antisocial behaviors particularly in adolescents with higher socioeconomic status. Gao et al. (2009) found that the positive relationship between risky decision-making and callous-unemotional/psychopathic traits was only significant in children from benign home environments.

One proposed explanation for this interaction effect is the “social push” theory (Raine,

2002). According to this theory, if an individual is not exposed to social risk factors that “push” him/her towards antisocial behaviors, the neurobiological risk factors will likely be more mean- ingful in predicting antisocial behaviors. In contrast, if an individual is exposed to social risk fac- tors, then those influences will better account for his/her antisocial tendencies. The “social push” theory provides important insights into the interplay between the biological and psychosocial in- fluences. Alternatively, some studies have illustrated a different kind of biosocial interacting ef- fect. For example, Obradović et al. (2010) found that children with excessive RSA reactivity to stress showed more externalizing behavior symptoms in the context of higher levels of family stress. Likewise, in a group of Hong Kong schoolchildren, Raine et al. (2014) found that low baseline HR in combination with high social adversity predicted high RA but not PA. Taken to- gether, these findings contribute to the growing body of research that examines both biological and psychosocial factors associated with antisocial and aggressive behavior, and highlight the importance of investigating their interaction effects to obtain a more holistic picture of the devel- opment of antisocial behavior.

38

A few studies have examined the differential biosocial interaction effects in relation to

RA and PA. For example, in a group of undergraduate participants (N = 84), researchers meas- ured RSA activity during rest and an emotion-evoking decision-making task. It was found that

high RA was related to high baseline RSA at the levels of high social adversity and elevated

RSA reactivity at the levels of low social adversity; in contrast, high PA was related to reduced

RSA reactivity at the low levels of social adversity (Zhang & Gao, 2015). Raine et al. (2014)

found that the interaction effect between low baseline HR and high social adversity was specific

to RA, suggesting that psychosocial influences may play a more critical role in the development of reactive than in proactive aggression (Card & Little, 2006; Polman, Orobio de Castro, Koops, van Boxtel, & Merk, 2007).

Moderating Effect of Gender

Research relating physiological regulation deficits and aggressive behaviors has been mostly conducted with boys or male adults, partly due to the fact that boys are more likely to de- velop aggressive and antisocial behaviors than girls (Beauchaine, Hong, & Marsh, 2008; Keenan,

2006; Lorber, 2004). Recent studies suggest there may be different ANS mechanisms of exter- nalizing behavior for boys versus girls (Beauchaine et al., 2008; Gordis et al., 2010). For exam- ple, among 175 eight to 12 years old children, Beauchaine et al. (2008) found that boys with

more aggression and conduct problems showed lower baseline RSA and reduced PEP reactivity

towards reward (suggesting reward insensitivity in this individuals), whereas aggressive girls

showed similar physiological responses as the girls from the control group. Furthermore, as pre-

viously reviewed in Chapter 1, boys and girls show differential correlates of the two subtypes of

39

aggression. Therefore, it is possible that the relationships between PNS activity and subtypes of

aggression are moderated by gender. No studies have been conducted to address this issue.

Chapter Summary

Based on prior literature, it was hypothesized that RA would be associated with low base- line RSA and increased RSA reactivity, and that PA be characterized by high baseline RSA and reduced RSA reactivity. Evidence has also suggested that baseline RSA and RSA reactivity may

interact in predicting antisocial behavior. Finally, the moderating effect of social adversity and

gender should also be examined. Addressing these research gaps may provide important insights

into better understanding of the etiology of aggressive and antisocial behavior, and may have im- plications for development of treatment and intervention programs.

40

CHAPTER 4 – CURRENT STUDY

No study has examined the effect of emotion dysregulation on RA and PA in children us-

ing psychophysiological approaches. The current study examined the relationships between pat-

terns of both baseline RSA and RSA reactivity and the two different types of aggressive behav-

ior, and the moderating effect of social adversity. In an attempt to address these research ques- tions, data from a longitudinal study with eight to 10 years old children at the initial assessment and the follow-up visit one-year later were analyzed. Hypotheses were tested by examining the concurrent relations between measures of RSA, aggression, and social adversity (SA) at each time point, and the predictive relations between these measures from Time 1 (T1) to Time 2

(T2). The moderating effect of gender was also explored. Specifically, the following aims and hypotheses were tested:

Aim 1: The concurrent relations between RSA, aggression, and SA (for T1 and T2 separately)

Hypothesis 1.1. RA would be characterized by low baseline RSA and increased

RSA reactivity, whereas PA would be linked to high baseline RSA and reduced RSA reactivity.

Hypothesis 1.2. The combination of low baseline RSA and high RSA reactivity would result in highest score on RA, and the combination of high baseline RSA and low RSA reactivity would lead to highest PA.

Hypothesis 1.3. The above associations would be stronger at the lower compared to higher levels of SA.

Aim 2: Predictive relations between RSA, aggression, and SA

41

Hypothesis 2.1. Individuals with low baseline RSA and/or increased RSA reactiv-

ity at both time points would show highest scores on RA at T2.

Hypothesis 2.2. Individuals with high baseline RSA and/or reduced RSA reactivity

at both time points would have highest scores on PA at T2.

Hypothesis 2.3. The above relations are more salient under the condition of low levels of SA.

Method

Participants

The original sample consisted of eight- to 10-year-old children (mean age = 9.06, SD =

0.60) that were drawn from a longitudinal study that examined the social and neurobiological risk factors for children and adolescents living in Brooklyn, New York. Within the study area, fliers soliciting enrollment were placed in public areas and targeted mailings were sent to par- ents. Children with a diagnosed psychiatric disorder, mental retardation, or a pervasive develop- mental disorder were excluded. At T1, there were 340 participants, 164 of whom were male

(48.2%), with ethnic breakdown as follows: 52% Black, 21% Caucasian, 11% Hispanic, 2%

Asian, and the remaining 14% of mixed/other. At T 2 (one year later), there were 254 partici- pants, 122 of whom were male (48%), with ethnicity breakdown as 47.3% Black, 22.9% Cauca- sian, 8.6% Hispanic, and the remaining 17.9% mixed or other.

Data from the T1 and T2 assessment were used in the current study. Participants and their primary caregivers were invited to the university for a 2-hour laboratory visit including be- havioral interviews, neurocognitive testing, psychophysiological recording, as well as social risk factor assessment. Monetary compensation and transportation reimbursement were provided to

42

the participating families at the end of the visit. The university Institutional Review Board ap- proved all procedures. Parental consent and child assent were obtained for all participants.

Measures

Reactive and Proactive Aggression. The self-report Reactive-Proactive Aggression

Questionnaire (RPQ; Raine et al., 2006) (Appendix A) was used to assess the two types of ag- gression at both T1 and T2. There are 11 items (e.g., “Reacted angrily when provoked by oth- ers?”) assessing RA and 12 items (e.g., “Had fights with others to show who was on top”) as- sessing PA. Participants rated the frequency of occurrence on a 3-point Likert scale (0 = “never”,

1 = “sometimes”, and 2 = “often”), with higher total scores denoting higher aggression. Prior studies showed that the correlation between RA and PA range from .67 to .83 (Dodge & Coie,

1987; Poulin & Boivin, 2000a; Price & Dodge, 1989; Raine et al., 2006). In the current study, the Cronbach’s alpha internal reliability for RA and PA were .815 and .808 at T1 and .792 and

.782 at T2.

Social Adversity. Following prior literature, a social adversity index was created based on caregiver’s report on the psychosocial information questionnaire at T1(Choy et al., 2015;

Gao, Raine, Chan, Venables, & Mednick, 2010; Raine, Reynolds, Venables, & Mednick, 2002)

(Appendix B). The social adversity index was created by adding 1 point for each of the following

10 items: Divorced Parents (single parent family or living with guardians other than parents),

Foster Home, Public Housing, Welfare Food Stamps, Parent Ever Arrested (either parent has been arrested at least once), Parents Physically Ill, Parents Mentally Ill, Crowded Home (five or more family members per house room), Teenager Mother (aged years 19 or younger when child

43

was born), and Large Family (sibling order fifth or higher by age 3 years). All items are scored either 0 = “no” or 1 = “yes”, with a higher total score denoting higher level of social adversity.

Psychophysiological Data Acquisition and Reduction

At both T1 and T2, children’s RSA activity was recorded during a 2-min rest period

(baseline) and an emotion-regulation paradigm (Musser et al., 2011).

Emotion Regulation Task. The emotion regulation task (Musser et al., 2011, 2013) con- sisted of four 2-min-long film clips taken from the movie Homeward Bound, the story of three pets who are left behind when their family goes on vacation and try to find their way home, were presented. Children were instructed to either induce or suppress their emotion while their physio- logical activities were measured. The first two movie clips elicited negative emotions and the last two elicited positive emotions. In the induction condition, children were asked to show the emo- tion they experience of the main characters, and in the suppression condition, children were told to think about how the characters were feeling but not to show the emotion on their faces. The same sequence of the clips and instruction was given to each child: 1) negative induction, 2) neg- ative suppression, 3) positive induction, and 4) positive suppression. A 2-min rest condition was present for helping restore emotion between the negative and the positive conditions.

RSA. All psychophysiological data were collected continually using a MP150 system and analyzed offline with AcqKnowledge 4.2 software (Biopac Systems Inc., Goleta, CA). RSA was derived from the ECG100C amplifier with a band pass filter of 35 Hz and 1.0 Hz and a

RSP100C respiration amplifier with a band pass filter of 1.0 Hz and 0.05 HZ. ECG was recorded using the ECG100C amplifier with two pre-jelled Ag-AgCl disposable vinyl electrodes placed at a modified Lead II configuration. Respiration was recorded using RSP100C amplifier by putting

44

a respiration belt around the abdomen of the subject at the point of complete exhalation. The Ac-

qKnowledge automated function for RSA analysis was utilized. This software followed the well-

validated peak-valley method (Grossman et al., 1990), in which RSA was computed in millisec-

onds as the difference between the minimum and the maximum R-R intervals during respiration.

Higher values reflect greater PNS activity while lower values indicate lower PNS activity

(Gruber, Harvey, & Johnson, 2009). As all RSA variables are positive skewed, log transfor-

mations were applied before further analysis.

Baseline RSA was calculated as the average of the two 2-min rest periods at the begin-

ning and the end of the 40-min long psychophysiological protocol. Following prior literature

(Calkins et al., 2007; Musser et al., 2013), RSA reactivity was computed by subtracting the base-

line RSA value from the average RSA value during each movie clip, with negative values re-

flecting RSA withdrawal or increased RSA reactivity and positive values reflecting RSA aug-

mentation or reduced RSA reactivity. RSA reactivity across the four movie conditions was

highly correlated (r = 0.80 - 0.92), therefore, the average of the four RSA reactivity measures

was used in following analyses.

Missing Data, Skewness, and Outliers

At each time point some RSA data were missing due to acquisition or scoring problems,

such as equipment malfunction, extraneous movement (e.g., too much noise to detect R-waves), and electrode misplacement or displacement (e.g., improper placement of the respiration belt). In addition, a few participants have missing data in aggression and social adversity measures. Miss- ing data were assessed by the Little’s MCAR test, which showed that data were missing com- pletely at random (Little & Rubin, 1989) (all ps > .600, non-significant p values indicate data are

45

missing completely at random). Pairwise deletion was therefore used to maximize all data availa- ble on an analysis by analysis basis. Furthermore, given that at T1 RSA reactivity data from 40% of participants (N=136) were missing due to technical issues, independent samples t-tests were performed to determine if the remaining participants (N = 202) differed significantly from the rest on gender, ethnicity, and the main study variables. Results indicated that these two groups did not differ on any of these measures (all ps >. 272).

Prior to analyses, univariate outliers (±3SDs from the mean) were removed. For highly skewed variables (e.g., RSA and PA), outliers were removed after a log-transformation was ap- plied. A full description of missing data and outliers for the primary study variables of the indi- vidual and combined T1 and T2 datasets is presented in Table 3. T1 dataset (N = 340) and T2 da- taset (N = 254) were each used independently to examine the concurrent relationships. The com- bined T1 & T2 dataset (N = 254) with children participated in both assessments was used in the longitudinal (repeated-measures) analyses.

46

Table 3. Missing Data and Outliers for Each Time Point Separately and for the Combined Da- taset. RSA RSA-R RA PA SA T1 Missing 48 (14%) 136 (40%) 11 (3%) 11 (3%) 5 (1%) N = 340 Outliers 0 2 0 1 1 Available Cases 290 202 329 328 334 Means 127.96 -3.18 6.40 1.59 2.95 SDs 61.14 26.48 4.37 2.63 2.00 Range [24.75, 319.22] [-88.78, 78.25] [0,20] [0,15] [0,8] Skewness 0.90 0.064 0.59 2.53 0.50 T2 Missing 4 (2%) 31 (12%) 7 (3%) 7 (3%) 5 (2%) N = 254 Outliers 1 2 0 1 0 Available Cases 249 221 247 246 249 Means 125.43 -1.62 7.47 1.44 2.74 SDs 60.64 36.32 3.92 2.24 1.88 Range [28.91,351.30] [-118.77, 115.66] [0,19] [0, 13] [0,8] Skewness 0.98 0.16 0.41 2.55 0.51 T1&T2 Missing from T1 35 (16%) 103 (40%) 8 (3%) 8 (3%) 5 (2%) N = 254 Missing from T2 4 (2%) 31(12%) 7 (3%) 7 (3%) 0 Outliers from T1 0 0 0 0 0 Outliers from T2 1 2 0 1 0 Available Cases 215 133 239 239 249 Note. RSA-R = RSA reactivity; RA = reactive aggression; PA = proactive aggression; SA = social adversity.

Statistical Analyses

Aim 1- Concurrent relations between RSA, aggression, and SA. At each time point, hierarchical multiple regressions were conducted to examine the relationships between RSA and aggression, and the moderating effect of SA. In all regression models, RA or PA was the depend- ent variable. First, baseline RSA, RSA reactivity, and SA were entered to test for their main ef- fects. Second, the two-way interaction terms were added (i.e., baseline RSA x RSA reactivity, baseline RSA x SA, and RSA reactivity x SA). Finally, the three-way interaction term (i.e., base- line RSA x RSA reactivity x SA) was entered. All variables were mean centralized before anal- yses. Collinearity diagnostic tests were conducted and showed that the regression analyses were

47

not affected by multicollinearity (i.e., tolerance >.10 and variance inflation factor <10;

Tabachnick & Fidell, 2013).

Significant two-way interaction effect would be probed by conducting the simple effect

analysis of baseline RSA/RSA reactivity on aggression at high (1 +SD) and low (1 –SD) level of

RSA reactivity or SA. Should the three-way interaction be significant, the two-way interaction

between baseline RSA and RSA reactivity would be examined at the low (1 –SD) vs. high (1

+SD) levels of SA (Aiken, Leona, & West, 1991).

Independent samples t-tests were performed to examine gender effects. Should any sig-

nificant gender difference be observed for either aggression measure, gender would be entered in

the first step in the regression models to control for its effect, and regression analyses would then

be conducted again separately for boys and girls.

Given that RA and PA are highly correlated, many researchers control for the non-focal

subtype of aggression or use a residual score (i.e., RA score is regressed on PA score as to create

a “pure” measure of RA) in their analyses (Cima & Raine, 2009; Fanti et al., 2008; Lynam,

Hoyle, & Newman, 2006; Miller & Lynam, 2006; Poulin & Boivin, 2000b; Raine et al., 2006).

However, it has been argued that the interpretation of the findings after partialling might be prob-

lematic due to the high proportion of error variances (Lynam et al., 2006). In contrast, some re- searchers have suggested that the removal of the common variances between RA and PA could be helpful for detecting the unique differences between the two subtypes of aggressive behavior

(Miller & Lynam, 2006; Raine et al., 2006). It is therefore recommended that results for both the

“raw” score relationships and the partial relationship shall be reported (Miller & Lynam, 2006;

Raine et al., 2006). Following this suggestion, in addition to using the “raw” scores, the unique

48

variance of each form of aggression would be evaluated by controlling for the other form of ag- gressive behavior in the regression models. In line with prior studies (Fanti et al., 2008; Poulin &

Boivin, 2000b; Smithmyer, Hubbard, & Simons, 2000), all regression analyses were performed again with the non-focal type of aggression added as a covariate in the first step.

Aim 2: Predictive relations between RSA, aggression, and SA.

Baseline RSA. To test the hypothesis that individuals with low baseline RSA at both time points would show higher T2 RA score, and that individuals with high baseline RSA at both time points would score highest on T2 PA, three discrete groups were formed on the basis of whether participants fell into the top or bottom 50% cutoffs on two waves of baseline RSA measures.

Persistently high baseline RSA participants were defined as those who fell into the top 50% of baseline RSA at both time points, persistently low baseline RSA group were those who fell into the bottom half of baseline RSA at both time points, and the other participants were in the mixed baseline RSA group. F tests (for continuous variables) and Fisher’s Exact tests (for categorical variables) were conducted to examine group differences on RSA measures, socio-demographic data (ethnicity, sex, and SA) and aggression.

Two-way ANCOVAs with RSA group (persistently low baseline RSA, persistently high baseline RSA, and mixed group) and SA (high vs. low; median split, median = 3) were con- ducted for RA and PA separately when controlling for covariates and T1 RA/PA measure.

Should the interaction between RSA group and SA be significant, one-way ANCOVAs would be conducted to examine the effect of RSA group separately for individuals with higher and those with lower levels of social adversity.

49

Hierarchical regression analyses were also conducted with T1 and T2 baseline RSA, SA, and their two-way and three-way interactions as predictors. The T2 aggression measure was the predicted variable with the T1 aggression measure as a covariate.

RSA Reactivity. Similarly, three groups were formed: the “persistently increased RSA reactivity” group consists of individuals who showed increased RSA reactivity at both time points, the “persistently reduced RSA reactivity” group are those showing reduced RSA reactiv- ity at both T1 and T2, and the rest are in the “mixed group”. Two-way ANCOVAs were first per- formed, and then one-way ANCOVAs were conducted if the interaction between RSA reactivity group and SA was significant.

Finally, a regression was performed with T2 RA/PA as the predicted variable, T1 aggres- sion measure as a covariate, and the T1 RSA reactivity, T2 RSA reactivity, SA, and their two- way and three-way interactions as predictors.

Results

Aim 1: Concurrent Relations between RSA, Aggression, and SA

Correlations. Correlations between the main study variables at T1 and T2 are presented in Table 4. As expected, at both waves, RA and PA were highly correlated (r = .60 at T1 and .63 at T2, both p < .01). Increased RSA reactivity was correlated to PA at T1 (r = -.15, p < .05) but not T2 (r = .02, n.s.). Baseline RSA was not associated with either RA or PA at both time points.

SA was positively associated with RA (r = .11 at T1 and .23 at T2, p < .05) at both time points, and also with PA (r = .25, p < .05) at T2. Since boys showed significantly higher RA scores than girls at both T1 and T2 (p < .022), when predicting RA gender was entered in the first step to

50

control for its effect, and separate regression analyses were conducted to predict RA in boys and girls. Gender differences were not found for PA, ethnicity, SA, baseline RSA, or RSA reactivity.

Table 4. Correlations between Main Study Variables at Each Time Point T 1 RSA† RSA-R† RA PA† SA

RSA† 1.00

RSA-R† -.32** 1.00

RA .02 -.07 1.00

PA† .04 -.15* .60** 1.00 SA .16** .00 .11* .07 1.00

T 2 RSA† RSA-R† RA PA† SA

RSA† 1.00

RSA-R† -.30** 1.00

RA .04 -.03 1.00

PA† -.03 .02 .63** 1.00 SA .02 -.02 .23** .25** 1.00 Note. †Log transformed variables. *p<.05, **p<.01

T1. Regression analyses showed that the models with main effects (baseline RSA, RSA

reactivity, and SA) did not reach significance when predicting either RA or PA (for RA, p =

.485; for PA, p = .156), although RSA reactivity remained a significant predictor for PA (p =

.034). See Table 5 for the summary of regression results. Similarly, adding the two-way interac- tions to the model did not add significant predictive ability (RA, p = .221; PA, p = .860), alt- hough the baseline RSA x RSA reactivity interaction effect remained significant when predicting

RA (p = .029). Finally, the models including the three-way interactions did not significantly pre- dict RA or PA (for RA, p = .313; for PA, p = .325). When analyses were conducted for boys and girls separately for RA, results were substantially the same.

51

Table 5. Summary of Hierarchical Regressions for RA and PA: Predicting Effects of Baseline RSA, RSA Reactivity, Social Adversity and Their Interactions T1 RA PA† B SE t ∆R2 B SE t ∆R2 Step 1 0.03* No gender effect Gender -0.17 0.07 -2.39* Step 2 0.01 0.03 Baseline RSA† 0.03 0.07 0.47 -0.04 0.08 -0.55 RSA Reactivity† -0.10 0.07 -1.28 -0.16 0.08 -2.14 * SA 0.03 0.08 0.45 0.02 0.08 0.32 Step 3 0.02 0.00 Baseline RSA† x RSA -0.17 0.08 -2.20* -0.06 0.08 -0.77 Reactivity† Baseline RSA† x SA -0.07 0.08 -0.91 0.00 0.08 0.02 RSA Reactivity† x SA -0.01 0.07 -0.07 -0.06 0.07 -0.80 Step 4 0.01 0.01 Baseline RSA† x RSA -0.08 0.08 -1.01 -0.08 0.08 -0.99 Reactivity† x SA

T2 RA PA† B SE t ∆R2 B SE t ∆R2 Step 1 0.04** No gender effect Gender -0.22 0.07 -3.31** Step 2 0.05* 0.05* Baseline RSA† 0.04 0.07 0.52 -0.02 0.07 -0.31 RSA Reactivity† 0.01 0.07 0.13 0.03 0.07 0.43 SA 0.20 0.07 3.00** 0.22 0.07 3.20** a Step 3 0.02 0.08 Baseline RSA† x RSA 0.07 0.07 1.02 -0.04 0.07 -0.58 Reactivity† Baseline RSA† x SA -0.03 0.07 -0.51 0.01 0.07 0.07 RSA Reactivity† x SA -0.14 0.06 -2.14* -0.16 0.07 -2.50* Step 4 0.00 0.00 Baseline RSA† x RSA -0.05 0.07 -0.69 -0.03 0.07 -0.40 Reactivity† x SA Note. †Log transformed variables. ap<.10, *p<.05, **p<.01.

Regression analyses were also conducted when the non-focal aggression was controlled for in the first step. Similar results were obtained in terms of the main effects of baseline RSA,

RSA reactivity, and SA as well as their two- and three-way interactions for the overall sample

(RA, ps > .160; PA, ps > .225). Finally, when analyses were examined separately for boys and

52

girls, after PA was controlled for, adding the two-way interactions was significant in predicting

RA in boys only (p = .034), with the baseline RSA x RSA reactivity interaction effect being sig- nificant (p = .009). Simple effect analysis showed that after controlling for PA, higher baseline

RSA was related to higher RA in those with increased RSA reactivity (p = .039), whereas lower baseline RSA was marginally associated with higher RA in boys with reduced RSA reactivity (p

= .076) (Figure 2). No such effects were found in girls.

0.4 0.3 p = .039 0.2 p = .076 0.1 0 Increased RSA Reactivity -0.1 Reduced RSA T1 RA (Boys) (Boys) T1 RA -0.2 Reactivity -0.3 -0.4 Low Baseline RSA High Baseline RSA

Figure 2. The Interaction Effect between Baseline RSA and RSA Reactivity in Predicting Time 1 RA (controlling for PA) in Boys Only

T2. Regression analyses showed that the models including main effects were significant

for both RA and PA (for RA, p = .018; for PA, p = .017). In particular, higher social adversity

was related to higher RA and PA (for RA, p = .003; for PA, p = .002), although neither baseline

RSA nor RSA reactivity was a significant predictor for RA or PA (all ps > .602). See Table 3.

53

For RA, adding the two-way interaction terms did not reach significance (p = .190), although the

RSA reactivity x SA interaction was significant (p = .034). After controlling for the main effects,

the model with two-way interactions was marginally significant for PA (p= .053), with the RSA

reactivity x social adversity interaction being significant (p = .013). Simple effect analysis

showed that reduced RSA reactivity was marginally related to higher PA at low levels of adver-

sity (p = .053), whereas RSA reactivity was not associated with PA at high levels of adversity (p

= .150) (Figure 3). Finally, when regression analyses were performed separately for girls and boys for RA and when the non-focal aggression was controlled for, results were substantially the same.

0.5 0.4 p = .150 0.3 0.2 0.1 Low Social 0 Adversity High Social -0.1

T2 PA (Log Transformed) (Log T2 PA Adversity -0.2 p = .053 -0.3 -0.4 -0.5 Increased RSA Reactivity Reduced RSA Reactivity

Figure 3. The Interaction Effect between RSA Reactivity and Social Adversity in Predicting Time 2 PA (Combining Boys and Girls)

54

Summary. Hierarchical regression models were conducted to examine the effects of

RSA measures and SA on the two types of aggression at each time point. It was found that nei- ther baseline RSA nor RSA reactivity was significantly associated with RA or PA. However, af- ter controlling for PA, the joint effect of higher baseline RSA and increased RSA reactivity was associated with higher RA in boys only at T1. In addition, reduced RSA reactivity was margin- ally related to higher PA in the overall sample at T2, but this effect was only significant in the condition of low levels of SA.

Aim 2: Predictive Relations between RSA, Aggression, and SA

Correlations. From T1 to T2, the autocorrelations of baseline RSA and RSA reactivity were .50 (p < .01) and .14 (n. s.), respectively (see Table 6). Figures 4 and 5 illustrate the indi- vidual differences in the change pattern of RSA measures from T1 to T2.

Table 6. Correlations between Time 1 and Time 2 Variables T1 T2 T1 RSA- T2 RSA- T1 RA T2 RA T1 PA† T2 PA† RSA† RSA† R† R† T1 RSA† 1.00

T2 RSA† .50** 1.00

T1 RSA-R† .07 -.07 1.00

T2 RSA-R† .07 -.30** .14 1.00

T1 RA .10 .14* -.01 -.05 1.00

T2 RA .00 .04 -.01 -.03 .42** 1.00

T1 PA† -.02 .10 -.09 -.05 .59** .26** 1.00

T2 PA† -.03 -.03 -.08 .02 .31** .63** .33** 1.00 Note. †Log transformed variables. *p<.05, **p<.01. 55

Figure 4. Individual Differences in the Change Pattern of Baseline RSA from Time 1 to Time 2 (r =.50, p < .01)

Figure 5. Individual Differences in the Change Pattern of RSA Reactivity from Time 1 to Time 2 (r = .14, p >.05).

56

Baseline RSA. Three baseline RSA groups were created (i.e., persistently high baseline

RSA, n = 78; mixed baseline RSA, n = 57; persistently low baseline RSA, n = 80) based on

whether participants fell into the top or bottom half on baseline RSA at T1 and T2. Means, SDs,

and ranges for the three groups on these measures are listed in Table 7. Fisher’s Exact test showed that the three groups differed on ethnicity (p = .001): there were more Caucasian and

Hispanic but fewer African American participants in the “persistently low baseline” group, and more African American participants in the high baseline group than expected.

Table 7. Baseline RSA Group Comparisons on RSA, Sociodemographic and Aggression Measures Persistently Mixed Baseline Persistently T1 & T2 Dataset Low Baseline RSA High Baseline F / χ2 p η 2 RSA (n = 80) (n = 57) RSA (n = 78) T1 Baseline RSA † 1.86 (0.18) 2.03 (0.15) 2.24 (0.12) 126.71 a 0.000 0.54

[1.39, 2.07] [1.60, 2.47] [2.08, 2.50] T2 Baseline RSA † 1.86 (0.11) 2.07 (0.17) 2.22 (0.12) 145.45 a 0.000 0.58 [1.53, 2.02] [1.59, 2.41] [2.03, 2.49] T1 RSA Reactivity † -0.00 (0.10) 0.00 (0.11) -0.04 (0.07) 3.20 a 0.044 0.04

[-0.20, 0.21] [-0.23, 0.20] [-0.24, 0.15] T2 RSA Reactivity † 0.01 (0.11) 0.02 (0.12) -0.02 (0.11) 1.84 a 0.141 0.02

[-0.24, 0.34] [-0.27, 0.36] [-0.31, 0.26] Ethnicity (%)) 25.67 b 0.001 - Caucasian 25 (32%) 10 (18%) 13 (26%) African American 24 (31%) 27 (47%) 49 (61%) Hispanic 11 (14%) 1 (2%) 5 (6%) Asian 3 (4%) 2 (4%) 2 (3%) Mixed or other 17 (21%) 17 (30%) 9 (12%) Male (%) 38 (48%) 31 (54%) 38 (49%) 0.69 b 0.710 - SA 2.56 (1.92) 2.93 (2.04) 2.82 (1.54) 0.77 a 0.466 0.01 T1 RA 6.67 (4.27) 6.11 (4.23) 7.53 (4.69) 1.76 0.174 0.02 T2 RA 7.60 (4.08) 7.27 (4.31) 8.14 (3.49) 0.83 0.438 0.01 T1 PA † 0.29 (0.34) 0.27 (0.25) 0.34 (0.36) 2.27 0.106 0.02 T2 PA † 0.30 (0.31) 0.27 (0.26) 0.28 (0.32) 0.12 0.890 0.00 Note. a F test, b Fisher’s Exact test (Fisher exact test was used where at least 1 cell count was less than 5). η 2 is re- ported as an index for the effect size. †Log transformed variables. *p<.05, **p<.01

57

Two-way ANCOVAs with ethnicity as a controlled variable showed that after T1 RA/PA

was controlled for, the three baseline RSA groups did not differ on either RA or PA at T2 (for

2 2 RA, F = .32, p =.725, η = .003; for PA, F = .479, p = .620, η = .005). See Table 5 for means

and SDs. The interaction between baseline RSA groups and SA was also non-significant (for

RA, p = .370; for PA, p = .348).

Regression yielded similar results. After T1 RA/PA was controlled for, the interaction be-

tween T1 and T2 baseline RSA was not related to either RA or PA at T2, and that the moderating

effect of SA was non-significant. See Table 8 for statistics.

Table 8. Summary of Hierarchical Regression for T2 RA/PA: Predicting Effects of T1 and T2 Baseline RSA, and SA and Their Interactions T2 RA T2 PA† B SE t ∆R2 B SE t ∆R2 Step 1 0.18** 0.12** Gender -0.04 0.06 -0.65 No gender effect

T1 RA/PA† 0.39 0.07 6.06** 0.31 0.07 4.69** Step 2 0.02 0.03a T1 Baseline RSA† -0.03 0.08 -0.38 -0.07 0.09 -0.85 T2 Baseline RSA† -0.02 0.08 -0.29 -0.07 0.08 -0.81 SA 0.12 0.07 1.61 0.12 0.08 1.59 Step 3 0.01 0.00 T1 Baseline RSA† x T2 0.01 0.07 0.18 0.01 0.07 0.11 Baseline RSA† T1 Baseline RSA† x SA -0.02 0.09 -0.22 -0.06 0.09 -0.62 T2 Baseline RSA† x SA -0.07 0.09 -0.79 0.00 0.09 0.01 Step 4 0.00 0.01 T1 Baseline RSA† x T2 0.05 0.07 0.71 0.12 0.07 1.57 Baseline RSA† x SA Note. †Log transformed variables. ap<.10, *p<.05, **p<.01.

RSA Reactivity. Similarly, three RSA reactivity groups were created (i.e., persistently

increased RSA reactivity, n = 42; mixed group, n = 67; persistently reduced RSA reactivity, n =

58

27). The means, SDs, and ranges of the three groups are listed in Table 9. The three groups dif-

fered significantly on their baseline RSA and RSA reactivity at each time point. Group differ-

ences were not observed for ethnicity, sex, or social adversity.

Table 9. RSA Reactivity Group Comparisons in RSA, Sociodemographic Data, and Aggression Measures Persistently In- Persistently Mixed RSA Re- T1 & T2 Dataset creased RSA Reduced RSA F / χ2 p η 2 activity Reactivity Reactivity (n = 42) (n = 67) (n = 27) T1 Baseline RSA † 2.14 (0.20) 2.02 (0.22) 1.99 (0.26) 4.53 a 0.013 0.07

[1.50, 2.47] [1.45, 2.50] [1.45, 2.38] T2 Baseline RSA † 2.13 (0.17) 2.01 (0.19) 2.02 (0.23) 5.05 a 0.008 0.07 [ 1.68, 2.46] [ 1.61, 2.45] [ 1.53, 2.41] T1 RSA Reactivity † -0.08 (0.06) -0.02 (0.08) 0.09 (0.06) 48.76 a 0.000 0.43

[-0.24, -0.01] [-0.20, 0.17] [0.00, 0.21] T2 RSA Reactivity † -0.09 (0.07) 0.02 (0.10) 0.10 (0.10) 39.69 a 0.000 0.38

[-0.31, -0.01] [-0.24,0.26] [0.01, 0.36] Ethnicity (%) 7.99b 0.381 - Caucasian 12 (29%) 16 (24%) 5 (19%) Black 17 (40%) 34 (51%) 10 (37%) Hispanic 3 (7%) 3 (4%) 1 (4%) Asian 0 (0%) 1 (1%) 1 (4%) Mixed or other 10 (24%) 13 (19%) 10 (37%) Male (%) 21 (50%) 29 (43%) 19 (70%) 4.86 b 0.088 - Social Adversity 2.81 (1.73) 2.61 (1.94) 2.41 (1.80) 0.40 a 0.673 0.01 T1 RA 6.95 (4.74) 6.29 (4.56) 6.30 (3.37) 0.32 0.730 0.01 T2 RA 7.52 (3.71) 7/62 (3.93) 7.77 (4.11) 0.03 0.970 0.00 T1 PA † 0.67 (0.34) 0.30 (0.33) 0.24 (0.27) 0.36 0.700 0.01 T2 PA † 0.25 (0.29) 0.30 (0.31) 0.31 (0.30) 0.49 0.616 0.01 Note. a F test, b Fisher’s Exact test. η 2 is reported as an index for the effect size. †Log transformed variables. *p<.05, **p<.01

Two-way ANCOVAs showed that the three RSA reactivity groups did not differ signifi-

cantly on either T2 RA or PA (for RA, F = .31, p = .737, η 2 = .005; for PA, F = .76, p = .470, η 2

= .013). See Table 7 for means and SDs. The interaction between RSA reactivity group and SA

was non-significant (for RA, p = .958; for PA, p = .714).

59

Regression analyses also indicated that after controlling for T1 RA/PA, the interaction effects between T1 and T2 RSA reactivity were not significant in predicting T2 RA/PA, and the moderating effects of SA was non-significant. See Table 10 for statistics.

Table 10. Summary of Hierarchical Regression for T2 RA/PA: Predicting Effects of T1 and T2 RSA reactivity, and SA and Their Interactions T2 RA T2 PA† B SE t ∆R2 B SE t ∆R2 Step 1 0.17** 0.14** Gender -0.01 0.08 -0.11 No gender effect T1 RA/PA† 0.39 0.09 4.56** 0.36 0.09 4.16** Step 2 0.03 0.04 T1 RSA Reactivity† 0.05 0.09 0.54 0.01 0.09 0.10 T2 RSA Reactivity† 0.00 0.09 -0.04 0.07 0.10 0.48 SA 0.18 0.08 2.17* 0.21 0.09 2.41* Step 3 0.02 0.03 T1 RSA Reactivity† x T2 -0.05 0.08 -0.62 -0.06 0.09 -0.74 RSA Reactivity† T1 RSA Reactivity† x SA 0.08 0.09 0.92 0.13 0.09 1.49 T2 RSA Reactivity† x SA 0.00 0.1 0.01 -0.08 0.10 -0.82 Step 4 0.01 0.00 T1 RSA Reactivity† x T2 0.11 0.11 0.96 0.05 0.12 0.45 RSA Reactivity† x SA Note. †Log transformed variables. ap<.10, *p<.05, **p<.01.

Summary. To examine the predictive relations between RSA measures and aggression, both ANCOVAs and hierarchical regressions were conducted. Results showed that having higher or lower baseline RSA on both time points were not related to RA or PA at T2. Similarly, show- ing increased or reduced RSA reactivity across the two time points were not associated with ei- ther type of aggression at T2. Finally, the moderating effect of SA was not significant in all mod- els.

60

Discussion

This is the first study to examine both the concurrent and longitudinal (repeated- measures) relationships among physiological regulation (e.g., RSA measures), social adversity, and reactive-proactive aggression in children. The major findings are: 1) after controlling for PA, high RA was related to high baseline RSA in conjunction with increased RSA reactivity during emotion regulation, although these findings were only significant in boys at T1; no such effects were found for girls; 2) high PA was associated with reduced RSA reactivity during emotion reg- ulation in both boys and girls at the low levels of social adversity at T2, and 3) longitudinal (re- peated-measures) investigations showed non-significant relationship between RSA activity and the two subtypes of aggression.

Inconsistent with our prediction, high RA was found to relate to high but not low baseline

RSA. Furthermore, we documented an interaction effect between baseline RSA and RSA reactiv- ity in predicting RA. Specifically, higher RA was related to higher baseline RSA in conjunction with increased RSA reactivity in boys at T1. Both the baseline RSA and RSA reactivity relate to the PNS-mediated emotion regulation process (Hinnant & El-Sheikh, 2009) and have been asso- ciated with problems behaviors in youth (Beauchaine et al., 2007; Boyce, 2001; Calkins et al.,

2007).Elevated vagal tone may indicate disinhibited temperament (Kagan, 1989) and may reflect a predominance PNS over SNS activity or excessive vagal sensitivity, which may further con- tribute to lower HR and higher aggressive behaviors (Raine & Venables, 1984; Venables, 1988).

This finding is consistent with our prior work with adult population showing that high baseline

RSA and increased RSA reactivity in an emotion-provoking decision-making task was related to high RA but not PA, although it was only significant in the condition of higher social adversity 61

(Zhang & Gao, 2015). It is also worth noting that in the current and in Zhang and Gao’s study,

the interaction effect was only significant after partialling out the effects of the non-focal subtype

of aggression. These findings suggest that the combination of high baseline RSA and increased

RSA reactivity may be particularly helpful for detecting the unique differences between RA and

PA (Miller & Lynam, 2006; Raine et al., 2006). Taken together, findings indicate that ANS hy- perarousal, as indexed by high baseline RSA and increased RSA reactivity, may reflect emo- tional liability (Beauchaine et al., 2007; Beauchaine, 2001; Sloan et al., 1994), which in turn con- tributes to the development of reactive/impulsive aggression that is characterized by irritable and hostile response to stress or provocation.

Interestingly, when predicting RA we found moderating effect of gender: the interaction effect of baseline RSA and RSA reactivity was only significant in boys. In addition, boys were found to show significantly more RA but not PA than girls. The restricted range of RA scores

(particularly after controlling for PA) in girls may partly contribute to the null findings in this gender group. Alternatively, our finding may indicate differential underlying etiology for RA in the two gender groups. Recent research suggests that there are distinct biological mechanisms of externalizing behavior for boys versus girls (Beauchaine et al., 2008; Gordis et al., 2010). For ex- ample, Beauchaine and colleagues (2008) found that compared to the normal controls, boys with aggression and conduct problems showed reduced autonomic activity, whereas girls showed ele- vated electrodermal activity. Our finding of the relationship between RSA activity and RA only in boys provide further support that the neurobiological mechanisms underlying aggression may be different for the two gender groups.

62

We found that children with higher PA exhibited reduced RSA reactivity during emotion

regulation in the conditions of low social adversity at T2. This is consistent with the notion that

an under-aroused ANS is associated with PA specifically. Reduced RSA reactivity may reflect

deficits in regulation when encountering stressor (Murray-Close & Rellini, 2012), ineffectively

coping with the environmental demands, and immobilization response to challenges (Calkins &

Dedmon, 2000; Calkins, 1997). Taken together, reduced ANS responses, reflecting impairments

in emotion regulation and coping skills, may at least in part contribute to PA that is associated

with psychopathic traits and blunted affect (Fanti et al., 2008; Frick et al., 2003).

More importantly, we found that reduced RSA reactivity was associated with higher PA

at low but not high levels of social adversity. This adds to the prior literature indicating that the

links between neurobiological activity and aggressive and antisocial behavior might be stronger

in participants from a benign social background (Gao et al., 2009; Raine & Venables, 1984). As

argued by the “social push” theory (Raine, 2002), without the “push” towards aggressive behav-

ior, neurobiological risk factors (i.e., reduced RSA reactivity) may have stronger effect on the

emerge of the antisocial behavior.

It is interesting that the moderating effect of social adversity was only found significant

for PA in our study. In contrast, a recent investigation with 11- to 17-year-old children and ado- lescents from Hong Kong showed that the interaction between baseline HR and social adversity was significant for RA but not for PA (Raine et al., 2014). Differences in sample characteristics and the measure of ANS functioning may partly contribute to this discrepancy. Raine et al.’s study was with older population from Asia, whereas ours were younger (eight to 10 years old)

63

and from the U.S. In addition, Raine et al. examined HR, which is controlled by both parasympa-

thetic and sympathetic branches of the ANS, whereas ours focused on the parasympathetic-influ- enced cardiac activity in particular. The latter is believed to be a better indicator of physiological regulation. Future studies are needed to further investigate the biosocial effects in relation to the subtypes of aggression. Nonetheless, our results have documented the differential biosocial inter- action effect for RA and PA, and again highlight the importance of examining both neurobiologi- cal activity and psychosocial factors to better understand the distinct etiologies for the develop- ment of antisocial behavior.

Different concurrent relationships between RSA measures, SA, and aggression were found at the two time points. Furthermore, when examining the longitudinal (repeated-measures) relationships among RSA activity, aggression, and social adversity, we found that the baseline

RSA was relatively stable from T1 to T2 whereas the measures of RSA reactivity were not sig- nificantly correlated. Prior findings have suggested that vagal activity increases significantly dur- ing infancy and childhood, then gradually decreases or stays unchanged with development

(Korkushko, Shatilo, Plachinda, & ShatiloShatilo, 1991; Umetani, Singer, McCraty, & Atkinson,

1998). However, it is still unclear at what age vagal activity reaches maximum. For example, some research suggests that the baseline RSA peaks between four and six years old (Finley &

Nugent, 1995; Yeragani, Pohl, Berger, Balon, & Srinivasan, 1994), whereas others suggests be- tween 10 and 14 years (Korkushko et al., 1991; Massin & von Bernuth, 1997). In addition, there is some evidence that baseline RSA showed better stability as compared to the RSA reactivity

(Bornstein & Suess, 2000; Hinnant & El-Sheikh, 2009; Pang & Beauchaine, 2013). For instance, in a recent study in eight- and 12-year-old children and adolescents with conduct problems

64

and/or depression, baseline RSA was found relatively stable (inter-correlation ranges from .35 to

.54) across the three time points (each separated by one year), whereas developmental increases were observed in RSA reactivity in response to emotional film clips across time (not signifi- cantly correlated) (Pang & Beauchaine, 2013). The null findings of the longitudinal (repeated- measures) relationships among RSA, psychological risk, and reactive-proactive aggression in our sample, may be partly due to the large individual differences in the developmental patterns of

RSA reactivity during this age period.

Finally, to the author’s knowledge, the present study is the first to use the paradigm of emotion induction and suppression in response to both negative and positive emotion film in children in attempt to study the relations between physiological regulation and aggression. The same paradigm has been used in children with ADHD. It was found that children with ADHD, especially those with both ADHD and low prosocial behavior, exhibited a stable pattern of re- duced RSA activity across all task conditions, suggesting deficits in the physiological function- ing of emotion regulation in general (Musser et al., 2011, 2013). Similarly, we found that the

RSA reactivity measures were highly correlated when inducing or suppressing positive or nega- tive emotions, suggesting that the physiological regulation deficits observed in children with ag- gressive behavior was not specific to the valance (positive or negative) or the direction (inducing or suppressing) of emotion regulation.

Limitations and Future Direction

Limitations

There are several strengths to the current study. Emotion dysregulation has been consist- ently reported in children with aggressive behavior. In the current study, we have expanded on 65

these findings by investigating the two distinct subtypes of aggression (i.e., RA and PA) with re-

gard to the PNS-mediated emotion regulation process. Additionally, the current study used data

from a longitudinal (repeated-measures) design with a relatively large sample size, and focused on the change during the early years of development. Moreover, the potential moderating roles of psychosocial influences as well as the effect of gender were examined. Despite the strengths of this study, several limitations need to be acknowledged.

First, although the current study was conducted using a longitudinal (repeated-measures) design, due to the funding limit only two-time points were available. This may not be sufficient to fully capture the developmental change over time and it was impossible to investigate the non- linear relationship among variables (Burchina, Nelson, & Poe, 2006; Rogosa, Brandt, &

Zimowski, 1982). In order to make stronger causal inferences, longitudinal designs with three or more time points are required. In addition, since the social adversity was only assessed at T1, we could not determine whether the change of psychosocial influences (or lack of) may contribute to the relationship between autonomic abnormality and aggression over time. To address this issue, hierarchical linear modeling is preferred as it measures both within individual (change over time) and between individuals (RSA, social adversity, and RSA x social adversity) variations

(Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002).

Second, the aggression measures of RA and PA were limited to the child’s self-report, although our data exhibited good reliability across the one-year period, ranging from .782 to .815. Previous studies have shown that the correlation between the parent-report and the self- report of antisocial behaviors are low to moderate (Loeber, Green, Lahey, & Stouthamer-Loeber,

66

1991; Shakoor et al., 2011). Further research may benefit from integrating data from multiple in- formants, including parents, teachers, and children, to assess children aggression (e.g., Raine et al., 2006). Similarly, the current study has measured emotion regulation using the psychophysio- logical measure (RSA) only, although it has been considered a valid and reliable measure of emotion regulation (Beauchaine, 2015b). Research using multiple methodologies to assess emo- tion regulation, such as self-reports (e.g., Gratz & Roemer, 2004; Ochsner et al., 2002; Zeman &

Garber, 1996), observation methods (e.g., Cole, 1986; Cummings, 1987; Cummings et al., 1989), neuroimaging (e.g., Davidson, 2000), EEG/ERP (e.g., Dennis, 2010; Lewis & Stieben, 2004), and RSA, are needed for the future research to fully understand the emotion regulation deficits in relation to aggressive behavior. Structural equation modeling with latent variables may help miti- gate these problems by integrating multiple informants and measures.

Lastly, it is possible that the missing RSA data could have affected our findings, although the Little’s MCAR test (Little & Rubin, 1989) was conducted to ensure that data were missing completely at random. In addition, a series of independent samples t-tests indicated that those in- cluded and excluded from analyses did not differ significantly on any of the key variables. None- theless, future studies need to pay extra attention to the experimental processes involving psy- chophysiological data collection, to maximum the amount of usable data and thus increase statis- tical power.

67

Future Research

Identifying the neurobiological mechanisms underlying physiological regulation deficits has the potential to advance our understanding of how the brain processes and modulates emo- tional experience as well as the neural circuitry involved in emotional dysfunction in relation to

RA and PA. The neural changes associated with emotional suppression have been of interest given that the inability to suppress negative emotion is linked to psychopathology and aggressive behaviors (Schaefer et al., 2002). Emotional suppression has been shown to be accompanied by increased HR and SC reactivity (Gross & Levenson, 1997; Gross, 1998; Gross, 2002; Ohira et al., 2006; Richards & Gross, 2000), and neuroimaging studies have found that several prefrontal regions including the left lateral prefrontal cortex, the adjacent medial prefrontal cortex, and the medial orbitofrontal cortex, play critical roles in inhibiting activation of limbic regions during emotion suppression among healthy adults (Davidson, 2000; Ohira et al., 2006). In contrast, rela- tively little is known about the neural substrates of emotion induction. Furthermore, in practice, individuals may wish to up-regulate positive affect and down-regulate negative affect. Research has shown that different neural networks involved are in positive and negative emotion regula- tion in adults. The ANS reacts differentially to specific emotions (such as happiness, sadness, fear, etc.) during emotion regulation (Driscoll, Tranel, & Anderson, 2009; Ohira et al., 2006;

Reynaud, El-Khoury-Malhame, Blin, & Khalfa, 2012). For example, when comparing an emo- tion suppression task to an emotion attending task, suppressing emotion leads to a significantly lower HR when viewing a happy film, and a higher SC response when viewing a fear-inducing film (Reynaud et al., 2012). Additionally, neuroimaging studies have shown that regulation of positive and negative emotions is associated with both shared (e.g., the left superior and lateral

68

frontal regions) and distinct neural regions (positive emotion: the prefrontal regions and the left insula; negative emotion: the left orbitofrontal gyrus, the left superior frontal gyrus, and the ante- rior cingulate gyrus) (Mak, Hu, Zhang, Xiao, & Lee, 2009). Future studies are needed to disen- tangle the neural subtracts of regulation of specific emotions in children and adolescents.

Future research should also investigate at what age the effect of physiological dysregula- tion on aggression can be observed. The transition from infancy to toddlerhood has been argued to be a critical period for the development of emotion regulation (Beauchaine, 2001). In the in- fant literature, higher baseline RSA is considered as a psychophysiological marker of negative emotionality and difficulty (Fox & Field, 1989; Porges et al., 1994; Stifter & Fox, 1990). How- ever, higher baseline RSA indicates positive emotionality and social competence in later child- hood (Beauchaine, 2001; Porges et al., 1994). In terms of RSA reactivity, moderate levels of

RSA reactivity have been consistently associated with more adaptive responding and better en- gagement during challenging tasks in from infancy to adulthood (see Beauchaine, 2001 for a review). For example, even in 3-month-old infants, those showing RSA withdrawal were rated higher on soothability and had increased duration of orienting than those who showed RSA aug- mentation (Huffman et al., 1998). Previous studies have suggested that researchers must take caution in interpretation of the relationship between RSA and aggression in the developmental context. More research is needed to further understand the complex relation between physiologi- cal regulation and aggressive behavior in early childhood.

Future research should also benefit from examining the possible moderating effects of puberty stages on the relationship between psychophysiological regulation, psychosocial factors

69

and reactive-proactive aggression. A failure to analyze this may partially explain the missing pre-

dictive effects of RSA activity in relation to RA or PA from the longitudinal (repeated-measures)

perspective in the current study. The development of the secondary sexual characteristics is im-

portant, and can signal the onset of the physiological and psychological changes of profound im-

portant perspectives to the individual, family and society (Herman-Giddens et al., 1997). It has

been found that in the U.S. children are developing pubertal characteristics at younger ages as

compared with the earlier generations (Herman-Giddens, Wang, & Koch, 2001; Sun et al.,

2002). In addition, significant variations of pubertal development between genders and among

different ethnicity groups have been documented. The average age of entering puberty in girls is

approximately around 11, while in boys is around 12 (Sun et al., 2002). The timing of onset of

sexual maturation is earlier for both black girls and boys than for children from other racial

groups (Herman-Giddens et al., 1997, 2001; Sun et al., 2002). For example, non-Hispanic black girls showed earlier sexual development for pubic hair and breast development than Hispanic and non-Hispanic white girls (while no significant difference was observed between Hispanic and Non-Hispanic white girls) (Sun et al., 2002). Herman-Giddens et al. (2001) found that the average ages for onset of hair growth was 11.2 and 12.0 for black boys and White boys, respec- tively.

The timing of puberty has been associated with different adjustment problems, such as

depression and externalizing behavior (Negriff & Susman, 2011). Additionally, early maturing

girls and late maturing boys have been posited to display the highest risks of adjustment prob-

lems (Petersen & Taylor, 1980). Elevated testosterone levels have been found in the early matur- ing boys, and higher testosterone levels are linked to increased physical aggression, delinquency

70

and negative emotion (Archer, 2006). However, testosterone may lead to aggression via the aro- matization of testosterone into estrogen in girls, and that higher estrogens is related to more ag- gressive behavior (Finkelstein, 1997). Finally, prior research has shown that pubertal hormones may influence brain development, both structurally and functionally (Blakemore, Burnett, &

Dahl, 2010). For example, previous literature has shown that puberty stages are positively associ- ated with the growth of the amygdala volume in boys, though these finding are less consistent in girls (Bramen et al., 2011; Neufang et al., 2009). Since puberty may exert a complex and pro- found impact on changes of brain and behaviors, in the future studies to investigate aggressive behavior in children and adolescents, consideration should be given to the pubertal timing. Ac- cording to the prior literature, puberty stages could be measured using a golden standard called the Tanner stages in which puberty development has been classified in five different stages

(Tanner, 1962). Although it is unknown how puberty may affect the development of RA or PA, or RSA changes, future studies should include this measure to assess its effect.

Conclusion and Implications

In conclusion, the present study provides further evidence that there are different etiolo- gies for RA and PA. In particular, our findings indicate that RA is related to ANS over-arousal that contributes to emotion liability and difficulties in regulating behavior and cognition, whereas

PA is associated with ANS underarousal that contributes to lack of emotional awareness and dif- ficulties engaging. Although further replication is needed, our findings highlight the importance of investigating both the baseline and task measures of RSA, as well as the moderating effects of social adversity and gender.

71

Further understanding of the distinct neurobiological and psychosocial profiles for RA and PA may provide important practical implication for the design of more effective intervention and treatment techniques to the specific needs for the at-risk individuals (Kempes, Matthys, De

Vries, & Van Engeland, 2005). As described by Kempes et al. (2005), reactively aggressive be- havior might be influenced by the intervention that focuses on anger management and is de- signed to remedy higher level of arousal and anger reaction towards stressful life events. In con- trast, in order to reduce PA, intervention shall take the form of program that emphasizes on teaching children to become more attentive to the positive and the negative outcome of their own behaviors. In addition, future studies may consider using RSA as a non-invasive and reliable bio- logical measure to assess the efficacy of treatment programs that focus on improving emotion regulation in children and adolescents.

72

APPENDIX A: RPQ

Self-report Reactive-Proactive Aggression Questionnaire (RPQ) (Raine et al., 2006)

Instructions: There are times when most of us feel angry, or have done things we should not have done. Rate each of the items below by putting a circle around 0 (never), 1 (sometimes), or 2 (of- ten). Do not spend a lot of time thinking about the items—just give your first response. Make sure you answer all the items (see below).

How often have you…

1. Yelled at others when they have annoyed you

2. Had fights with others to show who was on top

3. Reacted angrily when provoked by others

4. Taken things from other students

5. Gotten angry when frustrated

6. Vandalized something for fun

7. Had temper tantrums

8. Damaged things because you felt mad

9. Had a gang fight to be cool

10. Hurt others to win a game

11. Become angry or mad when you don’t get your way

12. Used physical force to get others to do what you want

13. Gotten angry or mad when you lost a game

73

14. Gotten angry when others threatened you

15. Used force to obtain money or things from others

16. Felt better after hitting or yelling at someone

17. Threatened and bullied someone

18. Made obscene phone calls for fun

19. Hit others to defend yourself

20. Gotten others to gang up on someone else

21. Carried a weapon to use in a fight

22. Gotten angry or mad or hit others when teased

23. Yelled at others so they would do things for you

Scoring: RA items (1, 3, 5, 7, 8, 11, 13, 14, 16, 19, 22) and PA items (2, 4, 6, 9, 10, 12, 15, 17,

18, 20, 21, 23). Items are summated to form reactive and proactive subscale scores.

74

APPENDIX B: SOCIAL ADVERSITY INDEX

*These questions are part of the demographic questionnaire that the caregiver filled out during the initial lab visit between 2012 and 2014.

References: (Choy et al., 2015; Gao et al., 2010; Raine et al., 2002)

1. Current marital status of biological parents (select one from next line)______:

(Married =1; Widowed = 2; Separated = 3; Divorced = 4; Never Married = 5)

2. From ages 6 months to two and a half years of age, how many times was your child separated

from the mother (for any reason: illness, hospitalization, institutionalization, orphaned)

______?

If so, for how long (in weeks) did this last? ______

Was your child separated from both parents? YES NO

3. Do you live in Government home/apartment? YES NO

4. Were you ever on welfare or food stamps from the government? YES NO

5. Has the father or mother ever been arrested? YES NO

How many times? ______

6. Has the father or mother had a physical illness (e.g., heart, lung problems, etc.) that impaired

their functioning at some time during the child’s lifetime? YES NO

7. Has the father or mother had a significant mental illness (e.g., alcoholism, major depression,

schizophrenia, anxiety) that impaired their functioning at some time during the child’s life-

time? YES NO

8. Number of people living with family (including immediate and extended family): ______

Number of rooms (include bedroom, living room, dining room, kitchen): ______

75

9. Mother’s age at birth of child: ______

10. Birth order of the child: ______

Number of brothers: ______full ______half

Number of older brothers: ______full ______half

Number of sisters: ______full ______half

Number of older sisters: ______full ______half

76

REFERENCES

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the child behavior checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile.

Burlington VT: Dept. of Psychiatry, University of Vermont.

Adrian, M., Zeman, J., & Veits, G. (2011). Methodological implications of the affect revolution:

A 35-year review of emotion regulation assessment in children. Journal of Experimental

Child Psychology, 110(2), 171–197. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.03.009

Aiken, Leona S. & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting

interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications Inc.

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across

psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217–237.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

Archer, J. (2006). Testosterone and human aggression: An evaluation of the challenge

hypothesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(3), 319–345.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.12.007

Archwamety, T., & Katsiyannis, A. (2000). Academic remediation, parole violations, and

recidivism rates among delinquent youths. Remedial and Special Education, 21(3), 161–

170. http://doi.org/10.1177/074193250002100306

Arsenio, W. F., Adams, E., & Gold, J. (2009). Social information processing, moral reasoning,

and emotion attributions: Relations with adolescents’ reactive and proactive aggression.

Child Development, 80(6), 1739–55. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01365.x

Atkins, M. S., & Stoff, D. M. (1993). Instrumental and hostile aggression in childhood disruptive

behavior disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 21(2), 165–178.

77

http://doi.org/10.1007/bf00911314

Baker, L. A., Raine, A., Liu, J., & Jacobson, K. C. (2008). Differential genetic and

environmental influences on reactive and proactive aggression in children. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(8), 1265–1278. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9249-1

Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Oxford, England: Prentice-Hall.

Barbas, H., Saha, S., Rempel-Clower, N., Ghashghaei, T., Kling, A., Steklis, H., … Uhlrich, D.

(2003). Serial pathways from primate prefrontal cortex to autonomic areas may influence

emotional expression. BMC Neuroscience, 4(1), 25. http://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-4-25

Barratt, E. S., Stanford, M. S., Kent, T. A., Felthous, A., & Alan, F. (1997). Neuropsychological

and cognitive psychophysiological substrates of impulsive aggression. Biological

Psychiatry, 41(10), 1045–1061. http://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-3223(96)00175-8

Batty, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2006). The development of emotional face processing during

childhood. Developmental Science, 9(2), 207–220. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-

7687.2006.00480.x

Beauchaine, T. P. (2001). Vagal tone, development, and Gray’s motivational theory: Toward an

integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psychopathology.

Development and Psychopathology, 13(2), 183–214.

http://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579401002012

Beauchaine, T. P. (2015a). Future directions in emotion dysregulation and youth

psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 44(5), 875–896.

http://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2015.1038827

Beauchaine, T. P. (2015b). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: A transdiagnostic biomarker of

78

emotion dysregulation and psychopathology. Current Opinion in Psychology,

3(FEBRUARY), 43–47. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.01.017

Beauchaine, T. P., Gatzke-Kopp, L., & Mead, H. K. (2007). Polyvagal theory and developmental

psychopathology: Emotion dysregulation and conduct problems from preschool to

adolescence. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 174–184.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.008

Beauchaine, T. P., Hong, J., & Marsh, P. (2008). Sex differences in autonomic correlates of

conduct problems and aggression. Journal of the American Academy of Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(7), 788–796. http://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e318172ef4b.Sex

Beauchaine, T. P., Katkin, E. S., Strassberg, Z., & Snarr, J. (2001). Disinhibitory

psychopathology in male adolescents: Discriminating conduct disorder from attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder through concurrent assessment of multiple autonomic states.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(4), 610–624. http://doi.org/10.1037/0021-

843X.110.4.610

Beauchaine, T. P., & McNulty, T. (2013). Comorbidities and continuities as ontogenic processes:

Toward a developmental spectrum model of externalizing psychopathology. Dev

Psychopathol, 25(4pt2), 1505–1528. http://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579413000746

Bekker, E. M., Kenemans, J. L., & Verbaten, M. N. (2005). Source analysis of the N2 in a cued

Go/NoGo task. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(2), 221–231.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.08.011

Berkowitz, L. (1962). Aggression: A social psychological analysis. New York, NY, US:

McGraw-Hill.

79

Berntson, G. G., Cacioppo, J. T., Binkley, P. F., Uchino, B. N., Quigley, K. S., & Fieldstone, A.

(1994). Autonomic cardiac control. III. Psychological stress and cardiac response in

autonomic space as revealed by pharmacological blockades. Psychophysiology, 31(6), 599–

608. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb02352.x

Berntson, G. G., Cacioppo, J. T., Quigley, K. S., & Fabro, V. T. (1994). Autonomic space and

psychophysiological response. Psychophysiology, 31(1), 44–61.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb01024.x

Birbaumer, N., Veit, R., Lotze, M., Erb, M., Hermann, C., Grodd, W., & Flor, H. (2005).

Deficient fear conditioning in psychopathy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(7), 799.

http://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.7.799

Blair, R. J. (2001). Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders,

and psychopathy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 71(6), 727–731.

http://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.71.6.727

Blair, R. J. R. (2003). Neurobiological basis of psychopathy. The British Journal of Psychiatry,

182(1), 5–7. http://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.182.1.5

Blakemore, S. J., Burnett, S., & Dahl, R. E. (2010). The role of puberty in the developing

adolescent brain. Human Brain Mapping, 31(6), 926–933.

http://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21052

Bobadilla, L., Wampler, M., & Taylor, J. (2012). Proactive and reactive aggression are

associated with different physiological and personality profiles. Journal of Social and

Clinical Psychology, 31(5), 458–487. http://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2012.31.5.458

Bokura, H., Yamaguchi, S., & Kobayashi, S. (2001). Electrophysiological correlates for response

80

inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112(12), 2224–2232.

http://doi.org/10.1016/s1388-2457(01)00691-5

Bornstein, M. H., & Suess, P. E. (2000). Child and mother cardiac vagal tone: Continuity,

stability, and concordance across the first 5 years. Developmental Psychology, 36(1), 54–65.

http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.36.1.54

Boyce, W. T. (2001). Autonomic reactivity and psychopathology in middle childhood. The

British Journal of Psychiatry, 179(2), 144–150. http://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.179.2.144

Boyce, W. T., Quas, J., Alkon, A., Smider, N. A., Essex, M. J., Kupfer, D. J., … Steinberg, L.

(2001). Autonomic reactivity and psychopathology in middle childhood. British Journal of

Psychiatry, 179(AUG.), 144–150. http://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.179.2.144

Bramen, J. E., Hranilovich, J. A., Dahl, R. E., Forbes, E. E., Chen, J., Toga, A. W., … Sowell, E.

R. (2011). Puberty influences medial temporal lobe and cortical gray matter maturation

differently in boys than girls matched for sexual maturity. Cerebral Cortex, 21(3), 636–646.

http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq137

Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Boivin, M., Dionne, G., & Pérusse, D. (2006). Examining genetic and

environmental effects on reactive versus proactive aggression. Developmental Psychology,

42(6), 1299–1312. http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1299

Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., & Lavoie, F. (2001). Reactive and proactive

aggression: Predictions to physical violence in different contexts and moderating effects of

parental monitoring and caregiving behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(4),

293–304. http://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010305828208

Brenner, S. L., Beauchaine, T. P., & Sylvers, P. D. (2005). A comparison of psychophysiological

81

and self-report measures of BAS and BIS activation. Psychophysiology, 42(1), 108–115.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00261.x

Buchanan, T. W., Driscoll, D., Mowrer, S. M., Sollers, J. J., Thayer, J. F., Kirschbaum, C., &

Tranel, D. (2010). Medial prefrontal cortex damage affects physiological and psychological

stress responses differently in men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(1), 56–66.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.09.006

Burchina, M., Nelson, L., & Poe, M. (2006). Growth curve analysis: An introduction to various

methods for analyzing longitudinal data. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child

Development, 71(3), 65–87.

Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior

cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 215–222. http://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-

6613(00)01483-2

Cacioppo, J. T., Uchino, B. N., & Berntson, G. G. (1994). Individual differences in the

autonomic origins of heart rate reactivity: The psychometrics of respiratory sinus

arrhythmia and preejection period. Psychophysiology, 31(4), 412–419.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb02449.x

Calkins, S. D. (1997). Cardiac vagal tone indices of temperamental reactivity and behavioral

regulation in young children. Developmental Psychobiology, 31(2), 125–135.

http://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199709)31:2<125::AID-DEV5>3.0.CO;2-M

Calkins, S. D., & Dedmon, S. E. (2000). Physiological and behavioral regulation in two-year-old

children with aggressive/destructive behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology, 28(2), 103–118. http://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005112912906

82

Calkins, S. D., Graziano, P. A, & Keane, S. P. (2007). Cardiac vagal regulation differentiates

among children at risk for behavior problems. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 144–153.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.09.005

Card, N. A., & Little, T. D. (2006). Proactive and reactive aggression in childhood and

adolescence: A meta-analysis of differential relations with psychosocial adjustment.

International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30(5), 466–480.

http://doi.org/10.1177/0165025406071904

Casey, B. J., Trainor, R. J., Orendi, J. L., Schubert, A. B., Nystrom, L. E., Giedd, J. N., …

Rapoport, J. L. (1997). A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation

during performance of a Go-No-Go task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(6), 835–

847. http://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.6.835

Choy, O., Raine, A., Portnoy, J., Rudo-Hutt, A., Gao, Y., & Soyfer, L. (2015). The mediating

role of heart rate on the social adversity-antisocial behavior relationship: A social

neurocriminology perspective. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 52(3), 301–

341. SAGE Publications. http://doi.org/10.1177/0022427814565905

Cima, M., & Raine, A. (2009). Distinct characteristics of psychopathy relate to different

subtypes of aggression. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 835–840.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.031

Coccaro, E. F., McCloskey, M. S., Fitzgerald, D. A., & Phan, K. L. (2007). Amygdala and

orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive aggression. Biological

Psychiatry, 62(2), 168–178. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.024

Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1987). Mother–infant face-to-face interaction: The sequence of

83

dyadic states at 3, 6, and 9 months. Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 68–77.

http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.68

Cole, P. M. (1986). Children’s spontaneous control of facial expression. Child Development,

57(6), 1309–1321. http://doi.org/10.2307/1130411

Cole, P. M., Marin, S. E., & Dennis, T. a. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific construct:

Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child

Development, 75(2), 317–333. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00673.x

Connor, D. F. (2002). Aggression and antisocial behavior in children and adolescents : Research

and treatment. Guilford.

Connor, D. F., Steingard, R. J., Anderson, J. J., & Melloni, R. H. (2003). Gender differences in

reactive and proactive aggression. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 33(4), 279–

294. http://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023084112561

Connor, D. F., Steingard, R. J., Cunningham, J. A., Anderson, J. J., & Melloni, R. H. (2004).

Proactive and reactive aggression in referred children and adolescents. The American

Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 74(2), 129–136. http://doi.org/10.1037/0002-9432.74.2.129

Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-

processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 74–

101. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74

Crick, N. R., Dodge, K. A., Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing

mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67(3), 993–1002.

http://doi.org/10.2307/1131875

Critchley, H. D. (2005). Neural mechanisms of autonomic, affective, and cognitive integration.

84

The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 493(1), 154–166. http://doi.org/10.1002/cne.20749

Crowell, S. E., Beauchaine, T. P., Gatzke-Kopp, L., Sylvers, P., Mead, H., & Chipman-Chacon,

J. (2006). Autonomic correlates of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and oppositional

defiant disorder in preschool children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(1), 174–178.

http://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.174

Cummings, E. M. (1987). Coping with background anger in early childhood. Child Development,

58(4), 976–984. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1987.tb01433.x

Cummings, J. S., Pellegrini, D. S., Notarius, C. I., & Cummings, E. M. (1989). Children’s

responses to angry adult behavior as a function of marital distress and history of interparent

hostility. Child Development, 60(5), 1035–1043. http://doi.org/10.2307/1130777

Dambacher, F., Schuhmann, T., Lobbestael, J., Arntz, A., Brugman, S., & Sack, A. T. (2015).

Reducing proactive aggression through non-invasive brain stimulation. Soc Cogn Affect

Neurosci, 10(10), 1303–1309. http://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv018

Davidson, R. J. (2000). Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation--a possible

prelude to violence. Science, 289(5479), 591–594.

http://doi.org/10.1126/science.289.5479.591

Day, D. M., Bream, L. A., & Pal, A. (1992). Proactive and reactive aggression: An analysis of

subtypes based on teacher perceptions. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21(3), 210–

217. http://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp2103_2 de Castro, B. O., Merk, W., Koops, W., Veerman, J. W., & Bosch, J. D. (2005). Emotions in

social information processing and their relations with reactive and proactive aggression in

referred aggressive boys. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology : The

85

Official Journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, American

Psychological Association, Division 53, 34(1), 105–116.

http://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3401_10 de Kloet, E. R. (1998). Brain corticosteroid receptor balance in health and disease. Endocrine

Reviews, 19(3), 269–301. http://doi.org/10.1210/er.19.3.269 de Wied, M., van Boxtel, A., Matthys, W., & Meeus, W. (2011). Verbal, facial and autonomic

responses to empathy-eliciting film clips by disruptive male adolescents with high versus

low callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(2), 211–223.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9557-8

Dennis, T. A. (2010). Neurophysiological markers for child emotion regulation from the

perspective of emotion–cognition integration: Current directions and future challenges.

Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(2), 212–230.

http://doi.org/10.1080/87565640903526579

Dennis, T. A., Buss, K. A., & Hastings, P. D. (2012). Introduction to the monograph:

Physiological measures of emotion from a developmental perspective: State of the science.

Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 77(2), 1–5.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00654.x

Dennis, T. A., Malone, M. M., & Chen, C.-C. (2009). Emotional face processing and emotion

regulation in children: An ERP study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(1), 85–102.

http://doi.org/10.1080/87565640802564887

Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of

temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9(04), 633–635.

86

http://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579497001375

Dietrich, A., Riese, H., Sondeijker, F. E. P. L., Greaves-Lord, K., Van Roon, A. M., Ormel, J.,

… Rosmalen, J. G. M. (2007). Externalizing and internalizing problems in relation to

autonomic function. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,

46(3), 378–386. http://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e31802b91ea

Dodge, K. A. (1991). The structure and function of reactive and proactive aggression. In D. J.

Pepler & K. H. Rubin (Eds.), The development and treatment of childhood aggression (pp.

201–218). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information-processing factors in reactive and

proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 53(6), 1146–1158. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.6.1146

Dodge, K. A., & Frame, C. L. (1982). Social cognitive biases and deficits in aggressive boys.

Child Development, 53(3), 620. http://doi.org/10.2307/1129373

Dodge, K. A., Lochman, J. E., Harnish, J. D., Bates, J. E., & al, et. (1997). Reactive and

proactive aggression in school children and psychiatrically impaired chronically assaultive

youth. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(1), 37–51. http://doi.org/10.1037/0021-

843x.106.1.37

Dodge, K. A., Murphy, R. R., & Buchsbaum, K. (1984). The assessment of intention-cue

detection skills in children: Implications for developmental psychopathology. Child

Development, 55(1), 163–173. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1984.tb00281.x

Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., McClaskey, C. L., Brown, M. M., & Gottman, J. M. (1986). Social

competence in children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,

87

51(2), i. http://doi.org/10.2307/1165906

Dolan, M. C., Deakin, J. F. W., Roberts, N., & Anderson, I. M. (2002). Quantitative frontal and

temporal structural MRI studies in personality-disordered offenders and control subjects.

Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 116(3), 133–149. http://doi.org/10.1016/s0925-

4927(02)00085-9

Dolan, R. J. (2002). Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science, 298(5596), 1191–1194.

http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1076358

Dollard, J., Miller, N. E., Doob, L. W., Mowrer, O. H., & Sears, R. R. (1939). Frustration and

aggression. New Haven, CT, US: Yale University Press. http://doi.org/10.1037/10022-000

Donchin, E. (1979). Event-related brain potentials: A tool in the study of human information

processing. Evoked Brain Potentials and Behavior. Springer Science + Business Media.

http://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-3462-0_2

Dougherty, D. M., Dew, R. E., Mathias, C. W., Marsh, D. M., Addicott, M. A., & Barratt, E. S.

(2007). Impulsive and premeditated subtypes of aggression in conduct disorder: Differences

in time estimation. Aggressive Behavior, 33(6), 574–582. http://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20219

Drevets, W. C., & Raichle, M. E. (1998). Suppression of regional cerebral blood during

emotional versus higher cognitive implications for interactions between emotion and

cognition. Cognition & Emotion, 12(3), 353–385. http://doi.org/10.1080/026999398379646

Driscoll, D., Tranel, D., & Anderson, S. W. (2009). The effects of voluntary regulation of

positive and negative emotion on psychophysiological responsiveness. International

Journal of Psychophysiology : Official Journal of the International Organization of

Psychophysiology, 72(1), 61–6. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.03.012

88

Durbin, C. E. (2010). Validity of young children’s self-reports of their emotion in response to

structured laboratory tasks. Emotion, 10(4), 519–535. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0019008

Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A, Shepard, S. A, Reiser, M., …

Guthrie, I. K. (2001). The relations of regulation and emotionality to children’s

externalizing and internalizing problem behavior. Child Development, 72(4), 1112–1134.

http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00337

Eisenberg, N., Losoya, S., Fabes, R. A, Guthrie, I. K., Reiser, M., Murphy, B., … Padgett, S. J.

(2001). Parental socialization of children’s dysregulated expression of emotion and

externalizing problems. Journal of Family Psychology : JFP : Journal of the Division of

Family Psychology of the American Psychological Association (Division 43), 15(2), 183–

205. http://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.15.2.183

El-Sheikh, M., Harger, J., & Whitson, S. M. (2001). Exposure to iinterparental conflict and

children’s adjustment and physical health: The moderating role of vagal tone. Child

Development, 72(6), 1617–1636. http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00369

El-Sheikh, M., & Whitson, S. A. (2006). Longitudinal relations between marital conflict and

child adjustment: Vagal regulation as a protective factor. Journal of Family Psychology,

20(1), 30–39. http://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.20.1.30

Eysenck, H. (1997). Personality and the biosocial model of anti-social and criminal behaviour.

Biosocial Bases of Violence, 21–37.

Fabes, R. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1997). Regulatory control and adults’ stress-related responses to

daily life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(5), 1107–1117.

http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.1107

89

Fanti, K. A., Frick, P. J., & Georgiou, S. (2008). Linking callous-unemotional traits to

instrumental and non-instrumental forms of aggression. Journal of Psychopathology and

Behavioral Assessment, 31(4), 285–298. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-008-9111-3

Field, T. (2009). The effects of mother’s physical and emotional unavailability on emotion

regulation. Emotion, 59(2), 208– 227. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01286

Finkelstein, J. W. (1997). Estrogen or testosterone increases self-reported aggressive behaviors

in hypogonadal adolescents. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 82(8),

2433–2438. http://doi.org/10.1210/jc.82.8.2433

Finley, J. P., & Nugent, S. T. (1995). Heart rate variability in infants, children and young adults.

Journal of the Autonomic Nervous System, 51(2), 103–108. http://doi.org/10.1016/0165-

1838(94)00117-3

Fite, P. J., & Colder, C. R. (2007). Proactive and reactive aggression and peer delinquency:

Implications for prevention and intervention. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 27(2), 223–

240. http://doi.org/10.1177/0272431606294838

Fite, P. J., Colder, C. R., Lochman, J. E., & Wells, K. C. (2008). Developmental trajectories of

proactive and reactive aggression from fifth to ninth grade. Journal of Clinical Child and

Adolescent Psychology : The Official Journal for the Society of Clinical Child and

Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division 53, 37(2), 412–421.

http://doi.org/10.1080/15374410801955920

Fox, N. A., & Field, T. M. (1989). Individual differences in preschool entry behavior. Journal of

Applied Developmental Psychology, 10(4), 527–540. http://doi.org/10.1016/0193-

3973(89)90025-7

90

Frick, P. J., Cornell, A. H., Bodin, S. D., Dane, H. E., Barry, C. T., & Loney, B. R. (2003).

Callous-unemotional traits and developmental pathways to severe conduct problems.

Developmental Psychology, 39(2), 246–260. http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.246

Gao, Y., Baker, L. A., Raine, A., Wu, H., & Bezdjian, S. (2009). Brief report: Interaction

between social class and risky decision-making in children with psychopathic tendencies.

Journal of Adolescence, 32(2), 409–414. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.09.001

Gao, Y., & Raine, A. (2009). P3 event-related potential impairments in antisocial and

psychopathic individuals: A meta-analysis. Biological Psychology, 82(3), 199–210.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.06.006

Gao, Y., Raine, A., Chan, F., Venables, P. H., & Mednick, S. A. (2010). Early maternal and

paternal bonding, childhood and adult psychopathic personality.

Psychological Medicine, 40(06), 1007–1016. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291709991279

Gao, Y., Tuvblad, C., Schell, A., Baker, L., & Raine, A. (2015). Skin conductance fear

conditioning impairments and aggression: A longitudinal study. Psychophysiology, 52(2),

288–295. http://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12322

Gerstle, J. E., Mathias, C. W., & Stanford, M. S. (1998). Auditory P300 and self-reported

impulsive aggression. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry,

22(4), 575–583. http://doi.org/10.1016/s0278-5846(98)00027-x

Giancola, P. R., Moss, H. B., Martin, C. S., Kirisci, L., & Tarter, R. E. (1996). Executive

cognitive functioning predicts reactive aggression in boys at high risk for substance abuse:

A prospective study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 20(4), 740–744.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb01680.x

91

Gill, K. L., & Calkins, S. D. (2003). Do aggressive/destructive toddlers lack concern for others?

Behavioral and physiological indicators of empathic responding in 2-year-old children.

Development and Psychopathology, 15(1), S095457940300004X.

http://doi.org/10.1017/S095457940300004X

Gilliom, M., Shaw, D. S., Beck, J. E., Schonberg, M. a, & Lukon, J. L. (2002). Anger regulation

in disadvantaged preschool boys: Strategies, antecedents, and the development of self-

control. Developmental Psychology, 38(2), 222–235. http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-

1649.38.2.222

Gordis, E. B., Feres, N., Olezeski, C. L., Rabkin, A. N., & Trickett, P. K. (2010). Skin

conductance reactivity and respiratory sinus arrhythmia among maltreated and comparison

youth: Relations with aggressive behavior. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 35(5), 547–

558. http://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsp113

Gordon, H. L., Baird, A. A., & End, A. (2004). Functional differences among those high and low

on a trait measure of psychopathy. Biological Psychiatry, 56(7), 516–521.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.030

Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and

dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in

emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26(1),

41–54. http://doi.org/10.1023/b:joba.0000007455.08539.94

Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent

consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 74(1), 224–237. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224

92

Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences.

Psychophysiol., 39(3), 281–291. http://doi.org/10.1017.S0048577201393198

Gross, J.J., & Thompson, R.A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J.J. Gross

(Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press. Emotion, 3, 3–24.

http://doi.org/10.1080/00140130600971135

Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological

Inquiry, 26(1), 1–26. http://doi.org/10.1080/1047840x.2014.940781

Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes:

Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 85(2), 348–362. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting negative and

positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(1), 95–103.

http://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.106.1.95

Grossman, P., Beek, J., & Wientjes, C. (1990). A comparison of three quantification methods for

estimation of respiratory sinus arrhythmia. Psychophysiology, 27(6), 702–714.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb03198.x

Gruber, J., Harvey, A. G., & Johnson, S. L. (2009). Reflective and ruminative processing of

positive emotional memories in bipolar disorder and healthy controls. Behaviour Research

and Therapy, 47(8), 697–704. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.05.005

Gyurak, A., & Ayduk, Ö. (2008). Resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia buffers against rejection

sensitivity via emotion control. Emotion, 8(4), 458–467. http://doi.org/10.1037/1528-

3542.8.4.458

93

Hare, T. A., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Voss, H. U., Glover, G. H., & Casey, B. J. (2008).

Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an

emotional go-nogo task. Biological Psychiatry, 63(10), 927–934.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.015

Harmon-Jones, E. (2003). Clarifying the emotive functions of asymmetrical frontal cortical

activity. Psychophysiology, 40(6), 838–848. http://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.00121

Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). On the relationship of frontal brain activity and anger: Examining the

role of attitude toward anger. Cognition & Emotion, 18(3), 337–361.

http://doi.org/10.1080/02699930341000059

Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, J. J. B. (1998). Anger and frontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry

consistent with approach motivation despite negative affective valence. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1310–1316. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-

3514.74.5.1310

Harmon-Jones, E., & Sigelman, J. (2001). State anger and prefrontal brain activity: Evidence that

-related relative left-prefrontal activation is associated with experienced anger and

aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(5), 797–803.

http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.5.797

Harmon-Jones, E., Vaughn-Scott, K., Mohr, S., Sigelman, J., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2004). The

effect of manipulated sympathy and anger on left and right frontal cortical activity.

Emotion, 4(1), 95–101. http://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.4.1.95

Hastings, P. D., Nuselovici, J. N., Utendale, W. T., Coutya, J., McShane, K. E., & Sullivan, C.

(2008). Applying the polyvagal theory to children’s emotion regulation: Social context,

94

socialization, and adjustment. Biological Psychology, 79(3), 299–306.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.07.005

Hastings, P. D., Sullivan, C., Mcshane, K. E., Coplan, R. J., Utendale, W. T., & Vyncke, J. D.

(2008). Parental socialization, vagal regulation, and preschoolers’ anxious difficulties:

Direct mothers and moderated fathers. Child Development, 79(1), 45–64.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01110.x

Henderson, H. A., & Wachs, T. D. (2007). Temperament theory and the study of cognition–

emotion interactions across development. Developmental Review, 27(3), 396–427.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.06.004

Herman-Giddens, M. E., Slora, E. J., Wasserman, R. C., Bourdony, C. J., Bhapkar, M. V., Koch,

G. G., & Hasemeier, C. M. (1997). Secondary sexual characteristics and menses in young

girls seen in office practice: A study from the pediatric research in office settings network.

Pediatrics, 99(4), 505–512. http://doi.org/10.1542/peds.99.4.505

Herman-Giddens, M. E., Wang, L., & Koch, G. (2001). Secondary sexual characteristics in boys.

Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 155(9), 1022.

http://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.155.9.1022

Hinnant, J. B., & El-Sheikh, M. (2009). Children’s externalizing and internalizing symptoms

over time: The role of individual differences in patterns of RSA responding. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(8), 1049–1061. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9341-1

Hoptman, M. J. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of violence and antisocial behavior. Journal of

Psychiatric Practice, 9(4), 265–278. http://doi.org/10.1097/00131746-200307000-00002

Hubbard, J. A., McAuliffe, M. D., Morrow, M. T., & Romano, L. J. (2010). Reactive and

95

proactive aggression in childhood and adolescence: Precursors, outcomes, processes,

experiences, and measurement. Journal of Personality, 78(1), 95–118.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00610.x

Hubbard, J. A., Smithmyer, C. M., Ramsden, S. R., Parker, E. H., Flanagan, K. D., Dearing, K.

F., … Simons, R. F. (2002). Observational, physiological, and self-report measures of

children’s anger: Relations to reactive versus proactive aggression. Child Development,

73(4), 1101–1118. http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00460

Huffman, L. C., Bryan, Y. E., del Carmen, R., Pedersen, F. A., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., &

Porges, S. W. (1998). Infant temperament and cardiac vagal tone: Assessments at twelve

weeks of age. Child Development, 69(3), 624–35. Retrieved from

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9680676

Hum, K. M., Manassis, K., & Lewis, M. D. (2013). Neural mechanisms of emotion regulation in

childhood anxiety. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines,

54(5), 552–564. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02609.x

Jackson, D. C., Malmstadt, J. R., Larson, C. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2000). Suppression and

enhancement of emotional responses to unpleasant pictures. Psychophysiology, 37(4), 515–

522.

Johnson, E. O., Kamilaris, T. C., Chrousos, G. P., & Gold, P. W. (1992). Mechanisms of stress:

A dynamic overview of hormonal and behavioral homeostasis. Neuroscience &

Biobehavioral Reviews, 16(2), 115–130. http://doi.org/10.1016/s0149-7634(05)80175-7

Kagan, J. (1989). The concept of behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. In J. S. Reznick (Ed.),

Perspectives on behavioral inhibition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Retrieved

96

from

http://ezproxy.lib.utexas.edu/login?url=http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true

&db=psyhref&AN=2001.16276.0050091

Kazdin, A. E., Bass, D., Siegel, T., & Thomas, C. (1989). Cognitive-behavioral therapy and

relationship therapy in the treatment of children referred for antisocial behavior. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57(4), 522–535. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-

006x.57.4.522

Keenan, K. (2006). Emotion dysregulation as a risk factor for child psychopathology. Clinical

Psychology: Science and Practice, 7(4), 418–434. http://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.7.4.418

Kempes, M., Matthys, W., De Vries, H., & Van Engeland, H. (2005). Reactive and proactive

aggression in children. A review of theory, findings and the relevance for child and

adolescent psychiatry. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 14(1), 11–19.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-005-0432-4

Kiehl, K. A. (2006). A cognitive neuroscience perspective on psychopathy: Evidence for

paralimbic system dysfunction. Psychiatry Research, 142(2-3), 107–128.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2005.09.013

Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Hare, R. D., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B., Brink, J., & Liddle, P. F.

(2001). Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed

by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 50(9), 677–684.

http://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01222-7

Korkushko, O. V., Shatilo, V. B., Plachinda, Y. I., & ShatiloShatilo, T. V. (1991). Autonomic

control of cardiac chronotropic function in man as a function of age: Assessment by power

97

spectral analysis of heart rate variability. Journal of the Autonomic Nervous System, 32(3),

191–198. http://doi.org/10.1016/0165-1838(91)90113-H

Kruh, I. P. (2005). Historical and personality correlates to the violence patterns of juveniles tried

as adults. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 32(1), 69–96.

http://doi.org/10.1177/0093854804270629

Lane, R. D., McRae, K., Reiman, E. M., Chen, K., Ahern, G. L., & Thayer, J. F. (2009a).

Corrigendum to “Neural correlates of heart rate variability during emotion” [NeuroImage

44 (2009) 213–222]. NeuroImage, 45(2), 643–644.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.023

Lane, R. D., McRae, K., Reiman, E. M., Chen, K., Ahern, G. L., & Thayer, J. F. (2009b). Neural

correlates of heart rate variability during emotion. NeuroImage, 44(1), 213–22.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.056

Lansford, J. E., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (2002). Developmental trajectories of

reactive and proactive aggression: Similarities and differences over time. Paper Presented

at the International Society for Research on Aggression, Montreal, Canada.

http://doi.org/10.1037/e552582012-146

Leibenluft, E. (2011). Severe mood dysregulation, irritability, and the diagnostic boundaries of

bipolar disorder in youths. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(2), 129–142.

http://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10050766

Lewis, M. D., Granic, I., & Lamm, C. (2006). Behavioral differences in aggressive children

linked with neural mechanisms of emotion regulation. Annals of the New York Academy of

Sciences, 1094(1), 164–177. http://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1376.017

98

Lewis, M. D., & Stieben, J. (2004). Emotion regulation in the brain: Conceptual issues and

directions for developmental research. Child Development, 75(2), 371–376.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00680.x

Lewis, M. D., Todd, R. M., & Honsberger, M. J. M. (2007). Event-related potential measures of

emotion regulation in early childhood. NeuroReport, 18(1), 61–65.

http://doi.org/10.1097/wnr.0b013e328010a216

Liew, J., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Eggum, N. D., Haugen, R. G., Kupfer, A., … Baham, M.

E. (2011). Physiological regulation and fearfulness as predictors of young children’s

empathy-related reactions. Social Development, 20(1), 111–134.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00575.x

Little, R. J. A., & Rubin, D. B. (1989). The analysis of social science data with missing values.

Sociological Methods & Research, 18(2-3), 292–326.

http://doi.org/10.1177/0049124189018002004

Loeber, R., Green, S. M., Lahey, B. B., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1991). Differences and

similarities between children, mothers, and teachers as informants on disruptive child

behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 19(1), 75–95.

http://doi.org/10.1007/BF00910566

Loney, B. R., Frick, P. J., Ellis, M., & McCoy, M. G. (1998). Intelligence, callous- unemotional

traits, and antisocial behavior. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,

20(3), 231–247. http://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023015318156

Lopez-Duran, N. L., Hajal, N. J., Olson, S. L., Felt, B. T., & Vazquez, D. M. (2009). Individual

differences in cortisol responses to fear and frustration during middle childhood. Journal of

99

Experimental Child Psychology, 103(3), 285–295. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.03.008

Lopez-Duran, N. L., Olson, S. L., Hajal, N. J., Felt, B. T., & Vazquez, D. M. (2009).

Hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis functioning in reactive and proactive aggression in

children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(2), 169–182.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9263-3

Lorber, M. F. (2004). Psychophysiology of aggression, psychopathy, and conduct problems: A

meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 130(4), 531–552. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-

2909.130.4.531

Luria, A. R. (1980). Higher cortical functions in man. Boston, MA: Springer US.

http://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-8579-4

Lynam, D. R., Hoyle, R. H., & Newman, J. P. (2006). The perils of partialling: Cautionary tales

from aggression and psychopathy. Assessment, 13(3), 328–41.

http://doi.org/10.1177/1073191106290562

Mak, A. K. Y., Hu, Z., Zhang, J. X., Xiao, Z., & Lee, T. M. C. (2009). Neural correlates of

regulation of positive and negative emotions: An fMRI study. Neuroscience Letters, 457(2),

101–106. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2009.03.094

Maliphant, R., Hume, F., & Furnham, A. (1990). Autonomic nervous system (ANS) activity,

personality characteristics and disruptive behaviour in girls. Journal of Child Psychology

and Psychiatry, 31(4), 619–628. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb00801.x

Mangun, G. R., & Hillyard, S. A. (1996). Mechanisms and models of selective attention. In

Electrophysiology of Mind (pp. 40–85). Oxford University Press.

http://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524168.003.0003

100

Marcovitch, S., Leigh, J., Calkins, S. D., Leerks, E. M., O’Brien, M., & Blankson, A. N. (2010).

Moderate vagal withdrawal in 3.5-year-old children is associated with optimal performance

on executive function tasks. Developmental Psychobiology, 52(6), 603–608.

http://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20462

Marsee, M. A., & Frick, P. J. (2007). Exploring the cognitive and emotional correlates to

proactive and reactive aggression in a sample of detained girls. Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology, 35(6), 969–981. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-007-9147-y

Massin, M., & von Bernuth, G. (1997). Normal ranges of heart rate variability during infancy

and childhood. Pediatric Cardiology, 18(4), 297–302.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s002469900178

Maughan, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2002). Impact of child maltreatment and interadult violence on

children’s emotion regulation abilities and socioemotional adjustment. Child Development,

73(5), 1525–1542. http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00488

McAuliffe, M. D., Hubbard, J. A., Rubin, R. M., Morrow, M. T., & Dearing, K. F. (2007).

Reactive and proactive aggression: Stability of constructs and relations to correlates. The

Journal of Genetic Psychology, 167(4), 365–382. http://doi.org/10.3200/gntp.167.4.365-382

Mcburnett, K., Lahey, B. B., Frick, P. J., Risch, C., Loeber, R., Hart, E. L., … Hanson, K. S.

(1991). Anxiety, inhibition, and conduct disorder in children: II. Relation to salivary

cortisol. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 30(2), 192–

196. http://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199103000-00005

McBurnett, K., Lahey, B. B., Rathouz, P. J., & Loeber, R. (2000). Low salivary cortisol and

persistent aggression in boys referred for disruptive behavior. Archives of General

101

Psychiatry, 57(1), 38. http://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.38

McHale, B. G., Obrzut, J. E., & Sabers, D. L. (2003). Relationship of cognitive functioning and

aggressive behavior with emotionally disabled and specific learning disabled students.

Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 15(2), 123–140.

http://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022823316505

Miles, D. R., & Carey, G. (1997). Genetic and environmental architecture on human aggression.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(1), 207–217. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-

3514.72.1.207

Milich, R., & Dodge, K. A. (1984). Social information processing in child psychiatric

populations. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12(3), 471–489.

http://doi.org/10.1007/bf00910660

Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2006). Reactive and proactive aggression: Similarities and

differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 41(8), 1469–1480.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.06.004

Moffitt, T. E., Lynam, D. R., & Silva, P. A. (1994). Neuropsychological tests predicting

persistent male delinquency. Criminology, 32(2), 277–300. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-

9125.1994.tb01155.x

Monk, C. S., Telzer, E. H., Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Mai, X., Louro, H. M. C., … Pine, D. S.

(2008). Amygdala and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation to masked angry faces in

children and adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry,

65(5), 568. http://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.65.5.568

Morris, A. S., Robinson, L. R., & Eisenberg, N. (2006). Applying a multimethod perspective to

102

the study of developmental psychology. Handbook of Multimethod Measurement in

Psychology. American Psychological Association (APA). http://doi.org/10.1037/11383-025

Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., & Robinson, L. R. (2009). The role of the family context

in the development of emotion regulation. Soc Dev, 16(2), 1–26.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x.The

Murray-Close, D., & Rellini, A. H. (2012). Cardiovascular reactivity and proactive and reactive

relational aggression among women with and without a history of sexual abuse. Biological

Psychology, 89(1), 54–62. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.09.008

Musser, E. D., Backs, R. W., Schmitt, C. F., Ablow, J. C., Measelle, J. R., & Nigg, J. T. (2011).

Emotion regulation via the autonomic nervous system in children with attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(6), 841–

852. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9499-1

Musser, E. D., Galloway-Long, H. S., Frick, P. J., & Nigg, J. T. (2013). Emotion regulation and

heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy

of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(2), 163–171.e2.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.11.009

Negriff, S., & Susman, E. J. (2011). Pubertal timing, depression, and externalizing problems: A

framework, review, and examination of gender differences. Journal of Research on

Adolescence, 21(3), 717–746. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00708.x

Nelson, C. A., & Nugent, K. M. (1990). Recognition memory and resource allocation as revealed

by children’s event-related potential responses to happy and angry faces. Developmental

Psychology, 26(2), 171–179. http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.26.2.171

103

Nelson, W. M., & Finch, A. J. (2000). Managing anger in youth: A cognitive- behavioral

intervention approach. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Child and adolescent therapy: Cognitive

behavioral procedures (2nd ed., pp. 129–170). New York: Guilford.

Neufang, S., Specht, K., Hausmann, M., Gunturkun, O., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Fink, G. R., &

Konrad, K. (2009). Sex differences and the impact of steroid hormones on the developing

human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 19(2), 464–473. http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn100

Obradović, J., Bush, N. R., Stamperdahl, J., Adler, N. E., & Boyce, W. T. (2010). Biological

sensitivity to context: The interactive effects of stress reactivity and family adversity on

socioemotional behavior and school readiness. Child Development, 81(1), 270–289.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01394.x

Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Rethinking feelings: An

fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,

14(8), 1215–1229. http://doi.org/10.1162/089892902760807212

Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J. D. E., &

Gross, J. J. (2004). For better or for worse: Neural systems supporting the cognitive down-

and up-regulation of negative emotion. NeuroImage, 23(2), 483–499.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.030

Ohira, H., Nomura, M., Ichikawa, N., Isowa, T., Iidaka, T., Sato, A., … Yamada, J. (2006).

Association of neural and physiological responses during voluntary emotion suppression.

NeuroImage, 29(3), 721–733. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.047

Ortiz, J., & Raine, A. (2004). Heart rate level and antisocial behavior in children and

adolescents: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

104

Psychiatry, 43(2), 154–162. http://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200402000-00010

Pang, K. C., & Beauchaine, T. (2013). Longitudinal patterns of autonomic nervous system

responding to emotion evocation among children with conduct problems and/or depression.

Developmental Psychobiology, 55(7), 698–706. http://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21065

Pang, K. C., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2012). Longitudinal patterns of autonomic nervous system

responding to emotion evocation among children with conduct problems and/or depression.

Developmental Psychobiology, 72(2), n/a–n/a. http://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21065

Patrick, C. J. (2008). Psychophysiological correlates of aggression and violence: An integrative

review. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological

Sciences, 363(1503), 2543–2555. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0028

Patriquin, M. A., Scarpa, A., Friedman, B. H., & Porges, S. W. (2013). Respiratory sinus

arrhythmia: A marker for positive social functioning and receptive language skills in

children with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(2), 101–112.

http://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21002

Petersen, A. C., & Taylor, B. (1980). The biological approach to adolescence. In Handbook of

adolescent psychology (Adelson, J, pp. 117–155). New York, NY: Wiley.

Peterson, C. K., Shackman, A. J., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). The role of asymmetrical frontal

cortical activity in aggression. Psychophysiology, 45(1), 86–92.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00597.x

Pitts, T. B. (1997). Reduced heart rate levels in aggressive children. Biosocial Bases of Violence.

Springer Science + Business Media, 317–320. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-4648-

8_22

105

Plomin, R., Nitz, K., & Rowe, D. C. (1990). Behavioral genetics and aggressive behavior in

childhood. Handbook of Developmental Psychopathology. Springer Science + Business

Media, 119–133. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-7142-1_10

Polman, H., Orobio de Castro, B., Koops, W., van Boxtel, H. W., & Merk, W. W. (2007). A

meta-analysis of the distinction between reactive and proactive aggression in children and

adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(4), 522–535.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-007-9109-4

Porges, S. W. (1995). Cardiac vagal tone: A physiological index of stress. Neuroscience &

Biobehavioral Reviews, 19(2), 225–233. http://doi.org/10.1016/0149-7634(94)00066-A

Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 116–143.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009

Porges, S. W., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., & Maiti, A. K. (1994). Vagal tone and the

physiological regulation of emotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child

Development, 59(2-3), 167–186. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01283.x

Poulin, F., & Boivin, M. (2000a). Reactive and proactive aggression: Evidence of a two-factor

model. Psychological Assessment, 12(2), 115–122. http://doi.org/10.1037/1040-

3590.12.2.115

Poulin, F., & Boivin, M. (2000b). The role of proactive and reactive aggression in the formation

and development of boys’ friendships. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 233–240.

http://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.36.2.233

Price, J. M., & Dodge, K. A. (1989). Reactive and proactive aggression in childhood: Relations

to peer status and social context dimensions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17(4),

106

455–471. http://doi.org/10.1007/bf00915038

Pulkkinen, L. (1996). Proactive and reactive aggression in early adolescence as precursors to

anti- and prosocial behavior in young adults. Aggressive Behavior, 22(4), 241–257.

http://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1996)22:4<241::AID-AB1>3.0.CO;2-O

Raine, A. (2002). Biosocial studies of antisocial and violent behavior in children and adults: A

review. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30(4), 311–326.

http://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015754122318

Raine, A., Dodge, K., Loeber, R., Gatzke-Kopp, L., Lynam, D., Reynolds, C., … Liu, J. (2006).

The reactive–proactive aggression questionnaire: Differential correlates of reactive and

proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggressive Behavior, 32(2), 159–171.

http://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20115

Raine, A., Fung, A. L. C., Portnoy, J., Choy, O., & Spring, V. L. (2014). Low heart rate as a risk

factor for child and adolescent proactive aggressive and impulsive psychopathic behavior.

Aggressive Behavior, 40(November 2013), 290–299. http://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21523

Raine, A., Meloy, J. R., Bihrle, S., Stoddard, J., Lacasse, L., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1998).

Reduced prefrontal and increased subcortical brain functioning assessed using positron

emission tomography in predatory and affective murderers. Behavioral Sciences & the Law,

16(3), 319–332. http://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0798(199822)16:3<319::AID-

BSL311>3.0.CO;2-G

Raine, A., Reynolds, C., Venables, P. H., & Mednick, S. A. (2002). Stimulation seeking and

intelligence: A prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 82(4), 663–674. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.4.663

107

Raine, A., & Venables, P. H. (1984). Tonic heart rate level, social class and antisocial behaviour

in adolescents. Biological Psychology, 18(2), 123–132. http://doi.org/10.1016/0301-

0511(84)90015-2

Raine, A., Venables, P. H., & Mednick, S. A. (1997). Low resting heart rate at age 3 years

predisposes to aggression at age 11 years: Evidence from the Mauritius child health project.

Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(10), 1457–1464.

http://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199710000-00029

Raine, A., Venables, P. H., & Williams, M. (1990). Relationships between N1, P300, and

contingent negative variation recorded at age 15 and criminal behavior at age 24, 27(5),

567–575. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb01978.x

Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data

analysis methods. SAGE.

Reed, J., & Warner-Rogers, J. (2009). Child neuropsychology: Concepts, theory, and practice.

John Wiley & Sons.

Reynaud, E., El-Khoury-Malhame, M., Blin, O., & Khalfa, S. (2012). Voluntary emotion

suppression modifies psychophysiological responses to films. Journal of Psychophysiology,

26(3), 116–123. http://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000074

Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: The cognitive costs of

keeping one’s cool. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(3), 410–424.

http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.410

Rogosa, D., Brandt, D., & Zimowski, M. (1982). A growth curve approach to the measurement

of change. Psychological Bulletin, 92(3), 726–748. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-

108

2909.92.3.726

Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 11–29.

http://doi.org/10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00277-x

Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Temperament and social behavior in

childhood. Journal of Developmental Psychology, 40(1), 21–39. Retrieved from

http://www.jstor.org/stable/23087906

Rottenberg, J. (2007). Cardiac vagal control in depression: A critical analysis. Biological

Psychology, 74(2), 200–211. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.010

Rybak, M., Crayton, J. W., Young, I. J., Herba, E., & Konopka, L. M. (2006). Frontal alpha

power asymmetry in aggressive children and adolescents with mood and disruptive

behavior disorders. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 37(1), 16–24.

http://doi.org/10.1177/155005940603700105

Saarni, C. (1984). An observational study of children’s attempts to monitor their expressive

behavior. Child Development, 55(4), 1504. http://doi.org/10.2307/1130020

Salmivalli, C., & Nieminen, E. (2002). Proactive and reactive aggression among school bullies,

victims, and bully-victims. Aggressive Behavior, 28(1), 30–44.

http://doi.org/10.1002/ab.90004

Scarpa, A., Fikretoglu, D., & Luscher, K. (2000). Community violence exposure in a young adult

sample: II. Psychophysiology and aggressive behavior. Journal of Community Psychology,

28(4), 417–425. http://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6629(200007)28:4<417::aid-jcop4>3.0.co;2-l

Scarpa, A., Haden, S. C., & Tanaka, A. (2010). Being hot-tempered: Autonomic, emotional, and

behavioral distinctions between childhood reactive and proactive aggression. Biological

109

Psychology, 84(3), 488–496. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.11.006

Scarpa, A., & Ollendick, T. H. (2003). Community violence exposure in a young adult sample:

III. Psychophysiology and victimization interact to affect risk for aggression. Journal of

Community Psychology, 31(4), 321–338. http://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.10058

Scarpa, A., & Raine, A. (1997). Psychophysiology of anger and violent behavior. Psychiatric

Clinics of North America, 20(2), 375–394. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-953X(05)70318-X

Scarpa, A., Tanaka, A., & Chiara Haden, S. (2008). Biosocial bases of reactive and proactive

aggression: the roles of community violence exposure and heart rate. Journal of Community

Psychology, 36(8), 969–988. http://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.20276

Schaefer, S. M., Jackson, D. C., Davidson, R. J., Aguirre, G. K., Kimberg, D. Y., & Thompson-

Schill, S. L. (2002). Modulation of amygdalar activity by the conscious regulation of

negative emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(6), 913–921.

http://doi.org/10.1162/089892902760191135

Shakoor, S., Jaffee, S. R., Andreou, P., Bowes, L., Ambler, A. P., Caspi, A., … Arseneault, L.

(2011). Mothers and children as informants of victimization: Results from an

epidemiological cohort of children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(3), 379–387.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9463-5

Shannon, K. E., Sauder, C., Beauchaine, T. P., & Gatzke-Kopp, L. M. (2009). Disrupted

effective connectivity between the medial frontal cortex and the caudate in adolescent boys

with externalizing behavior disorders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36(11), 1141–1157.

http://doi.org/10.1177/0093854809342856

Shaw, P., Malek, M., Watson, B., Sharp, W., Evans, A., & Greenstein, D. (2012). Development

110

of cortical surface area and gyrification in attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

Biological Psychiatry, 72(3), 191–197. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.031

Sheppes, G., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation and psychopathology. Annual

Review of Clinical Psychology, 11(1), 379–405. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-

032814-112739

Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among school-age children: The

development and validation of a new criterion Q-sort scale. Developmental Psychology,

33(6), 906–916. http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.906

Shoal, G. D., Giancola, P. R., & Kirillova, G. P. (2003). Salivary cortisol, personality, and

aggressive behavior in adolescent boys: A 5-Year longitudinal study. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(9), 1101–1107.

http://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000070246.24125.6d

Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2003). Adolescents’ emotion regulation in daily life:

Links to depressive symptoms and problem behavior. Child Development, 74(6), 1869–

1880. http://doi.org/10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00643.x

Sloan, R. P., Shapiro, P. A., Bigger, J. T., Bagiella, E., Steinman, R. C., & Gorman, J. M. (1994).

Cardiac autonomic control and hostility in healthy subjects. The American Journal of

Cardiology, 74(3), 298–300. http://doi.org/10.1016/0002-9149(94)90382-4

Smithmyer, C. M., Hubbard, J. A., & Simons, R. F. (2000). Proactive and reactive aggression in

delinquent adolescents: Relations to aggression outcome expectancies. Journal of Clinical

Child Psychology, 29(1), 86–93. http://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424jccp2901_9

Sobanski, E., Banaschewski, T., Asherson, P., Buitelaar, J., Chen, W., Franke, B., … Faraone, S.

111

V. (2010). Emotional lability in children and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity

disorder (ADHD): Clinical correlates and familial prevalence. Journal of Child Psychology

and Psychiatry, 51(8), 915–923. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02217.x

Stanford, M. S., Houston, R. J., Villemarette-Pittman, N. R., & Greve, K. W. (2003).

Premeditated aggression: Clinical assessment and cognitive psychophysiology. Personality

and Individual Differences, 34(5), 773–781. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00070-3

Sterzer, P., & Stadler, C. (2009). Neuroimaging of aggressive and violent behaviour in children

and adolescents. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 3(October), 35.

http://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.08.035.2009

Stickle, T. R., Marini, V. A., & Thomas, J. N. (2012). Gender differences in psychopathic traits,

types, and correlates of aggression among adjudicated youth. Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology, 40(4), 513–525. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9588-1

Stifter, C. A., & Braungart, J. M. (1995). The regulation of negative reactivity in infancy:

Function and development. Developmental Psychology, 31(3), 448–455.

http://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.31.3.448

Stifter, C. A., & Fox, N. A. (1990). Infant reactivity: Physiological correlates of newborn and 5-

month temperament. Developmental Psychology, 26(4), 582–588.

http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.26.4.582

Sukhodolsky, D. G., Kassinove, H., & Gorman, B. S. (2004). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for

anger in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9(3),

247–269. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2003.08.005

Sun, S. S., Schubert, C. M., Chumlea, W. C., Roche, A. F., Kulin, H. E., Lee, P. A., … Ryan, A.

112

S. (2002). National estimates of the timing of sexual maturation and racial differences

among us children. Pediatrics, 110(5), 911–919. http://doi.org/10.1542/peds.110.5.911

Sutton, S., Braren, M., Zubin, J., & John, E. R. (1965). Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus

uncertainty. Science, 150(3700), 1187–1188. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.150.3700.1187

Suveg, C., & Zeman, J. (2004). Emotion regulation in children with anxiety disorders. Journal of

Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 33(4), 750–759.

http://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3304_10

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed.). Boston, MA:

Pearson.

Tanner, J. M. (1962). Growth at adolescence: With a general consideration of the effects of

heredity and environmental factors upon growth and maturation from birth to maturity.

Oxford, UK: Blackwell Scientific.

Ter Horst, G. J., Postema, F., Allen, G. V., Cechetto, D. F., Barron, H. V., Lesh, M. D., …

Schwartz, P. J. (1997). Forebrain parasympathetic control of heart activity: Retrograde

transneuronal viral labeling in rats. The American Journal of Physiology, 273(6 Pt 2),

H2926–30. http://doi.org/10.1002/cne.903300310

Thayer, J. F., Ahs, F., Fredrikson, M., Sollers, J. J., Wager, T. D., Åhs, F., … Wager, T. D.

(2012). A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: Implications for

heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral

Reviews, 36(2), 747–756. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009

Thayer, J. F., Hansen, A. L., Saus-Rose, E., & Johnsen, B. H. (2009). Heart rate variability,

prefrontal neural function, and cognitive performance: The neurovisceral integration

113

perspective on self-regulation, adaptation, and health. Annals of Behavioral Medicine,

37(2), 141–153. http://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-009-9101-z

Thompson, R. A. (2008). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of

the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2-3), 25–52.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01276.x

Todd, R. M., Lewis, M. D., Meusel, L.-A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2008). The time course of social-

emotional processing in early childhood: ERP responses to facial affect and personal

familiarity in a Go-Nogo task. Neuropsychologia, 46(2), 595–613.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.011

Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. The American

Psychologist, 44(2), 112–9. http://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.2.112

Tuvblad, C., Raine, A., Zheng, M., & Baker, L. A. (2009). Genetic and environmental stability

differs in reactive and proactive aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 35(6), 437–452.

http://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20319

Umetani, K., Singer, D. H., McCraty, R., & Atkinson, M. (1998). Twenty-four hour time domain

heart rate variability and heart rate: Relations to age and gender over nine decades. Journal

of the American College of Cardiology, 31(3), 593–601. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0735-

1097(97)00554-8

van Bokhoven, I., Van Goozen, S. H. M., van Engeland, H., Schaal, B., Arseneault, L., Séguin, J.

R., … Tremblay, R. E. (2005). Salivary cortisol and aggression in a population-based

longitudinal study of adolescent males. Journal of Neural Transmission, 112(8), 1083–

1096. http://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-004-0253-5

114

Venables, P. H. (1988). Psychophysiology and crime: Theory and data. Biological Contributions

to Crime Causation. Springer Science + Business Media, 3–13. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-

94-009-2768-1_1

Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Barker, E. D. (2006). Subtypes of aggressive behaviors: A

developmental perspective. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30(1), 12–19.

http://doi.org/10.1177/0165025406059968

Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2002). Reactively and proactively aggressive

children: antecedent and subsequent characteristics. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 43(4), 495–505. http://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00040

Vitaro, F., Gendreau, P. L., Tremblay, R. E., & Oligny, P. (1998). Reactive and proactive

aggression differentially predict later conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 39(3), 377–385. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0021963097002102

Volavka, J. (1990). Aggression, electroencephalography, and evoked potentials: A critical

review. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology, 3(4), 249–259.

Weinberg, A., & Klonsky, E. D. (2009). Measurement of emotion dysregulation in adolescents.

Psychological Assessment, 21(4), 616–621. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0016669

White, B. A., & Turner, K. A. (2014). Anger rumination and effortful control: Mediation effects

on reactive but not proactive aggression. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 186–

189. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.08.012

White, S. F., Meffert, H., & Blair, J. R. (2015). Functional neurobiology of aggression. The

Courts and Science Journal, 1(1), 2–11.

Wilson, L. C., & Scarpa, A. (2012). Criminal behavior: The need for an integrative approach that

115

Iincorporates biological influences. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 28(3), 366–

381. http://doi.org/10.1177/1043986212450232

Wong, S. W., Kimmerly, D. S., Massé, N., Menon, R. S., Cechetto, D. F., & Shoemaker, J. K.

(2007). Sex differences in forebrain and cardiovagal responses at the onset of isometric

handgrip exercise: a retrospective fMRI study. Journal of Applied Physiology (Bethesda,

Md. : 1985), 103(4), 1402–11. http://doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00171.2007

Xu, Y., Raine, A., Yu, L., & Krieg, A. (2013). Resting heart rate, vagal tone, and reactive and

proactive aggression in Chinese children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(3),

501–514. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9792-2

Yang, Y., & Raine, A. (2009). Prefrontal structural and functional brain imaging findings in

antisocial, violent, and psychopathic individuals: A meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research:

Neuroimaging, 174(2), 81–88. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.03.012

Yeragani, V. K., Pohl, R., Berger, R., Balon, R., & Srinivasan, K. (1994). Relationship between

age and heart rate variability in supine and standing postures: A study of spectral analysis of

heart rate. Pediatric Cardiology, 15(1), 14–20. http://doi.org/10.1007/BF00797000

Zeman, J., Cassano, M., Perry-Parrish, C., & Stegall, S. (2006). Emotion regulation in children

and adolescents. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2), 155–168.

http://doi.org/10.1097/00004703-200604000-00014

Zeman, J., & Garber, J. (1996). Display rules for anger, sadness, and pain: It depends on who is

watching. Child Development, 67(3), 957–973. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-

8624.1996.tb01776.x

Zeman, J., Shipman, K., & Suveg, C. (2002). Anger and sadness regulation: predictions to

116

internalizing and externalizing symptoms in children. Journal of Clinical Child and

Adolescent Psychology : The Official Journal for the Society of Clinical Child and

Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division 53, 31(3), 393–398.

http://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3103_11

Zhang, W., & Gao, Y. (2015). Interactive effects of social adversity and respiratory sinus

arrhythmia activity on reactive and proactive aggression. Psychophysiology, 52(10), 1343–

1350. http://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12473

Zoccolillo, M. (1993). Gender and the development of conduct disorder. Development and

Psychopathology, 5, 1–2. http://doi.org/doi:10.1017/s0954579400004260

117