Chapter 9: the Economics of Communication

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Chapter 9: the Economics of Communication Principles of Animal Communication, Second Edition Jack W. Bradbury and Sandra L. Vehrencamp Chapter 9: The Economics of Communication Literature Cited 1 Abrams, P. A. 2001. Adaptive dynamics: Neither F nor G. Evolutionary Ecology Research 3: 369–373. 2 Abrams, P. A. 2005. ‘Adaptive Dynamics’ vs. ‘adaptive dynamics’. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 18: 1162–1165. 3 Abrams, P. A. 2009. Analysis of Evolutionary Processes: The Adaptive Dynamics Approach and Its Applications. American Journal of Human Biology 21: 714–715. 4 Adret, P. and D. Margoliash. 2002. Metabolic and neural activity in the song system nucleus robustus archistriatalis: Effect of age and gender. Journal of Comparative Neurology 454: 409–423. 5 Adret, P. 2004. Vocal imitation in blindfolded zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) is facilitated in the presence of a non-singing conspecific female. Journal of Ethology 22: 29–35. 6 Agutter, P. S. 2008. The flux-summation theorem and the ‘evolution of dominance’. Journal of Theoretical Biology 254: 821–825. 7 Akino, T., J. J. Knapp, J. A. Thomas, and G. W. Elmes. 1999. Chemical mimicry and host specificity in the butterfly Maculinea rebeli, a social parasite of Myrmica ant colonies. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 266: 1419–1426. 8 Albert, A. Y. K. and S. P. Otto. 2005. Sexual selection can resolve sex-linked sexual antagonism. Science 310: 119–121. 9 Alcock, J. 2009. Animal Behavior: An Evolutionary Approach, 9th Edition. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates. 10 Alexander, R. D. 1962. Evolutionary change in cricket acoustical communication. Evolution 16: 443–467. 11 Allen, B. J. and J. S. Levinton. 2007. Costs of bearing a sexually selected ornamental weapon in a fiddler crab. Functional Ecology 21: 154–161. Chapter 9 Literature Cited 2 12 Allen, G. R. 2000. Call structure variability and field survival among bushcrickets exposed to phonotactic parasitoids. Ethology 106: 409–423. 13 Alonzo, S. H. and B. Sinervo. 2001. Mate choice games, context-dependent good genes, and genetic cycles in the side-blotched lizard, Uta stansburiana. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 49: 176–186. 14 Alvarado, A. S. 2000. Regeneration in the metazoans: why does it happen? Bioessays 22: 578–590. 15 Amat, J. A., E. Aguilera, and G. H. Visser. 2007. Energetic and developmental costs of mounting an immune response in greenfinches (Carduelis chloris). Ecological Research 22: 282–287. 16 Amorim, M. C. P., M. L. McCracken, and M. L. Fine. 2002. Metabolic costs of sound production in the oyster toadfish, Opsanus tau. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie 80: 830–838. 17 Amrein, I. and H. P. Lipp. 2009. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis of mammals: evolution and life history. Biology Letters 5: 141–144. 18 Andersson, M. 1994. Sexual Selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. 19 Andrews, C. B. and T. R. Gregory. 2009. Genome size is inversely correlated with relative brain size in parrots and cockatoos. Genome 52: 261–267. 20 Andrews, P. W., S. W. Gangestad, and D. Matthews. 2002. Adaptationism – how to carry out an exaptationist program. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25: 489–553. 21 Apaloo, J., J. S. Brown, and T. L. Vincent. 2009. Evolutionary game theory: ESS, convergence stability, and NIS. Evolutionary Ecology Research 11: 489–515. 22 Apaloo, J. and S. Butler. 2009. Evolutionary stabilities in multidimensional-traits and several-species models. Evolutionary Ecology Research 11: 637–650. 23 Apollonio, M. and I. Di Vittorio. 2004. Feeding and reproductive behaviour in fallow bucks (Dama dama). Naturwissenschaften 91: 579–584. 24 Arnold, E. N. 1988. Caudal autotomy as a defence. In Biology of the Reptilia (C. Gans and R. Huey, eds.), pp. 235–273. New York, NY: Alan R. Liss. 25 Arnold, S. J., R. Burger, P. A. Hohenlohe, B. C. Ajie, and A. G. Jones. 2008. Understanding the evolution and stability of the G-matrix. Evolution 62: 2451– 2461. Chapter 9 Literature Cited 3 26 Atallah, H. E., M. J. Frank, and R. C. O’Reilly. 2004. Hippocampus, cortex, and basal ganglia: Insights from computational models of complementary learning systems. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 82: 253–267. 27 Auger-Methe, M. and H. Whitehead. 2007. The use of natural markings in studies of long-finned pilot whales (Globicephala melas). Marine Mammal Science 23: 77– 93. 28 Axelrod, R., R. A. Hammond, and A. Grafen. 2004. Altruism via kin-selection strategies that rely on arbitrary tags with which they coevolve. Evolution 58: 1833– 1838. 29 Bachman, G. C. and M. A. Chappell. 1998. The energetic cost of begging behaviour in nestling house wrens. Animal Behaviour 55: 1607–1618. 30 Bacigalupe, L. D. and F. Bozinovic. 2002. Design, limitations and sustained metabolic rate: lessons from small mammals. Journal of Experimental Biology 205: 2963–2970. 31 Badyaev, A. V. and G. E. Hill. 2000. Evolution of sexual dichromatism: contribution of carotenoid- versus melanin-based coloration. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 69: 153–172. 32 Badyaev, A. V. and A. Qvarnstrom. 2002. Putting sexual traits into the context of an organism: A life-history perspective in studies of sexual selection. Auk 119: 301– 310. 33 Badyaev, A. V. 2007. Evolvability and robustness in color displays: Bridging the gap between theory and data. Evolutionary Biology 34: 61–71. 34 Badyaev, A. V. and C. M. Vleck. 2007. Context-dependent development of sexual ornamentation: implications for a trade-off between current and future breeding efforts. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20: 1277–1287. 35 Bailey, C. H. and E. R. Kandel. 1993. Structural changes accompanying memory storage. Annual Review of Physiology 55: 397–426. 36 Bailey, W. J., P. C. Withers, M. Endersby, and K. Gaull. 1993. The energetic costs of calling in the bush cricket Requena verticalis (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae, Listroscelidinae). Journal of Experimental Biology 178: 21–37. 37 Bailey, W. J. 1995. Costs of calling in tettigoniid Orthoptera: a case study of Requena verticalis (Tettigoniidae: Listroscelidinae). Journal of Orthoptera Research 4: 65–73. Chapter 9 Literature Cited 4 38 Ball, G. F. and S. A. MacDougall-Shackleton. 2001. Sex differences in songbirds 25 years later: what have we learned and where do we go? Microscopy Research Techniques 54: 327–334. 39 Ballentine, B. 2006. Morphological adaptation influences the evolution of a mating signal. Evolution 60: 1936–1944. 40 Bao, S. W., W. T. Chan, and M. M. Merzenich. 2001. Cortical remodelling induced by activity of ventral tegmental dopamine neurons. Nature 412: 79–83. 41 Barbero, F., S. Bonelli, J. A. Thomas, E. Balletto, and K. Schonrogge. 2009. Acoustical mimicry in a predatory social parasite of ants. Journal of Experimental Biology 212: 4084–4090. 42 Barnea, A. and F. Nottebohm. 1994. Seasonal recruitment of hippocampal neurons in adult free-ranging black-capped chickadees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 91: 11217–11221. 43 Barnea, A. 2009. Interactions between environmental changes and brain plasticity in birds. General and Comparative Endocrinology 163: 128–134. 44 Barnett, C. A. and J. V. Briskie. 2007. Energetic state and the performance of dawn chorus in silvereyes (Zosterops lateralis). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 61: 579–587. 45 Barrett, L., S. P. Henzi, T. Weingrill, J. E. Lycett, and R. A. Hill. 1999. Market forces predict grooming reciprocity in female baboons. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 266: 665–670. 46 Barta, Z., A. I. Houston, J. M. McNamara, R. K. Welham, A. Hedenstrom, T. P. Weber, and O. Fero. 2006. Annual routines of non-migratory birds: optimal moult strategies. Oikos 112: 580–593. 47 Bartholomew, G. A. and T. M. Casey. 1978. Oxygen consumption of moths during rest, pre-flight warm-up, and flight in relation to body size and wing morphology. Journal of Experimental Biology 76: 11–25. 48 Bartkowska, K., R. L. Djavadian, J. R. E. Taylor, and K. Turlejski. 2008. Generation recruitment and death of brain cells throughout the life cycle of Sorex shrews (Lipotyphla). European Journal of Neuroscience 27: 1710–1721. 49 Barton, N. H. and M. Turelli. 1987. Adaptive landscapes, genetic distance, and the evolution of quantitative characters. Genetical Research 49: 157–173. 50 Barton, N. H. and M. Turelli. 1991. Natural and sexual selection on many loci. Genetics 127: 229–255. Chapter 9 Literature Cited 5 51 Barton, N. H. and J. Polechova. 2005. The limitations of adaptive dynamics as a model of evolution. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 18: 1186–1190. 52 Basibuyuk, H. H. and D. L. J. Quicke. 1999. Grooming behaviours in the Hymenoptera (Insecta): potential phylogenetic significance. Zoological Journal of the Linnean Society 125: 349–382. 53 Bass, A. H. and M. A. Marchaterre. 1989. Sound generating (sonic) motor system in a teleost fish (Porichthys notatus)-sexual polymorphism in the ultrastructure of myofibrils. Journal of Comparative Neurology 286: 141–153. 54 Bass, A. H., E. H. Gilland, and R. Baker. 2008. Evolutionary origins for social vocalization in a vertebrate hindbrain-spinal compartment. Science 321: 417–421. 55 Batang, Z. B. and H. Suzuki. 2000. Gill structure and gill-cleaning mechanisms of the redclaw crayfish Cherax quadricarinatus (Decapoda, Astacidea, Parastacidae). Journal of Crustacean Biology 20: 699–714. 56 Bateman, P. W. and P. A. Fleming. 2005. Direct and indirect costs of limb autotomy in field crickets, Gryllus bimaculatus. Animal Behaviour 69: 151–159. 57 Bateman, P. W. and P. A. Fleming. 2006. Sex, intimidation and severed limbs: the effect of simulated predator attack and limb autotomy on calling and emergence behaviour in the field cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 59: 674–681. 58 Bateman, P. W. and P. A. Fleming. 2006. Increased susceptibility to predation for autotomized house crickets (Acheta domestica). Ethology 112: 670–677. 59 Bateman, P. W. and P. A. Fleming. 2009.
Recommended publications
  • Leonard, A.S. and Hedrick, A.V. 2009
    Animal Behaviour 77 (2009) 151–159 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Animal Behaviour journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/yanbe Single versus multiple cues in mate discrimination by males and females Anne S. Leonard*, Ann V. Hedrick 1 Department of Neurobiology, Physiology and Behavior, University of California, Davis article info Courtship communication can function in both species recognition and mate choice. Little is known Article history: about how animals prioritize signals or cues for species identification versus intraspecific mate choice Received 28 June 2008 when several information sources are available, such as when communication spans several sensory Initial acceptance 4 August 2008 modalities or spatiotemporal scales. Cricket courtship, for example, involves transmission of acoustic Final acceptance 4 September 2008 signals as well as chemosensory contact. We explored how chemical cues function in sex and species Published online 14 November 2008 recognition for both male and female crickets, and then evaluated their use in a mating context where MS. number: A08-00417 additional stimuli were available. First, we observed the response of female and male Gryllus integer to the chemical cues of conspecifics and sympatric G. lineaticeps. Males’ strongest response was to Keywords: conspecific female chemical cues. Although females responded most strongly to male chemical cues, they courtship did not show species discrimination. Next, we compared the responses of male and female G. integer to Gryllus integer conspecifics and heterospecifics in mating trials. Females directed more aggressive behaviour and less Gryllus lineaticeps mating preferences chemosensory behaviour towards heterospecific males, but males courted females of both species with multiple cues equal intensities.
    [Show full text]
  • Effect of Diet Quanitity and Quality on Female Sampling Behaviour and Mating Preferences in a Field Cricket
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Dissertations and Theses in Biological Sciences Biological Sciences, School of Winter 12-2011 EFFECT OF DIET QUANITITY AND QUALITY ON FEMALE SAMPLING BEHAVIOUR AND MATING PREFERENCES IN A FIELD CRICKET Heidi L. Bulfer University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscidiss Part of the Life Sciences Commons Bulfer, Heidi L., "EFFECT OF DIET QUANITITY AND QUALITY ON FEMALE SAMPLING BEHAVIOUR AND MATING PREFERENCES IN A FIELD CRICKET" (2011). Dissertations and Theses in Biological Sciences. 35. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscidiss/35 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Biological Sciences, School of at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations and Theses in Biological Sciences by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. EFFECT OF DIET QUANITITY AND QUALITY ON FEMALE SAMPLING BEHAVIOUR AND MATING PREFERENCES IN A FIELD CRICKET by Heidi L. Bulfer A THESIS Presented to the Faculty of The Graduate College at the University of Nebraska In Partial Fulfillment of Requirements For the Degree of Master of Science Major: Biological Sciences Under the Supervision of Professor William E. Wagner, Jr. Lincoln, Nebraska December, 2011 EFFECT OF DIET QUANITITY AND QUALITY ON FEMALE SAMPLING BEHAVIOUR AND MATING PREFERENCES IN A FIELD CRICKET Heidi L. Bulfer, M.S. University of Nebraska, 2011 Advisor: William E. Wagner, Jr. Understanding the adaptive significance of variation in female mating behaviour is important because variation may often be favored by selection instead of a change in mean mating behaviour, particularly in variable environments.
    [Show full text]
  • Based on Comparative Morphological Data AF Emel'yanov Transactions of T
    The phylogeny of the Cicadina (Homoptera, Cicadina) based on comparative morphological data A.F. Emel’yanov Transactions of the All-Union Entomological Society Morphological principles of insect phylogeny The phylogenetic relationships of the principal groups of cicadine* insects have been considered on more than one occasion, commencing with Osborn (1895). Some phylogenetic schemes have been based only on data relating to contemporary cicadines, i.e. predominantly on comparative morphological data (Kirkaldy, 1910; Pruthi, 1925; Spooner, 1939; Kramer, 1950; Evans, 1963; Qadri, 1967; Hamilton, 1981; Savinov, 1984a), while others have been constructed with consideration given to paleontological material (Handlirsch, 1908; Tillyard, 1919; Shcherbakov, 1984). As the most primitive group of the cicadines have been considered either the Fulgoroidea (Kirkaldy, 1910; Evans, 1963), mainly because they possess a small clypeus, or the cicadas (Osborn, 1895; Savinov, 1984), mainly because they do not jump. In some schemes even the monophyletism of the cicadines has been denied (Handlirsch, 1908; Pruthi, 1925; Spooner, 1939; Hamilton, 1981), or more precisely in these schemes the Sternorrhyncha were entirely or partially depicted between the Fulgoroidea and the other cicadines. In such schemes in which the Fulgoroidea were accepted as an independent group, among the remaining cicadines the cicadas were depicted as branching out first (Kirkaldy, 1910; Hamilton, 1981; Savinov, 1984a), while the Cercopoidea and Cicadelloidea separated out last, and in the most widely acknowledged systematic scheme of Evans (1946b**) the last two superfamilies, as the Cicadellomorpha, were contrasted to the Cicadomorpha and the Fulgoromorpha. At the present time, however, the view affirming the equivalence of the four contemporary superfamilies and the absence of a closer relationship between the Cercopoidea and Cicadelloidea (Evans, 1963; Emel’yanov, 1977) is gaining ground.
    [Show full text]
  • Studies of Human Physique and Sexual Attractiveness: Sexual Preferences of Men and Women in China
    AMERICAN JOURNAL OF HUMAN BIOLOGY 19:88–95 (2007) Original Research Article Studies of Human Physique and Sexual Attractiveness: Sexual Preferences of Men and Women in China 1 2 3 1 BARNABY J. DIXSON, ALAN F. DIXSON, * BAOGUO LI, AND M.J. ANDERSON 1Department of Conservation and Research for Endangered Species, Zoological Society of San Diego, San Diego, California 2School of Biological Sciences, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand 3College of Life Sciences, and Key Laboratory Resource Biology and Biotechnology in Western China, Ministry of Education, Northwest University, Xi’an, China ABSTRACT Men and women at Northwest University (n ¼ 631), Xi’an, China, were asked to rate the attractiveness of male or female figures manipulated to vary somatotype, waist-to- hip ratio (WHR), secondary sexual traits, and other features. In study 1, women rated the aver- age masculine somatotype as most attractive, followed by the mesomorphic (muscular), ecto- morphic (slim), and endomorphic (heavily built) somatotypes, in descending order of preference. In study 2, the amount and distribution of masculine trunk (chest and abdominal) hair were altered progressively in a series of front-posed figures. Women rated figures with no or little trunk hair as most attractive. Study 3 assessed the attractiveness of front-posed male figures which varied only in length of their nonerect penis. Numerical ratings for this trait were low, but moderate lengthening of the penis (22% or 33% above average) resulted in a significant increase in scores for attractiveness. In study 4, Chinese men rated the attractiveness of back- posed female images varying in waist-to-hip ratio (WHR from 0.5–1.0).
    [Show full text]
  • Dominant Frequency Characteristics of Calling Songs in Cicada
    Journal of Entomology and Zoology Studies 2014; 2 (6): 330-332 ISSN 2320-7078 Dominant frequency characteristics of calling songs in JEZS 2014; 2 (6): 330-332 © 2014 JEZS cicada Chloropsalta smaragdula Haupt, 1920 (Hem: www.entomoljournal.com Received: 21-10-2014 Cicadidae) and its relationship with body length, width, Accepted: 21-11-2014 and weight. Akbar Mohammadi-Mobarakeh Department of Entomology, Khorasgan (Isfahan) Branch, Islamic Azad University, Isfahan, Akbar Mohammadi-Mobarakeh, Bijan Hatami, Alireza Jalali Zand, Iran, 00989132375720. Rassoul Amir Fattahi, Hossein Zamanian Bijan Hatami Abstract Department of Entomology, To study the calling songs in cicada Chloropsalta smaragdula and its relationship with body length, Khorasgan (Isfahan) Branch, width, and weight, a research was conducted in the Iranian city of Mobarakeh, Isfahan Province, in 2011- Islamic Azad University, Isfahan, 2012. Different sound samples were taken under field and laboratory conditions. Throughout the Iran. sampling period, the calling songs of nine male cicadas were recorded and studied. The sound of each individual cicada was separately analyzed by MATLAB Software, and the size of its main parameter Alireza Jalali Zand Department of Entomology, (i.e., dominant frequency) was determined and recorded. The results showed that this cicada species have Khorasgan (Isfahan) Branch, a mean dominant frequency of 9.121 kHz. Moreover, the correlation coefficients indicated that there is a Islamic Azad University, Isfahan, positive relationship between the dominant frequency for the audio signal of the calling songs with body Iran. width and weight, and is significant at p<0.0001 probability level, thus, indicating that dominant frequency increases as body width and weight increase.
    [Show full text]
  • Sound Radiation by the Bladder Cicada Cystosoma Saundersii
    The Journal of Experimental Biology 201, 701–715 (1998) 701 Printed in Great Britain © The Company of Biologists Limited 1998 JEB1166 SOUND RADIATION BY THE BLADDER CICADA CYSTOSOMA SAUNDERSII H. C. BENNET-CLARK1,* AND D. YOUNG2 1Department of Zoology, Oxford University, South Parks Road, Oxford, OX1 3PS, UK and 2Department of Zoology, University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3052, Australia *e-mail: [email protected] Accepted 26 November 1997: published on WWW 5 February 1998 Summary Male Cystosoma saundersii have a distended thin-walled air sac volume was the major compliant element in the abdomen which is driven by the paired tymbals during resonant system. Increasing the mass of tergite 4 and sound production. The insect extends the abdomen from a sternites 4–6 also reduced the resonant frequency of the rest length of 32–34 mm to a length of 39–42 mm while abdomen. By extrapolation, it was shown that the effective singing. This is accomplished through specialised mass of tergites 3–5 was between 13 and 30 mg and that the apodemes at the anterior ends of abdominal segments 4–7, resonant frequency was proportional to 1/√(total mass), which cause each of these intersegmental membranes to suggesting that the masses of the tergal sound-radiating unfold by approximately 2 mm. areas were major elements in the resonant system. The calling song frequency is approximately 850 Hz. The The tymbal ribs buckle in sequence from posterior (rib song pulses have a bimodal envelope and a duration of 1) to anterior, producing a series of sound pulses.
    [Show full text]
  • A New Neotibicen Cicada Subspecies (Hemiptera: Cicadidae)
    Zootaxa 4272 (4): 529–550 ISSN 1175-5326 (print edition) http://www.mapress.com/j/zt/ Article ZOOTAXA Copyright © 2017 Magnolia Press ISSN 1175-5334 (online edition) https://doi.org/10.11646/zootaxa.4272.4.3 http://zoobank.org/urn:lsid:zoobank.org:pub:C6234E29-8808-44DF-AD15-07E82B398D66 A new Neotibicen cicada subspecies (Hemiptera: Cicadidae) from the southeast- ern USA forms hybrid zones with a widespread relative despite a divergent male calling song DAVID C. MARSHALL1 & KATHY B. R. HILL Dept. of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Connecticut, 75 N. Eagleville Rd., Storrs, CT 06269 USA 1Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected] Abstract A morphologically cryptic subspecies of Neotibicen similaris (Smith and Grossbeck) is described from forests of the Apalachicola region of the southeastern United States. Although the new form exhibits a highly distinctive male calling song, it hybridizes extensively where it meets populations of the nominate subspecies in parapatry, by which it is nearly surrounded. This is the first reported example of hybridization between North American nonperiodical cicadas. Acoustic and morphological characters are added to the original description of the nominate subspecies, and illustrations of com- plex hybrid song phenotypes are presented. The biogeography of N. similaris is discussed in light of historical changes in forest composition on the southeastern Coastal Plain. Key words: Acoustic behavior, sexual signals, hybridization, hybrid zone, parapatric distribution, speciation Introduction The cryptotympanine cicadas of North America have received much recent attention with the publication of comprehensive molecular and cladistic phylogenies and the reassignment of all former North American Tibicen Latreille species into new genera (Hill et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Biology 123 – Human Biology
    Biology 123.001 – Human Biology Dr. Robert J. Wiggers, Dept. Biology Text (Required): Human Biology, Concepts & Current Issues, 6th ed. By M. Johnson Supplemental Materials (Required): Access to Mastering Biology website Supplemental Materials (Suggested): Lecture notes available on Desire 2 Learn Class Time & Place: TR 11 – 12:15, S233 Instructor Office: 204S Office phone / e-mail: 468-2147 / [email protected] Office Hours: MWF 10-11, TR 9:30-11, W 1-3, R 1-3 Course Description: Four semester hours, three hours lecture, two hours lab per week. Biological principles for non-science majors. Study of the evolution of man, organ systems, and the human organism. May not be used to meet graduation requirements of students majoring in the College of Sciences and Mathematics or for certification of high school teachers in biology. Required lab fee. Pre-requisites: None Co-requisite: BIO 123L Program Learning Outcomes: There are no specific program learning outcomes for this major addressed in this course. It is a general education core curriculum course and / or a service course. General Education Core Curriculum Objectives / Outcomes EEO #3: To identify and recognize the differences among competing models of scientific theories EEO #4: To demonstrate knowledge of the major issues and problems facing modern science, including issues that touch upon ethics, values, and public policies EEO #5: To demonstrate knowledge of the interdependence of science and technology and their influence on, and contribution, to modern culture Student Learning Outcomes: An understanding of the biological molecules found in human cells, the ultra-structure of human cells, as well the basic processes cells use to survive and reproduce An understanding of the basic human organ systems, including their anatomy and physiology, their control, and their function in the whole organism context.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Biology (HUM BIOL) 1
    Human Biology (HUM BIOL) 1 Human Biology (HUM BIOL) Courses HUM BIOL 102. Introduction to Human Biology. 3 Credits. Basic concepts, principles, and processes in human biology; the origin of life, evolution, cells, biochemical processes, physiological systems, genetics and metabolism. Fall and Spring. HUM BIOL 116. First Aid and Emergency Care Procedures. 3 Credits. Student will learn all aspects of first aid training such as victim assessment and treating all types of illnesses and injuries; all skills for Professional Rescuer CPR; dealing with infectious diseases and their transmission. Fall and Spring. HUM BIOL 198. First Year Seminar. 3 Credits. topics vary Reserved for New Incoming Freshman. HUM BIOL 202. Ethnic Minorities in Science. 3 Credits. The history and culture of science in the US will be examined, in order to understand what has led to the current under-representation of ethnic minorities in science. The often overlooked contributions of scientists who are members of ethnic minorities will be recognized. Spring. HUM BIOL 205. Biotechnology and Human Values. 3 Credits. Examination of technological developments in biology and medicine, including genetic, behavioral, and organism modification and the moral and ethical concerns raised by such technologies. P: Hum Biol 102 or Biology 201/202. Fall and Spring. HUM BIOL 206. Fertility, Reproduction, and Family Planning. 3 Credits. Factors that influence reproduction and fertility, i.e., physiological, psychological, social, cultural, and ethical; the methods available for limiting or increasing reproduction; the nature of family planning programs. P: Hum Biol 102 or Biology 201/202. Fall and Spring. HUM BIOL 208. Scientific Conditioning of the Athlete.
    [Show full text]
  • Biology — M.S., Ph.D. 1
    Biology — M.S., Ph.D. 1 BIOL 407. Herpetology. 3 Units. BIOLOGY — M.S., PH.D. Covers a broad range of topics in herpetology, including systematics, diversity, morphology, physiology, behavior, ecology, conservation, and The Biology Program leading to the Master of Science and Doctor of research methodology. Focuses field experience on Southern California Philosophy degrees is offered by the Department of Earth and Biological herpetology. Two hours lecture, three-hour laboratory each week. Sciences. M.S. and Ph.D. curricula provide a broad and unified approach BIOL 409. Mammalogy. 4 Units. to the life sciences, as well as specialization—as evidenced by the Studies the mammals of the world, with emphasis on North America. conduct of significant, original research; and in the selection of courses Includes classroom and field study of systematics, distribution, behavior, related to the area of research interest. Study in various areas, from and ecology. Per week: class three hours, one three-hour laboratory. molecular biology to natural history, is available to the student seeking preparation for teaching or for research in modern biology. Some areas BIOL 414. Biology of Marine Invertebrates. 4 Units. of specialization are animal behavior, animal physiology, molecular Behavior, physiology, ecology, morphology, and systematics of marine systematics, ecological physiology, behavioral ecology, conservation invertebrates, with emphasis on morphology and systematics. Three biology, marine biology, and paleontology. class hours per week, one-day field trip alternate weeks, or the equivalent. BIOL 415. Ecology. 3 Units. Objectives Covers the interaction between living organisms and their environment, The Biology Program strives to: animal and plant diversity, and the biotic and abiotic factors that contribute to organisms interacting with their environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Worldwide Spread of the Ruby Ant, Myrmica Rubra (Hymenoptera: Formicidae)
    Myrmecological News 14 87-96 Vienna, January 2011 Worldwide spread of the ruby ant, Myrmica rubra (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) James K. WETTERER & Alexander G. RADCHENKO Abstract The ruby ant, Myrmica rubra (LINNAEUS, 1758) (formerly Myrmica laevinodis NYLANDER, 1846), an aggressive Eur- asian species with a powerful sting, is now spreading through temperate North America. To document the worldwide distribution of M. rubra and evaluate its potential for further spread, we compiled published and unpublished specimen records from > 2000 sites. We report the earliest known M. rubra records for 71 geographic areas (countries, major is- lands, US states, Canadian provinces, and Russian federal districts), including three areas with no previously published records: Prince Edward Island, Washington State, and the Far Eastern Federal District of Russia. All earlier published records of M. rubra from East Asia, including the Far East of Russia, Japan, and China, appear to be misidentifications of Myrmica kotokui FOREL, 1911. Myrmica rubra is native to an enormous expanse extending from Ireland and Portugal in westernmost Europe across 8000 km to central Asia and eastern Siberia, and from 39 to 70° N in latitude. Exotic populations of M. rubra were first recorded in eastern North America more than 100 years ago. Myrmica rubra is now documented from five southeastern Canadian provinces (New Brunswick, Nova Scotia, Ontario, Prince Edward Island, and Quebec), six northeastern US states (Maine, Massachusetts, New Hampshire, New York, Rhode Island, and Vermont), and one northwestern state (Wash- ington) ranging from 41.5 to 47.6° N. Given the vast range of M. rubra in Eurasia, perhaps the most striking aspect about this species in North America is how little it has spread over the past century.
    [Show full text]
  • Chamber Music: an Unusual Helmholtz Resonator for Song Amplification in a Neotropical Bush-Cricket (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae) Thorin Jonsson1,*, Benedict D
    © 2017. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd | Journal of Experimental Biology (2017) 220, 2900-2907 doi:10.1242/jeb.160234 RESEARCH ARTICLE Chamber music: an unusual Helmholtz resonator for song amplification in a Neotropical bush-cricket (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae) Thorin Jonsson1,*, Benedict D. Chivers1, Kate Robson Brown2, Fabio A. Sarria-S1, Matthew Walker1 and Fernando Montealegre-Z1,* ABSTRACT often a morphological challenge owing to the power and size of their Animals use sound for communication, with high-amplitude signals sound production mechanisms (Bennet-Clark, 1998; Prestwich, being selected for attracting mates or deterring rivals. High 1994). Many animals therefore produce sounds by coupling the amplitudes are attained by employing primary resonators in sound- initial sound-producing structures to mechanical resonators that producing structures to amplify the signal (e.g. avian syrinx). Some increase the amplitude of the generated sound at and around their species actively exploit acoustic properties of natural structures to resonant frequencies (Fletcher, 2007). This also serves to increase enhance signal transmission by using these as secondary resonators the sound radiating area, which increases impedance matching (e.g. tree-hole frogs). Male bush-crickets produce sound by tegminal between the structure and the surrounding medium (Bennet-Clark, stridulation and often use specialised wing areas as primary 2001). Common examples of these kinds of primary resonators are resonators. Interestingly, Acanthacara acuta, a Neotropical bush- the avian syrinx (Fletcher and Tarnopolsky, 1999) or the cicada cricket, exhibits an unusual pronotal inflation, forming a chamber tymbal (Bennet-Clark, 1999). In addition to primary resonators, covering the wings. It has been suggested that such pronotal some animals have developed morphological or behavioural chambers enhance amplitude and tuning of the signal by adaptations that act as secondary resonators, further amplifying constituting a (secondary) Helmholtz resonator.
    [Show full text]