Phonology Development Chart

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Phonology Development Chart Phonology Development 0 - 3 Months 3 - 6 Months 6 – 9 Months 9 – 12 Months • Birth cry – undifferentiated • Babbling begins • Uses m, n, t, d, b, p, y in babbling multiple • Vocalizes during play • Reflexive sound making produces glottal Double syllables – VCV, aga syllables • Vocalizes to mirror Puts lips together – says “m” catch and vowels (ah, eh, uh) • Babbles tunefully – singing tones • Jabbers loudly – wide variety of sounds and Nasal tone is heard • Some variety in non-crying sounds • Uses wide variety of sound combinations intonations • Vocalizes pleasure and displeasure • Differentiated cry (true vocal communication including non-English sounds • Uses most sounds (C&V) in vocal play – begins) • Stops vocalizing when adult enters • Inflected vocal play – intonation patterns beginning of phonetic drift • Coos and gurgles • Self-initiated vocal play heard • May acquire first true word –0-18 months • Produces single syllables • Coos, chuckles, gurgles and laughs • Imitates intonation and speech sounds in • Variegated babbling begins – combines his/her own repertoire • Begins blowing bubbles • Babbles to self, others, and objects different syllables in vocal play • Babbling show pitch and inflection change • Reduplicative babbling begins – bababa • Vocally expresses eagerness 1 – 1½ Years 1 ½ - 2 Years 2- 2 ½ Years 2½- 3 Years • Uses sentence-like intonations (jargon) • Words increasing in frequency – jargon • Approximately 70% intelligible • Still some substitutions and distortion of • Some echolalia almost gone by 2 years • May omit final consonant, reduce consonant consonants • Uses most vowels and consonants and some • Asks questions by raising intonation at end blends; substitute one consonant for another • Continuing to improve intelligibility – now initial consonants of phrase. approximately 80% intelligible • Basically unintelligible with exception of a • Improvement in intelligibility – now • Consonants mastered: p, m, n, w, h few words approximately 65% intelligible by 2 years • Omits final consonants and some initial • Appearance of words produced with CVC consonants structure (hot) • Words produced with VC structure (bo/boat) emerge) • Accurately imitates some words 3- 3 ½ Years 3 ½ - 4 Years 4- 4 ½ Years 4½- 5 Years • Uses final consonants most of time • Becoming very intelligible in connected • Should be few omissions and substitutions of • Most consonant sounds used consistently • Phonological processes disappearing by age speech consonants and accurately, though may not be mastered 3: consonant assimilation, diminutization, • Continued refinement of articulatory skills • Very intelligible in connected speech in all contexts doubling , final consonant deletion, taking place • More errors present in difficult blends prevocalic voicing, reduplication, unstressed • Consonants mastered: b, d, k, g, f, y syllable deletion, velar fronting • Phonological processes continuing after age 3: cluster reduction, deplatalization, epenthesis, final devoicing, gliding, stopping, vocalization 5- 6 Years 6 – 7 Years • • From: Speech and Language Development Consonants mastered: t, ing, r, l Consonants mastered: voiceless th, sh, ch, j nd (by 8 years, voiced th, v, s, zh are Chart (2 Ed.) by Addy Gard, Leslea Gilman, mastered) and Jim Gorman, Pro-Ed. From: Speech and Language Development Chart (2nd Ed.) by Addy Gard, Leslea Gilman, and Jim Gorman, Pro-Ed. Semantics Development Chart 0 - 3 Months 3 - 6 Months 6 – 9 Months 9 – 12 Months • Startle response to loud sound • Anticipates feeding upon sight of bottle • Attempts to imitate gross gestures • Begins to relate symbol and object – first • Visually tracks while on back • Watches or closely attends to movement • Comprehends parental gestures true word • Moves eyes toward source of sound patterns • Looks at some common objects when names • Will give block, toy or object on request • Attends to voice – turns head toward voice • Smiles on sight of face are spoken • Understands and follows simple commands regarding body action • Fixes gaze on spoon or bottle • Recognizes spoon or bottle • Comprehends “no” – inhibits on command • Responds with searching movements to • Orients to light • Laughs when playing with objects • Interest in pictures maintained for full minute while they are name simple questions • Briefly holds and inspects two objects (1-4 • Looks in correct place for toys out of sight months) • Searches for partially hidden object (4-8 months) • Turns heads immediately to own name • Mouths some objects • Bangs objects together • Understands the meaning of “hot” • Performs many actions on objects – shakes, • In presence of more than one object, shows tears, slides, mouths, rubs awareness of more than one • Sight of object or person produces gross • Indicates displeasure when object is gesture (emerging nomination) removed • Imitates ringing of bell Attending Phase (0 – 9 Months) • Can name or look for subject out of sight – Prerequisite of Semantic Development first true word • Gestures and/or vocalizes to indicate wants and needs (emerging state, recurrence, nomination) • Recognizes inverted object – emerging awareness of top/bottom, front/back 1 – 1½ Years 1 ½ - 2 Years 2- 2 ½ Years 2½- 3 Years • Follows simple one-step commands • Comprehends approximately 300 words • Comprehends approximately 500 words • Comprehends approximately 900 words • Points to recognized objects (emerging • Listens as pictures are name • Listens to 5 to 10 minute story • Points to pictures of 10 objects described by nomination) • Listens to simple stories – especially likes • Carries out series of two related commands their use • Point to wanted objects (emerging state) ones heard before • Identifies action in pictures • Listens to 20 minute story • Begins to claim certain objects (emerging • Points to five body parts on self or doll • Has concept of “one” and “all” • Knows own sex and difference possession) • Responds approximately to yes/no • Knows in/on/under • Points to one to three body parts on command questions (head shake) • Knows big/little • Identifies two or more objects or pictures • Object permanence fully acquired • Matches colors from a group • Discriminates food from other objects • Completes three piece form board, matches • Perceives other’s emotions (unwraps candy before eating) shapes • Imitates only events that are present to the • Is cautious of common dangers, such as senses, not those from past experiences stairways, animals • Has complicated, sequenced routines for daily activities (bedtime, meals); objects to change (beginning of time/sequence awareness From: Speech and Language Development Chart (2nd Ed.) by Addy Gard, Leslea Gilman, and Jim Gorman (Pro-Ed). 1 – 1½ Years 1 ½ - 2 Years 2- 2 ½ Years 2½- 3 Years • Uses 3 to 20 words • Uses approximately 50 recognizable words • Uses 200 intelligible words • Uses 500 intelligible words • Vocalizes with gestures • Uses names of most familiar objects • Names six objects by use • Answers six to seven agent/action questions • Says “all gone” (emerging negation) • Produces animal sound or uses its name • Repeats two numbers correctly (what runs?) • Answers question “what’s this?” • Verbalizes toilet needs (closer to 2 years) • Answers “where” questions • Answers simple “who, why, where, how many “ questions (3 years) • Asks for “more” (emerging recurrence) may verbalize need before, during, or after • Answers “what….doing” questions act • Answers one of three questions (what do • Answers “what do you hear with?” • Identifies and names five or more pictures you do when you’re hungry, sleepy or by 2 years cold?) • Says own name on request – refers to self • Can ask simple questions (what’s that?) with full name • Yes/No questions emerging (is he sleeping?) • Verbalizes “no” • Repeats sentence of sex to seven syllables • Verbalizes immediate experiences accurately • Combines two words into phrases, ma use three to four word responses (2 years) • Begins to use some verbs and adjectives 3- 3 ½ Years 3 ½ - 4 Years 4- 4 ½ Years 4½- 5 Years • Comprehends 1,200 words • Comprehends 1,500 – 2,000 words • Understands concept of the number three • Comprehends 2500 to 2800 words • Knows “in front of” and “behind” when • Knows front and back of clothes (give me just three) • Answers two complex comprehension object with logical front and back if used • Responds to commands involving three • Knows between, above, below, top, bottom questions • Identifies hard/soft, rough/smooth actions • Names one color (54 months) • Executes three commissions in sequence • Identifies circle and square • Recognizes one color • Can recognize two to three primary colors • Points to red, yellow, green, and blue on • Responds to commands involving two (54 months) request (60 months) actions • Knows heavy/light, loud/soft, like/unalike, • Responds to commands involving two discriminates /long/short objects • Classifies according to form, color or use • Able to match sets (42 months) • Uses 800 words • Uses 1000 to 1500 words • Answers 14 agent + action questions • Uses 1500 to 2000 words • Responds appropriately to simple “how” • Answers 13 agent + action questions • Responds appropriately – not necessarily • Repeats two nonsense syllables question • Can do simple verbal analogies (Daddy is correctly, to “how far” questions • Answers simple “when” questions (when • Can answer two or three questions (what do a man, Mommy is a …..) • Defines four words in terms of use do you sleep?) you do when you’re hungry/sleepy/cold?) • Answers (responds appropriately)
Recommended publications
  • Manual for Language Test Development and Examining
    Manual for Language Test Development and Examining For use with the CEFR Produced by ALTE on behalf of the Language Policy Division, Council of Europe © Council of Europe, April 2011 The opinions expressed in this work are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the official policy of the Council of Europe. All correspondence concerning this publication or the reproduction or translation of all or part of the document should be addressed to the Director of Education and Languages of the Council of Europe (Language Policy Division) (F-67075 Strasbourg Cedex or [email protected]). The reproduction of extracts is authorised, except for commercial purposes, on condition that the source is quoted. Manual for Language Test Development and Examining For use with the CEFR Produced by ALTE on behalf of the Language Policy Division, Council of Europe Language Policy Division Council of Europe (Strasbourg) www.coe.int/lang Contents Foreword 5 3.4.2 Piloting, pretesting and trialling 30 Introduction 6 3.4.3 Review of items 31 1 Fundamental considerations 10 3.5 Constructing tests 32 1.1 How to define language proficiency 10 3.6 Key questions 32 1.1.1 Models of language use and competence 10 3.7 Further reading 33 1.1.2 The CEFR model of language use 10 4 Delivering tests 34 1.1.3 Operationalising the model 12 4.1 Aims of delivering tests 34 1.1.4 The Common Reference Levels of the CEFR 12 4.2 The process of delivering tests 34 1.2 Validity 14 4.2.1 Arranging venues 34 1.2.1 What is validity? 14 4.2.2 Registering test takers 35 1.2.2 Validity
    [Show full text]
  • Second Language Acquisition Through Neurolinguistic Programming: a Psychoanalytic Approach
    International Journal of Engineering & Technology, 7 (4.36) (2018) 624-629 International Journal of Engineering & Technology Website: www.sciencepubco.com/index.php/IJET Research paper Second Language Acquisition Through Neurolinguistic Programming: A Psychoanalytic Approach A. Delbio1*, M. Ilankumaran2 1Research Scholar in English, Noorul Islam Centre for Higher Education, Kumaracoil. 2Professor of English, Noorul Islam Centre for Higher Education, Kumaracoil, Thuckalay, Tamilnadu, India. E-mail:[email protected] *Corresponding author E-mail: [email protected] Abstract English is the only lingua-franca for the whole world in present age of globalization and liberalization. English language is considered as an important tool to acquire a new and technical information and knowledge. In this situation English learners and teachers face a lot of problems psychologically. Neuro linguistic studies the brain mechanism and the performance of the brain in linguistic competences. The brain plays a main role in controlling motor and sensory activities and in the process of thinking. Studies regarding development of brain bring some substantiation for psychological and anatomical way of language development. Neuro-Linguistic Programming (NLP) deals with psychological and neurological factors. It also deals with the mode of brain working and the way to train the brain to achieve the purpose. Many techniques are used in the NLP. It improves the fluency and accuracy in target language. It improves non-native speaker to improve the LSRW skills. This paper brings out the importance of the NLP in language learning and teaching. It also discusses the merits and demerits of the NLP in learning. It also gives the solution to overcome the problems and self-correction is motivated through neuro-linguistic programming.
    [Show full text]
  • CONNECTED SPEECH IPA Symbols Represent the Correct Pronunciation
    CONNECTED SPEECH IPA symbols represent the correct pronunciation of single words or segments of speech. However, we use single words or segments in isolation only occasionally. E.g., we can say “ah!” /ɑː/ to express surprise, or “sh!” /ʃ/ if we want to tell somebody to be quiet. Most commonly we use segments of speech or single words in a sequence, to form phrases or sentences. The sound “ah!” /ɑː/, for example, is likely to be found in whole sentences, to express: - surprise (“Ah, there you are!”) - pleasure (“Ah, this coffee is good.”) - admiration or sympathy (“Ah well, better luck next time.”) - when you disagree with somebody (“Ah, but that may not be true.”) In fact, we commonly use language in continuous, connected speech. Connected speech means speech produced without pauses. A consequence of connected speech is that single segments of speech are influenced by neighbouring segments (that is to say, speech sounds that come before and after them), and may slightly change their place or manner of articulation, or may sometimes totally disappear. Therefore the pronunciation of an isolated word may be different from the pronunciation of the same word in connected speech. These changes fall into two main types: assimilation and elision. ASSIMILATION In assimilation, one sound becomes phonetically similar to an adjacent sound. For example, the two words this and shop in isolation are pronounced - this /ðɪs/ - shop /ʃɒp/. In rapid speech, when the two words are pronounced together, the sound /s/ in this is influenced by the following initial sound and changes to /ʃ/. What we actually hear is: - this shop /ðɪʃʃɒp/.
    [Show full text]
  • Statistical Language Learning: Learning-Oriented Theories Is That Mechanisms and Constraints Such Accounts Seem at Odds with One of the Central Observations 1 Jenny R
    110 VOLUME 12, NUMBER 4, AUGUST 2003 most important arguments against Statistical Language Learning: learning-oriented theories is that Mechanisms and Constraints such accounts seem at odds with one of the central observations 1 Jenny R. Saffran about human languages. The lin- Department of Psychology and Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin–Madison, guistic systems of the world, de- Madison, Wisconsin spite surface differences, share deep similarities, and vary in non- arbitrary ways. Theories of language Abstract guage, it seems improbable that chil- acquisition that focus primarily on What types of mechanisms dren could ever discern its structure. preexisting knowledge of language underlie the acquisition of hu- The process of acquiring such a sys- do provide an elegant explanation man language? Recent evidence tem is likely to be nearly as com- for cross-linguistic similarities. suggests that learners, includ- plex as the system itself, so it is not Such theories, which are exempli- ing infants, can use statistical surprising that the mechanisms un- fied by the seminal work of Noam properties of linguistic input to derlying language acquisition are a Chomsky, suggest that linguistic uni- discover structure, including matter of long-standing debate. One versals are prespecified in the child’s sound patterns, words, and the of the central focuses of this debate linguistic endowment, and do not re- beginnings of grammar. These concerns the innate and environ- quire learning. Such accounts gener- abilities appear to be both pow- mental contributions to the lan- ate predictions about the types of erful and constrained, such that guage-acquisition process, and the patterns that should be observed some statistical patterns are degree to which these components cross-linguistically, and lead to im- more readily detected and used draw on information and abilities portant claims regarding the evolu- than others.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Acquisition Device and the Origin of Language Briana Sobecks
    Language Acquisition Device and the Origin of Language Briana Sobecks In the early twentieth century, psychologists re- a particular language, the universal grammar can be refined alized that language is not just understanding words, but to fit a specific language. Even if some children may hear a also requires learning grammar, syntax, and semantics. specific language pattern more than others, the fact that all Modern language is incredibly complex, but young chil- children know it indicates a poss ble innate language sense. dren can understand it remarkably well. This idea supports One of Chomsky’s main tenants in his LAD theory Chomsky’s idea that language learning is innate. According is the Poverty of Stimulus argument. Though children do to his hypothesis, young children receive “primary linguistic collect data to learn a language, it is unlikely that the data data” from what is spoken around them, which helps them they are exposed to is enough to master an entire lan- develop knowledge of that specific language (Cowie 2008). guage. Instead, they must infer grammatical rules through Children passively absorb language from adults, peers, an internal sense. There are several cognitive factors that and exposure to media. However, this data is not sufficient support this argument. Underdetermination states that the to explain how children can learn unique constructions finite data is applicable in infinite situations. In context, of words and grammar patterns. Previously structuralists this means that children utilize the finite amount of data created a list of “phrase structure rules” to generate all they hear to generate any possible sentence.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonetic and Phonological Systems Analysis (PPSA) User Notes For
    Phonetic and Phonological Systems Analysis (PPSA) User Notes for English Systems Sally Bates* and Jocelynne Watson** *University of St Mark and St John **Queen Margaret University, Edinburgh “A fully comprehensive analysis is not required for every child. A systematic, principled analysis is, however, necessary in all cases since it forms an integral part of the clinical decision-making process.” Bates & Watson (2012, p 105) Sally Bates & Jocelynne Watson (Authors) QMU & UCP Marjon © Phonetic and Phonological Systems Analysis (PPSA) is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-Non-Commercial-NoDerivs 3.0 Unported License. Table of Contents Phonetic and Phonological Systems Analysis (PPSA) Introduction 3 PPSA Child 1 Completed PPSA 5 Using the PPSA (Page 1) Singleton Consonants and Word Structure 8 PI (Phonetic Inventory) 8 Target 8 Correct Realisation 10 Errored Realisation and Deletion 12 Other Errors 15 Using the PPSA (Page 2) Consonant Clusters 16 WI Clusters 17 WM Clusters 18 WF Clusters 19 Using the PPSA (Page 3) Vowels 20 Using the PPSA (Page 2) Error Pattern Summary 24 Child 1 Interpretation 25 Child 5 Data Sample 27 Child 5 Completed PPSA 29 Child 5 Interpretation 32 Advantages of the PPSA – why we like this approach 34 What the PPSA doesn’t do 35 References 37 Key points of the Creative Commons License operating with this PPSA Resource 37 N.B. We recommend that the reader has a blank copy of the 3 page PPSA to follow as they go through this guide. This is also available as a free download (PPSA Charting and Summary Form) under the same creative commons license conditions.
    [Show full text]
  • Laryngeal Features in German* Michael Jessen Bundeskriminalamt, Wiesbaden Catherine Ringen University of Iowa
    Phonology 19 (2002) 189–218. f 2002 Cambridge University Press DOI: 10.1017/S0952675702004311 Printed in the United Kingdom Laryngeal features in German* Michael Jessen Bundeskriminalamt, Wiesbaden Catherine Ringen University of Iowa It is well known that initially and when preceded by a word that ends with a voiceless sound, German so-called ‘voiced’ stops are usually voiceless, that intervocalically both voiced and voiceless stops occur and that syllable-final (obstruent) stops are voiceless. Such a distribution is consistent with an analysis in which the contrast is one of [voice] and syllable-final stops are devoiced. It is also consistent with the view that in German the contrast is between stops that are [spread glottis] and those that are not. On such a view, the intervocalic voiced stops arise because of passive voicing of the non-[spread glottis] stops. The purpose of this paper is to present experimental results that support the view that German has underlying [spread glottis] stops, not [voice] stops. 1 Introduction In spite of the fact that voiced (obstruent) stops in German (and many other Germanic languages) are markedly different from voiced stops in languages like Spanish, Russian and Hungarian, all of these languages are usually claimed to have stops that contrast in voicing. For example, Wurzel (1970), Rubach (1990), Hall (1993) and Wiese (1996) assume that German has underlying voiced stops in their different accounts of Ger- man syllable-final devoicing in various rule-based frameworks. Similarly, Lombardi (1999) assumes that German has underlying voiced obstruents in her optimality-theoretic (OT) account of syllable-final laryngeal neutralisation and assimilation in obstruent clusters.
    [Show full text]
  • The Phonetics-Phonology Interface in Romance Languages José Ignacio Hualde, Ioana Chitoran
    Surface sound and underlying structure : The phonetics-phonology interface in Romance languages José Ignacio Hualde, Ioana Chitoran To cite this version: José Ignacio Hualde, Ioana Chitoran. Surface sound and underlying structure : The phonetics- phonology interface in Romance languages. S. Fischer and C. Gabriel. Manual of grammatical interfaces in Romance, 10, Mouton de Gruyter, pp.23-40, 2016, Manuals of Romance Linguistics, 978-3-11-031186-0. hal-01226122 HAL Id: hal-01226122 https://hal-univ-paris.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01226122 Submitted on 24 Dec 2016 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Manual of Grammatical Interfaces in Romance MRL 10 Brought to you by | Université de Paris Mathematiques-Recherche Authenticated | [email protected] Download Date | 11/1/16 3:56 PM Manuals of Romance Linguistics Manuels de linguistique romane Manuali di linguistica romanza Manuales de lingüística románica Edited by Günter Holtus and Fernando Sánchez Miret Volume 10 Brought to you by | Université de Paris Mathematiques-Recherche Authenticated | [email protected] Download Date | 11/1/16 3:56 PM Manual of Grammatical Interfaces in Romance Edited by Susann Fischer and Christoph Gabriel Brought to you by | Université de Paris Mathematiques-Recherche Authenticated | [email protected] Download Date | 11/1/16 3:56 PM ISBN 978-3-11-031178-5 e-ISBN (PDF) 978-3-11-031186-0 e-ISBN (EPUB) 978-3-11-039483-2 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data A CIP catalog record for this book has been applied for at the Library of Congress.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Processes
    Phonological Processes Phonological processes are patterns of articulation that are developmentally appropriate in children learning to speak up until the ages listed below. PHONOLOGICAL PROCESS DESCRIPTION AGE ACQUIRED Initial Consonant Deletion Omitting first consonant (hat → at) Consonant Cluster Deletion Omitting both consonants of a consonant cluster (stop → op) 2 yrs. Reduplication Repeating syllables (water → wawa) Final Consonant Deletion Omitting a singleton consonant at the end of a word (nose → no) Unstressed Syllable Deletion Omitting a weak syllable (banana → nana) 3 yrs. Affrication Substituting an affricate for a nonaffricate (sheep → cheep) Stopping /f/ Substituting a stop for /f/ (fish → tish) Assimilation Changing a phoneme so it takes on a characteristic of another sound (bed → beb, yellow → lellow) 3 - 4 yrs. Velar Fronting Substituting a front sound for a back sound (cat → tat, gum → dum) Backing Substituting a back sound for a front sound (tap → cap) 4 - 5 yrs. Deaffrication Substituting an affricate with a continuant or stop (chip → sip) 4 yrs. Consonant Cluster Reduction (without /s/) Omitting one or more consonants in a sequence of consonants (grape → gape) Depalatalization of Final Singles Substituting a nonpalatal for a palatal sound at the end of a word (dish → dit) 4 - 6 yrs. Stopping of /s/ Substituting a stop sound for /s/ (sap → tap) 3 ½ - 5 yrs. Depalatalization of Initial Singles Substituting a nonpalatal for a palatal sound at the beginning of a word (shy → ty) Consonant Cluster Reduction (with /s/) Omitting one or more consonants in a sequence of consonants (step → tep) Alveolarization Substituting an alveolar for a nonalveolar sound (chew → too) 5 yrs.
    [Show full text]
  • EVALUATING an INSTRUMENT for ASSESSING CONNECTED SPEECH PERFORMANCE USING FACETS ANALYSIS YOON AH SEONG University of Hawai‘I at M Ānoa
    EVALUATING AN INSTRUMENT FOR ASSESSING CONNECTED SPEECH PERFORMANCE USING FACETS ANALYSIS YOON AH SEONG University of Hawai‘i at M ānoa ABSTRACT In the area of English pronunciation teaching, connected speech is increasingly being introduced and covered in pronunciation textbooks (e.g., Hagen, 2000; Weinstein, 2001). Connected speech is a phenomenon in spoken language that collectively includes phonological processes such as reduction, elision, intrusion, assimilation, and contraction. Several research studies have shown that connected speech instruction can help learners to more easily comprehend rapid speech used by native speakers (e.g., Brown & Hilferty, 2006; Celce-Murcia, Brinton, & Goodwin, 1996; Matsuzawa, 2006). Moreover, use of connected speech features can make learners sound more comprehensible and natural with less marked foreign accent (Brown & Kondo-Brown, 2006a; Dauer & Browne, 1992). However, compared to the growing connected speech literature regarding what forms to teach and how, there seems to be very little information on how to assess connected speech especially in terms of production. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to develop and evaluate a new test of connected speech performance within the context of an English study abroad program. The multi-faceted Rasch software FACETS was used to examine the effectiveness of the test instrument. The analyses used data from two administrations, a pretest and a posttest, and examined the relationships between examinee scores and various aspects of the testing situation (i.e., facets). The four facets investigated in this study were: (a) the examinees, (b) items, (c) raters, and (d) the rater L1 background. The results indicated that assessing the production of certain connected speech forms using this type of test instrument has potential.
    [Show full text]
  • Connected Speech in Romanian: Exploring Sound Change Through an ASR System Ioana Chitorana, Ioana Vasilescub, Bianca Vieruc, Lori Lamelb
    Connected speech in Romanian: Exploring sound change through an ASR system Ioana Chitorana, Ioana Vasilescub, Bianca Vieruc, Lori Lamelb a Université Paris Diderot and Laboratoire de Phonétique et Phonologie (UMR 7018) b LIMSI CNRS c Vocapia Research Abstract This study explores the hypothesis that traces of sound change can be found in connected speech processes as synchronic variation. To test this hypothesis we study the variation in connected speech through the output of an ASR system (Adda-Decker and Lamel, 1999). The case study is the vowel alternation [e]–[ʌ] after labials, encountered in Romanian historically, as part of a relatively sporadic sound change with unclear conditioning, as well as synchronically, as a phenomenon commonly observed in continuous speech. The specific question we address is whether the synchronic phenomenon in Romanian continuous speech is simply the result of general vowel reduction, or may reflect the sound change, with its apparent sensitivity to a labial consonantal context. We focus on the prepositions [pe] ‘on’ and [de] ‘of’, chosen specifically for their status as function words, more prone to vowel reduction than lexical words. We compare the ASR system’s choice of two transcription variants and their respective contexts. The results of this comparison suggest that the synchronic variation is grounded in the historical sound change, thus motivating a future systematic study of controlled speech, to determine the exact role of segmental contextual factors. The main result is that the selection of transcription variants is significantly different for the two prepositions. This allows us to better understand the conditioning environment of the relevant sound change, and reveals the language-specific articulatory and co-articulatory settings and dynamic patterns that may have led to it, as they emerge and can be observed in continuous speech.
    [Show full text]
  • Modeling Language Variation and Universals: a Survey on Typological Linguistics for Natural Language Processing
    Modeling Language Variation and Universals: A Survey on Typological Linguistics for Natural Language Processing Edoardo Ponti, Helen O ’Horan, Yevgeni Berzak, Ivan Vulic, Roi Reichart, Thierry Poibeau, Ekaterina Shutova, Anna Korhonen To cite this version: Edoardo Ponti, Helen O ’Horan, Yevgeni Berzak, Ivan Vulic, Roi Reichart, et al.. Modeling Language Variation and Universals: A Survey on Typological Linguistics for Natural Language Processing. 2018. hal-01856176 HAL Id: hal-01856176 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01856176 Preprint submitted on 9 Aug 2018 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Modeling Language Variation and Universals: A Survey on Typological Linguistics for Natural Language Processing Edoardo Maria Ponti∗ Helen O’Horan∗∗ LTL, University of Cambridge LTL, University of Cambridge Yevgeni Berzaky Ivan Vuli´cz Department of Brain and Cognitive LTL, University of Cambridge Sciences, MIT Roi Reichart§ Thierry Poibeau# Faculty of Industrial Engineering and LATTICE Lab, CNRS and ENS/PSL and Management, Technion - IIT Univ. Sorbonne nouvelle/USPC Ekaterina Shutova** Anna Korhonenyy ILLC, University of Amsterdam LTL, University of Cambridge Understanding cross-lingual variation is essential for the development of effective multilingual natural language processing (NLP) applications.
    [Show full text]