<<

Tit for Tat? The Spiraling Effect of in the Author(s): Lynne M. Andersson and Christine M. Pearson Source: The Academy of Management Review, Vol. 24, No. 3 (Jul., 1999), pp. 452-471 Published by: Academy of Management Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/259136 . Accessed: 06/10/2014 11:55

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

.

Academy of Management is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Academy of Management Review.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions t Academy of Management Review 1999, Vol. 24, No. 3, 452-471.

TIT FOR TAT? THE SPIRALINGEFFECT OF INCIVILITYIN THE WORKPLACE

LYNNEM. ANDERSSON Saint Joseph's University

CHRISTINEM. PEARSON University of North Carolina

In this article we introduce the concept of and explain how incivility can potentially spiral into increasingly intense aggressive behaviors. To gain an understanding of the mechanisms that underlie an "incivility spiral," we examine what happens at key points: the starting and tipping points. Furthermore, we describe several factors that can facilitate the occurrence and escalation of an inci- vility spiral and the secondary spirals that can result. We offer research propositions and discuss implications of workplace incivility for researchers and practitioners.

You should follow honorable mean and vent your of the many sacrifices we are called to make for wrath on the wicked (Disticha Catoni, A Medieval the sake of living together" (1998: 11) and Wilson Textbook, as quoted in Elias, 1982:63). as "a way of signaling the existence of self- traditionally has been viewed by so- control" (1993: 83). Although manifest in varied ciety as a source of power in American cul- ways, norms concerning how people ought to ture-a means of gaining favor and asserting behave in order to live cooperatively can be cultural superiority-an acceptable ploy for at- witnessed in every community and culture taining social advantage. The spread of civility (Elias, 1982; Goffman, 1967; Hartman, 1996). Thus, has served to muffle the issue of class, softening civility, as a moral standard, can be considered the divisions between rich and poor and em- a virtue. ployers and employees (Elias, 1982; Morris, 1996). According to some social scientists and histo- In scholarly authors have suggested civil- rians (e.g., Carter, 1998; Chen & Eastman, 1997; ity serves as the vehicle for providing answers Elias, 1982; Erickson, 1962; Goffman, 1967), the to unanswered questions of conduct (Bellah, need for civility becomes even greater when the 1970) and have linked civility to such related interactions among people increase in complex- phenomena as the necessity for ritualized be- ity and frequency. Parties in complex interac- havior in light of divorce (Johnson, 1988), the tions must attune their conduct to that of others foundation for human rationality necessary for by behaving in predictably "civil" ways: successful education (Shulman & Carey, 1984), and the courteous treatment of professional col- The web of actions must be organized more and more strictly and accurately, if each in individual leagues correspondence and feedback (Rob- action is to fulfill its social function. The individ- erts, 1985). ual is compelled to regulate his conduct in an Nonetheless, civility is not only functional or increasingly differentiated, more even and more instrumental but holds moral implications as stable manner.... This seeks to prevent offens- well. The basis for civility is love of thy neigh- es.. . (Elias, 1982:232). bor-a demonstration of respect for fellow hu- As we approach the next millennium, we face man beings (Carter, 1998; Elias, 1982; Wilson, the growing challenge of relationships medi- 1993). Carter has referred to civility as "the sum ated by high-tech, asynchronous, global interac- tion. With history as counsel, one might assume a need for increased civility in forging and rec- We greatly appreciate the inspiring comments of Blake onciling increasingly complex interactions. Yet, Ashforth and the three anonymous reviewers on several drafts of this article. Their patience, persistence, and in- despite the implicit need for increasingly civil sightful thinking contributed immensely to our conceptual- interaction, a recent poll of the American public ization of incivility in this and related works. revealed that 90 percent of the respondents

452

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1999 Andersson and Pearson 453 think incivility is a serious problem (Marks, number of ways to show disregard for fellow 1996). workers (Carter, 1998; Marks, 1996; Neuman & Today-some scholars and social critics be- Baron, 1997). lieve-we support an ethic of self-expression, In the burgeoning stream of research on "de- and we detest the pretense of civility because viant behavior," "," and "violence" in we believe it denies our desire for freedom and the workplace, researchers have focused mainly individuality (e.g., Gordon, 1989; Morris, 1996; on physical, active, and direct forms of aggres- Steinberg, 1996; Wilson, 1993). This has been sion-those overt types of aggression undoubt- deemed the age of "whatever," implying that no edly motivated by intent to harm (e.g., Folger, one wants to make a judgment, impose a stan- Robinson, Dietz, McLean Parks, & Baron, 1998; dard, or call conduct unacceptable (Morris, 1996). O'Leary-Kelly, Griffin, & Glew, 1996; Perlow & Historians may view the dawn of the twenty- Latham, 1993; Robinson & Bennett, 1995, 1997; first century as a time of thoughtless acts and Robinson & O'Leary-Kelly, 1996; VandenBos & : we tailgate, even in the slow lane; Bulatao, 1996). Data have confirmed that aggres- we dial wrong numbers and then slam the sion and violence occur in the American work- receiver on the innocent respondent; we break place. Over 20 percent of the human resource appointments with nonchalance. Indeed, the managers participating in a recent study re- nineties have been characterized by rudeness ported that their had experienced to such an extent that " experts" are since 1990, and an addi- proliferating, spreading the gospel of good tional 33 percent reported that there had been manners to families, social and organizations, threats of violence in their workplace (Romano, businesses (e.g., Hamilton & Sullivan, 1997; 1994). During a 12-month period ending in 1993, Martin, 1996). an estimated 2.2 million U.S. workers were vic- The business world was thought by many to tims of physical attacks, 6.3 million were threat- be one of the last bastions of civility. The rela- ened, and 16.1 million were harassed (North- tionship between coworkers was, for decades, western National Life Insurance Company, characterized by formality yet friendliness, dis- 1993). It has been estimated that, on the average, tance yet politeness. However, business has there are more than 2 million physical assaults started to reflect the informality of society at in the workplace per year, and over large. Scholars have cited employee , 1,000 homi- reengineering, downsizing, budget cuts, in- cides (Segal, 1994). creased pressures for productivity, autocratic Little research, however, has been conducted work environments, and the use of part-time em- on lesser forms of mistreatment, such as rude ployees as causes for the increase in uncivil and comments, thoughtless acts, or negative ges- aggressive workplace behaviors (Baron & Neu- tures (Neuman & Baron, 1997). Nonetheless, a man, 1996; Chen & Eastman, 1997; Neuman & survey of 178 employees revealed that a major- Baron, 1997). As organizations have flattened ity of the aggression occurring in work settings and gone casual, there are fewer obvious cues is of a less intense form: verbal rather than as to what constitutes "proper" business behav- physical, passive rather than active, indirect ior (Martin, 1996; Morand, 1998). rather than direct, and subtle rather than overt Examples of incivility in the workplace (Baron & Neuman, 1996). Several other studies abound: answering the phone with a "yeah," have shown similar findings. In a survey of 338 neglecting to say thank you or please, using university employees in Finland, Bjorkqvist, Os- voice mail to screen calls, leaving a half cup of terman, and Hjelt-Back (1994) found that 32 per- coffee behind to avoid having to brew the next cent of the respondents had observed others be- pot, standing uninvited but impatiently over the ing exposed to verbally harassing behavior at desk of someone engaged in a telephone con- work. A survey of first-line American workers versation, dropping trash on the floor and leav- revealed that more than half of the 327 respon- ing it for the maintenance crew to clean up, and dents reported experiencing acts of mistreat- talking loudly on the phone about personal mat- ment at work within a 3-year time frame (Ehrlich ters (Martin, 1996). As the complexity of work- & Larcom, 1994). Moreover, in a survey of 603 place interaction increases, discourteous behav- Toronto nurses, Graydon, Kasta, and Khan ior has more nuances: there are a greater (1994) found that 33 percent had experienced

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 454 Academy of Management Review July

verbal during their previous 5 days of DEFINITIONALISSUES work. Less intense forms of mistreatment in Civility and Incivility the workplace, such as incivil-ities, deserve scholarly attention. The terms civility and incivility have come Of particular interest is whether these less into vogue in the past several years. Overused intense forms of mistreatment can be precursors and often misconstrued, both have lost some of to more intense, overtly aggressive, and/or vio- their intended meaning. Encompassing every- lent acts (Baron & Neuman, 1996; MacKinnon, thing from etiquette to professional conduct, 1994). In the poll of the American public men- from civic order to a moral imperative (Carter, tioned previously, 91 percent of the respondents 1998; Gladwell, 1996; Martin, 1996; Roberts, 1985), has surveyed believe that incivility has contributed "civility" transcended its dictionary defini- tion of "courtesy and politeness toward fellow to the increase in violence in this country human beings" (Random House (Marks; 1996). Some empirical research has ver- Dictionary). Civil behavior involves treating others with dig- ified this belief. For instance, researchers have nity, acting with regard to others' feelings, and shown incivilities to be highly correlated with preserving the social norms for mutual respect , progressing in an upward-spiraling pro- (Carter, 1998; Elias, 1982; Johnson, 1988; Morris, cess to increasingly serious levels (Goldstein, 1996). Observing formal rules of etiquette has 1994; Taylor & Gottfredson, 1986). In a study of less to do with civility than does being polite incarcerated males, Felson and Steadman and demonstrating a sensibility of concern and (1983) revealed that the sequence of events regard (Carter, 1998). leading up to assault invariably begins with Workplace civility, then-as a behavior in- an exchange of rude comments, which gener- volving politeness and regard for others in the ates an attack on identity and spirals ulti- workplace, within workplace norms for re- mately to physical attack. Further, Spratlen spect-can be distinguished from similar work- (1994) found workplace mistreatment in a place behaviors and values, such as prosocial health care setting to be directly related to (e.g., Brief & Motowidlo, interpersonal violence. 1986), organizational citizenship behavior (e.g., It has been suggested that, in the workplace, Organ, 1988), ingratiation (e.g., Yukl & Falbe, violence is rarely a spontaneous act but more 1990), and concern for others (Ravlin & Meglino, often the culmination of escalating patterns of 1987). Like many extrarole behaviors in organi- negative interaction between individuals (Baron zations, civility includes modest, trivial behav- & Neuman, 1996; Kinney, 1995). Thus, workplace iors that do not often invite public scrutiny or incivility may very well be a precursor to more official documentation (Van Dyne, Cummings, & intense, overtly aggressive acts in the work- McLean Parks, 1995). However, unlike such ex- place. trarole behaviors as prosocial organizational Our aim in this article is to introduce the con- behavior and organizational citizenship behav- cept of workplace incivility and examine how it ior, workplace civility does not necessarily im- ply the may relate to more intense forms of workplace intent of benefit to the . One may be civil with intent aggression. To capture some of the less intense to benefit the organiza- tion, or one may be civil without intent forms of organizational mistreatment, we (e.g., because it is "the right thing to do"). present the concept of workplace incivility. We Similarly, one can distinguish civility from the establish a place for workplace incivility among influence tactic of ingratiation-a behavior in several of the the other conceptualizations of which an agent seeks to increase a target's feel- mistreatment in organizations, portraying inci- ings of positive regard in order to get the target vility as a social interaction. We then explain, to do something (Yukl & Falbe, 1990)-because building a framework and offering research of the apparent intent to influence that is inher- propositions, how incivility can spiral and po- ent in such behavior. Further, workplace civility tentially escalate into increasingly intense, ag- can be differentiated from the work value "con- gressive workplace behaviors. Finally, we dis- cern for others" (Ravlin & Meglino, 1987). cuss some of the research and practical Whereas a work value is an intrinsic, enduring implications. perspective of what is right or wrong in a work

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1999 Andersson and Pearson 455 setting, capable of influencing perceptions and behavior that disrupts mutual respect in the behaviors, workplace civility is a behavior that workplace. helps to preserve the norms for mutual respect in the workplace. Civility reflects concern for Incivility and Other Forms of Mistreatment in others. Organizations Like civility, "incivility" has taken on a variety of nuances-from breaches of etiquette to pro- Mistreatment in organizations has been de- fessional , from general civil unrest scribed, modeled, and analyzed in various con- to moral decay (Carter, 1998; Gladwell, 1996; ceptual forms: as aggressive (e.g., Baron & Neu- Johnson, 1988; Martin, 1996; Roberts, 1985). The man, 1996; O'Leary-Kelly et al., 1996), violent dictionary definition of incivility is more spe- (e.g., Kinney, 1995; VandenBos & Bulatao, 1996), cific, however. The Random House Dictionary harassing (e.g., Bjorkqvist et al., 1994), physi- defines incivility as "the quality or condition of cally abusive (e.g., Perlow & Latham, 1993), ty- being uncivil, uncivil behavior or treatment" rannical (e.g., Ashforth, 1994), deviant (e.g., Rob- and uncivil as "without good manners, unman- inson & Bennett, 1995, 1997), and antisocial (e.g., nerly, rude, impolite, discourteous." As the an- Giacalone & Greenberg, 1997) workplace behav- tonym of civility, incivility implies rudeness and iors. Some of these concepts capture more in- disregard for others, in violation of norms for tense forms of mistreatment-those with obvi- respect in interpersonal relations (Brown & ous intent to harm-yet to include Levinson, 1987; Morris, 1996). In the criminology recognition of the less intense forms of mistreat- literature, for example, authors have defined in- ment, in which intent to harm is less obvious. civilities as "low-level breaches of community Some represent violation of workplace norms, standards that signal an erosion of convention- whereas others do not necessarily involve norm ally accepted norms and values" (LaGrange, violation. What must be addressed at this point Ferraro, & Supancic, 1992: 311-312). Whereas is how the concept of workplace incivility differs civil behavior is expected and often goes unno- from and overlaps with these other conceptual- ticed, uncivil behavior is conspicuous (Brown & izations of mistreatment in organizations. Levinson, 1987; Sapir, 1927). Aggressive behavior and violence have re- We have established that incivility involves ceived recent attention in the academic and acting rudely or discourteously, without regard practitioner management literature (e.g., Baron for others, in violation of norms for respect in & Neuman, 1996; Folger et al., 1998; Kinney, 1995; social interactions. It follows, then, that work- Neuman & Baron, 1997; O'Leary-Kelly et al., 1996; place incivility involves acting with disregard Robinson & O'Leary-Kelly, 1996; VandenBos & for others in the workplace, in violation of work- Bulatao, 1996; Weisinger, 1995). Although there place norms for respect. Workplace norms are has been some disagreement among social sci- the norms of the community of which one is a entists in the fields of criminology, psychology, part while at work, consisting of basic moral and sociology as to the definitions of and differ- standards and others that have arisen out of the ences between aggression and violence (Ted- tradition of that community, including those pre- eschi & Felson, 1994), researchers examining ag- scribed by formal and informal organizational gression and violence in organizations seem to policies, rules, and procedures (Feldman, 1984; concur that aggression is attempted injurious or Hartman, 1996). We recognize that particular destructive behavior, in violation of social norms differ across organizations, industries, norms, and that violence is a high-intensity, and cultures, but we posit that in every work- physical form of aggression (Baron & Neuman, place there exist norms for respect for fellow 1996; VandenBos & Bulatao, 1996). coworkers-a shared moral understanding and A tremendous range and variety of acts con- sentiment among the members of the organiza- stitute , from tion that allow cooperation (Hartman, 1996; So- and to , , lomon, 1998)-and that incivility is in violation and homicide (Neuman & Baron, 1997). The com- of these norms. What is considered to be uncivil mon aspect of all of these acts of aggression is in one organization may not be universally the obvious intent to harm or injure someone considered uncivil, yet we can still hold a com- physically or psychologically (Baron & Richard- mon understanding of workplace incivility as son, 1994; Berkowitz, 1993; Neuman & Baron,

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 456 Academy of Management Review July

1997; Tedeschi & Felson, 1994). A distinguishing intense negative behaviors associated with the characteristic of incivility, however, is that the instigator's abuse of position of authority. intent to harm-as perceived through the eyes of Two other conceptualizations of workplace the instigator, the target, and/or the observ- mistreatment receiving recent attention are the ers-is ambiguous. One may behave uncivilly overlapping constructs of deviant and antisocial as a reflection of intent to harm the target, or one employee behaviors. Robinson and Bennett may behave uncivilly without intent (e.g., igno- have defined employee as "voluntary rance or oversight). Furthermore, the instigator behavior that violates significant organization- may intend to harm the target, yet he or she may al norms and, in so doing, threatens the well- not even be conscious of such intent. Unlike being of an organization, its members, or both" instigators of aggression, instigators of incivil- (1995: 556). This definition implies a broad range ity can easily deny or bury any intent, if present, of mistreatment-both of people and of property in ignorance of the effect (e.g., "It wasn't meant in organizations, with and without intent to as an attack"), in misinterpretation by the target harm-and is inclusive of workplace aggression (e.g., "I didn't mean to be rude; I was just in a and incivility. Even more broadly encompassing hurry"), or in hypersensitivity of the target (e.g., than employee deviance, and also inclusive of "Don't take it so personally;" Bies, Tripp, & workplace aggression and incivility, is antiso- Kramer, 1997; Kramer, 1994; Morrill, 1992). With cial employee behavior, which Giacalone and incivility, the intent is not transparent and is Greenberg have defined as "any behavior that subject to varying interpretation. brings harm, or is intended to bring harm, to an Incivility is similar in intensity to several di- organization, its employees, or stakeholders" mensions of the construct of petty tyranny (Ash- (1997: vii). forth, 1994). Both constructs include behaviors To summarize, we can illustrate (as shown in demonstrating a lack of consideration toward Figure 1) how incivility differs from and over- others, in which the intent to harm is ambigu- laps with some of these other forms of mistreat- ous. Petty tyranny, however, is a profile attrib- ment in organizations. Antisocial employee be- uted to leaders, referring also to a host of more havior, as behavior that brings harm to the

FIGURE 1 Incivility and Other Forms of Mistreatment in Organizations

Antisocial - Behavior that harms organization behavior and/or members

Deviant - Antisocial behavior that violates ms.u. ;3; .;;;., ; ;;;.;;u.;.;.m....ummummbehavior norms

*.uagu.a.~u~ ;;Violence - High-intensity, physically No ;.;...... ,; .aggressive behavior

Aggression - Deviant behavior with intent to harm

Incivility - Low-intensity deviant behavior with ambiguous intent to harm

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1999 Andersson and Pearson 457 organization and/or its stakeholders, is inclu- taken with the intent of imposing harm on an- sive of the other conceptualizations of mistreat- other person. Encompassing a fairly wide range ment in organizations. Deviant employee behav- of behaviors-from threats (the communication ior is a type of antisocial behavior that violates of intention to harm) to maligning , vio- workplace norms and includes employee ag- lence, and homicide-the term coercive actions gression and incivility. Aggression is inclusive includes all those moderate- to high-intensity of violence and of some forms of incivility (e.g., behaviors that occur to deter and compel others, those with intent to harm, but in which the in- to obtain justice, or to assert and defend one's tent-as perceived by the instigator, the target, identity. Because the term corresponds well with and/or observers-is ambiguous). Yet other our current conceptualization, we have chosen forms of incivility (e.g., those without intent to to use "coercive actions" and not "aggression" to harm, but in which the intent is ambiguous, describe those more intense behaviors beyond such as those that occur out of ignorance or incivility that involve the obvious intent to harm oversight) outside the realm of aggression. in organizations. Thus, in adopting the social Thus, incivility is, like aggression, a deviant interactionist perspective and the accompany- behavior, but one that is less intense and am- ing terminology, we propose that incivilities are biguous as to intent to harm. exchanged between individuals and that this Thus, based on the previous discussion and in dynamic interchange can, in turn, emerge into light of the conceptual similarities and distinc- an exchange of coercive actions whereby the tions among incivility and existing constructs, obvious intent to harm comes into play. we offer the following working definition of Unlike much of the recent research in which workplace incivility: researchers model workplace aggression and deviance as single acts in time-focusing on the Workplace incivility is low-intensity motives of the instigator (e.g., O'Leary-Kelly et deviant behavior with ambiguous in- al., 1996; Robinson & Bennett, 1997)-our social tent to harm the target, in violation of interactionist perspective emphasizes the inter- workplace norms for mutual respect. personal and situational factors involved in the Uncivil behaviors are characteristi- exchange of incivilities and coercive actions. cally rude and discourteous, display- This perspective allows us to examine incivility ing a lack of regard for others. and coercive action as processes rather than as events-processes affected by a particular set of constraints that make up the situation. Workplace Incivility As a Social Interaction That is not to say that the determination of Incivility is an interactive event-an event in whether an uncivil or coercive behavior is good which two or more parties are involved (Brown & or bad is dependent on the situation. We assert Levinson, 1987; Carter, 1998). The instigator(s), that incivility and coercive actions are negative the target(s), the observer(s), and the social con- behaviors but that it is important to consider the text all contribute to and are affected by an situation in understanding how the process of uncivil encounter. Emphasizing this characteris- exchange between parties unfolds. For exam- tic of incivility while recognizing its moral im- ple, the situation can sometimes cause instiga- plications, we adopt a social interactionist per- tors to perceive their own incivilities as legiti- spective on the concept of incivility and its mate or moralistic, potentially perpetuating the escalation, just as a recent and prominent the- exchange of negative behaviors. This does not ory of aggression-the theory of coercive actions make incivility "right" or "good;" it merely helps (Tedeschi & Felson, 1994)-takes a social inter- to explain how the negative behaviors continue actionist perspective on the concepts of aggres- or escalate. Considering the situation allows us sion and violence. to acknowledge that specific workplace norms In viewing aggression as a social interaction, do, indeed, vary, at the same time recognizing Tedeschi and Felson (1994) solve some of the that in every organization norms for mutual re- definitional problems inherent in the concept of spect are necessary for employees to work to- aggression by integrating the concepts of ag- gether (Hartman, 1996; Solomon, 1998). gression and violence into the concept of "coer- In the next section we provide a framework, cive action," which they describe as an action based on our view of incivility as a social inter-

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 458 Academy of Management Review July

action between two or more parties, depicting dying. The use of usually leads to coun- how incivility spirals begin and how they can tercoercion, resulting in an escalating spiral. As potentially escalate into an exchange of coer- the escalation of coercion progresses, the stakes cive actions within an organization. of the dispute seem to rise for both sides, and inhibitions about hurting the other side are re- duced (Pruitt & Rubin, 1986). Despite strong evi- THEINCIVILITY SPIRAL dence of the existence of these spirals, however, The pleasantries are the first to go: the pleases, scholars have devoted relatively little theoreti- the thank-yous, and the excuse-mes. Pretty soon, cal and empirical attention to the mechanisms no one holds a door open, returns a smile, or lets that underlie the escalation of coercive behavior another driver move ahead of them in traffic (Steinberg, 1996:13). (Kim & Smith, 1993). Baron and Neuman (1996) have posited that, in Spirals or circular patterns have been used to organizations, acts of "aggression" that are ver- explain several important phenomena in organ- bal and/or less intense in nature serve as the izations, at varying levels of analysis. For exam- initial step in an upward spiral that leads to ple, scholars have used spirals to explain such physical and/or more intense forms of aggres- phenomena as organizational decline (Ham- sion. Thus, we address this question in the fol- brick & D'Aveni, 1988; Masuch, 1985), the rela- lowing paragraphs: What sort of social interac- tionship between efficacy and performance tion transpires prior to the more overt acts of (Lindsley, Brass, & Thomas, 1995), and the per- aggression, before obvious intent to harm comes petuation of tyrannical leadership behaviors into play? We propose that an important aspect (Ashforth, 1994). Defined as a pattern of consec- of workplace incivility is that it can be a factor utive increases or decreases (Lindsley et al., in the formation and escalation of conflict spi- 1995), spirals in organizations are created by rals in organizations-that incivility may be a human actors because they lack adequate un- precursor to the exchange of coercive actions. In derstanding of their situation or are unwilling or an attempt to understand the mechanisms that unable to alter their behavior (Masuch, 1985). In underlie these incivility spirals, we examine examining the escalation of incivility into coer- what happens at key points in the spiral: the cive action in organizations, one would term the starting and tipping points. Furthermore, we de- potential spiral deviation amplifying, as the scribe some of the factors that can facilitate the negative action of one party leads to the nega- occurrence and escalation of an incivility spiral, tive action of the second party, which results as well as the secondary spirals that can result. in increasingly counterproductive behaviors Figure 2 presents a conceptual framework sum- (Masuch, 1985). marizing this potential spiraling effect of inci- The existence of interpersonal conflict spirals vility in the workplace. Although incivility spi- has been well documented. A number of re- rals can take numerous forms, involving varying searchers have demonstrated the relation be- numbers of parties and diverse types of uncivil tween perceived wrongdoing and subsequent and coercive behaviors, we show a sample inci- aggressive actions that escalate into a spiral of vility spiral depicting a possible dyadic interac- conflict (e.g., Bies & Tripp, 1995; Felson & Stead- tion in Figure 3. man, 1983; Luckenbill, 1977; Pruitt & Rubin, 1986; Youngs, 1986). One person mocks another; the The Starting second responds with an obscene . The Point first shoves; the second hits. And the conflict In a study of the sequence of events in aggres- escalates until one person is seriously wounded. sive interactions, Felson (1982) found that violent Examining 70 transactions that ended in mur- incidents usually begin when someone believes der, Luckenbill (1977) found that all involved a that a norm has been violated. Incivility, as a transaction in which the victim issued what the breach of norms for mutual respect, can engen- offender deemed an offensive move; the of- der perceptions of interactional injustice (Bies, fender retaliated with a verbal or physical chal- in press; Bies & Moag, 1986). When norms con- lenge; a working agreement favoring the use of cerning demeanor, consideration, and polite- violence was forged with the victim's response; ness are not met, perceptions of unfairness con- and a battle ensued, leaving the victim dead or cerning interpersonal treatment (interactional

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1999 Andersson and Pearson 459

mLso~>

p U

CL4

ai i >

P-

> .~~~~~~~~~~~~~>

...... -O 4 C1 CO ..

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 460 Academy of Management Review July

FIGURE3 Sample Incivility Spiral

- .< Perceived Coercive 4< Desire for coercive behavior nonproportional action (threat of physical revenge attack) I

Anger

Desire for Coercive Perceived Loss of ) revenge behavior coercive face ( (counterinsult) action

Anger

_%umm Loss of 4 Perceived Coercive 4 Desire for face coercive behavior revenge action (maligning insult) l

Anger Tipping Point 4 )

Desire for _ Uncivil Perceived _ Loss of %reciprocation behavior coercive face (rude remark) action Negative affect Departuri iInteractional -.A Perceived Uncivil _ Desire for injustice incivility behavior reciprocation (thoughtless act) Negative Starting Point affect Uncivil Perceived Interactional 4 behavior incivility injustice (thoughtless act)

Departure

Party A Party B (Instigator) (Target) injustice) occur (Bies, in press; Bies & Moag, 1994). The most commonly employed means of 1986; Solomon, 1998). Perceptions of interactional releasing negative affect and restoring fairness injustice then create negative affect and stimu- in this situation is to reciprocate with further late a desire to reciprocate the perceived unfair unfairness (Donnerstein & Hatfield, 1982; Kim & act (Berkowitz, 1993; Bies & Tripp, 1995; Donner- Smith, 1993), which, in turn, may lead to similar stein & Hatfield, 1982; Kim & Smith, 1993; Skar- perceptions and reactions by the other party, licki & Folger, 1997). Negative affect can cause thus potentially resulting in a cycle of injustice an individual to be less attentive to politeness (Patterson, 1982). norms and less inhibited by future costs of var- In the workplace an incivility spiral may be- ious action alternatives (Tedeschi & Felson, gin when an employee or group of employees

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1999 Andersson and Pearson 461

(Party A) performs an uncivil act toward another Proposition 1: Perception of interac- employee or group of employees (Party B). As tional injustice by the target in a so- depicted in the sample incivility spiral in Figure cial interaction will increase the prob- 3, Party B perceives the incivility and may cog- ability of the occurrence of an nitively interpret it as an interactional injustice. incivility spiral. This cognition may result in negative affect, Proposition 2: Feelings of negative af- which can stimulate in Party B a desire to recip- fect by the target in a social interac- rocate. This desire to reciprocate may not reflect tion will increase the probability of intent to psychologically or physically harm the occurrence of an incivility spiral. Party A, but merely to display the negative af- Proposition 3: Desire to reciprocate the fect that has been aroused. Party B then per- incivility by the target in a social in- forms an uncivil act in response to Party A, who teraction will increase the probability perceives the incivility, attributes it to Party B, of the occurrence of an incivility spi- and goes through the same cognitive, affective, ral. and behavioral response sequence experienced by Party B. Either party to the uncivil interaction may The Tipping Point choose departure as an alternative to continu- In a classic article Gouldner (1960) postulated ing the uncivil interaction: an uncivil behavior a generalized positive norm of reciprocity, stip- may be ignored by one of the parties at any time, ulating that (1) people should help those who or even if one party perceives an interactional have helped them, and (2) people should not injustice, he or she may choose not to respond in harm those who help them. Extending this kind, releasing the emotional energy (negative premise, Helm, Bonoma, and Tedeschi demon- affect) without reciprocating (Bies et al., 1997). strated that a negative norm of reciprocity ex- For example, Party B may depart from the spiral ists: the "frequency of reciprocated (counterag- by ignoring Party A, giving Party A the "benefit gression) was a direct and linear function of of the doubt," or deeming Party A unworthy of frequency of initial aggression delivered" (1972: further attention (Bies & Tripp, 1995; Shriver, 97). Further, they found that when initial aggres- 1995); or Party A may apologize, deny intent, sion is perceived by the target as unprovoked, and/or offer an excuse for the uncivil behavior the target employs counteraggression for re- (e.g., I'm sorry; I didn't mean to be rude, I was venge, rather than as a counterdeterrence. Thus, under a lot of stress"), prompting Party B to for- Helm and associates discovered that, in re- give Party A. In such situations the incivility venge, punishment may be more severe than the spiral will end. Several of the potential points of crime. This finding was replicated by Youngs, departure for Party B are illustrated by the who concluded that "one of the keys to the ex- dashed arrows in Figure 3. plosiveness of some (interaction) spirals may be Although Figure 3 shows only two parties, in the size of the deviation involved in overmatch- ing during the reality any number of parties can be involved in initial stages of conflict" (1986: 545). In this manner an ordinary conflict this exchange of incivilities. For example, Party situa- tion can suddenly develop into a crisis. A's incivility may be perceived by Parties B and Epidemiologists have used the term tipping C, who, in turn, act uncivil. Party A may then point to describe how an infectious disease sud- reciprocate in response solely to the incivility of denly escalates into epidemic proportions (e.g., Party B, or may reciprocate in response to the Gladwell, 1996). Others have also used the term multiple incivilities of Parties B and C. In what- to describe social phenomena, such as crime, ever form it takes, incivility represents a viola- denoting the point at which seemingly trivial tion of norms of mutual respect. As a conse- problems like petty crime and graffiti can esca- quence, such interactions are inherently late into widespread serious crime (Brown, 1978; disruptive to the social equilibrium, whether of Gladwell, 1996; Wilson & Kelling, 1982). Perhaps a group or an organization (Goffman, 1967). the most famous social science research on the The arguments in this section suggest the fol- tipping point is Zimbardo's (1969) "broken win- lowing propositions (shown in Figure 2): dow hypothesis," which posits that a single bro-

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 462 Academy of Management Review July ken window, if left unrepaired, can serve as a change of increasingly counterproductive be- tipping point to more heinous : the rela- haviors-each with the obvious intent to harm tively trivial signal suggests to potential insti- the other party (Masuch, 1985). Because of one gators that no one cares enough about the prop- particularly offensive incivility or the accumula- erty to replace the window, thus signaling that tion of incivilities over a period of time, one inflicting additional damage in the area will not party perceives an identity threat or a loss of warrant reprimand (Wilson & Kelling, 1982). The face, and the spiral suddenly escalates (Felson, term tipping point, therefore, is analogous to the 1982). transformation of water into steam with the ap- Identities play an important role in the esca- plication of heat; it can refer to a change in lation of coercive encounters (Felson & Stead- scope (escalation), as well as a change in com- man, 1983; Luckenbill, 1977). One's desired iden- position (alteration of form). tity-one's social face-is the combination of The concept of the tipping point can be ap- attributes (e.g., smart, capable, or strong) and plied to individual-level phenomena as well. social identities (e.g., gender or race) that one For example, an individual may experience the wants to present in a given situation (Erez & "straw that breaks the camel's back": the point Earley, 1993). In times of conflict, an image of at which the last small injustice in a chain of strength becomes very important to the identity injustices suddenly invokes a strong punitive of the individual (Tjosvold, 1983). Schlenker response (Morrill, 1992; Tedeschi & Felson, 1994). (1980) states, in his self-presentation theory, that A recovering alcoholic attempting to self- individuals engage in protective self-presenta- regulate her intake of alcohol at a party may tion (coercive actions) when their desired iden- sneak one drink, activating a "snowballing pat- tities are challenged or threatened. Thus, the tern" that involves her consuming ten more perception of an incivility as destructive criti- drinks during the course of the party (Baumeis- cism, an insult, or a threat-as an attack on ter, Heatherton, & Tice, 1994). In the workplace one's desired identity-can lead to the use of an employee may experience one incident after coercive behavior (Tedeschi & Felson, 1994). a series of aggravating encounters that acts as a Field and laboratory studies have revealed "triggering mechanism," causing the employee the effects of identity threats on aggressive be- to arrive at work the next morning with a gun havior. For example, in their study of incarcer- (Kinney, 1995). Each of these examples-the ated males who committed homicides and as- "straw that breaks the camel's back," the lapse saults, Felson and Steadman (1983) showed that in self-regulation leading to the "snowballing attacks on identity led to an exchange of threats pattern," and the "triggering mechanism"- and, ultimately, to physical attacks. Further- demonstrates a tipping point at which the indi- more, in a field study of former mental patients, vidual suddenly feels threatened by an adverse former criminal offenders, and the general pop- situation and somehow loses the to ulation, Felson (1982) found that individuals are maintain control over his or her actions. more likely to act aggressively when they have We use the notion of the tipping point in this been insulted. article to describe the relatively infrequent oc- The affective response to perceived loss of casion in which an exchange of incivilities es- face is usually anger (Averill, 1983; Berkowitz, calates into an exchange of coercive actions. We 1993; Tedeschi & Felson, 1994); the behavioral postulate that when at least one of the parties response can be revenge (Bies & Tripp, 1995; involved in an exchange of incivilities perceives Bies et al., 1997; Felson, 1982; Morrill, 1992). An- an identity threat, the tipping point is reached, ger, which increases the propensity for various prompting a more intense behavioral response forms of aggression (Allcorn, 1994; Anderson, by the threatened party (Helm et al., 1972; Deuser, & DeNeve, 1995; Weisinger, 1995), can Youngs, 1986) so that it escapes the confines of breed a particularly unconstrained urge to seek incivility (in which the goal of inflicting harm on revenge. Revenge, as the expression of per- the target remains ambiguous) and crosses into ceived loss of face and anger, is a claim by an the realm of coercive action (in which the goal of individual that he or she has socially valued inflicting harm to the target becomes obvious). attributes and is deserving of respectful behav- This is the point at which an incivility spiral ior. Revenge, unlike mere desire for reciproca- becomes a deviation-amplifying spiral-an ex- tion, obviously is intended to have harmful con-

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1999 Andersson and Pearson 463

sequences (Bies et al., 1997; Tedeschi & Felson, from the situation. Furthermore, even after 1994), and the taking of revenge can serve to reaching the tipping point, either party can re- restore one's degraded sense of self-worth-to frain from entering into an exchange of coercive reinstate a favorable identity (Kim & Smith, 1993; actions. Rather than engaging in the scenario Tjosvold, 1983). described above, for example, Party B could The level of revenge administered by an indi- choose to depart from the situation by ignoring vidual is a function of the perceived severity of the perceived coercive action or by cognitively harm to identity and the importance of the norm reinterpreting the situation (Bies et al. 1997; Ma- that has been violated (Tedeschi & Felson, 1994). such, 1985). Or, after experiencing a loss of face Revenge can act as a deterrence function, ward- and anger, Party B could choose to release his or ing off future conflict. If this were always the her anger without revenge behavior, venting to case, however, revenge would reduce, rather others about the situation, choosing a de- than escalate, conflict. Hard feelings would end escalating action alternative, or doing nothing once the score was evened. But research shows (Bies & Tripp, 1995; Bies et al. 1997; Shriver, 1995). that conflict escalation is frequently the result of It is also possible that Party A may apologize for nonproportional revenge (e.g., Felson & Stead- the behavior, prompting Party B to forgive Party man, 1983; Helm et al., 1972; Youngs, 1986). Re- A. We illustrate several of the potential points of venge produces counterattacks because one departure after the tipping point for Party B by party has subjective and often exaggerated as- the dashed arrows in Figure 3. sessments of the severity of the harm to his or Based on this discussion, we extend the fol- her identity. When one party's revenge is an lowing propositions (shown in Figure 2): overreaction, it creates the groundwork for a fresh attack on identity, and it grants victim Proposition 4: Perception of a dam- status to the other party, resulting in both par- aged social identity by the target in a ties believing they are victims (Kim & Smith, social interaction will increase the 1993; Youngs, 1986). One party's nonproportional probability of the escalation of an in- revenge may provoke the other party's nonpro- civility spiral. portional counterrevenge, causing an escalat- Proposition 5: Feelings of anger by the ing spiral of revenge and counterrevenge (Bies target in a social interaction will in- & Tripp, 1995; Kim & Smith, 1993; Youngs, 1986). crease the probability of the escala- At the tipping point in the sample incivility tion of an incivility spiral. spiral shown in Figure 3, the incivility per- formed by Party A may or may not be intended Proposition 6: Desire for revenge by as the first step in an aggressive attack, yet it is the target in a social interaction will perceived as a coercive act by Party B (Baumeis- increase the probability of the escala- ter et al., 1994). Party B may cognitively interpret tion of an incivility spiral. the situation as a loss of face (Felson, 1982; Ma- such, 1985; Tedeschi & Felson, 1994), which then Facilitators of the may result in the affective state of anger. To- Spiral gether, loss of face and anger can stimulate a The social interactionist perspective we take desire for revenge, potentially of a magnitude emphasizes that the interaction between two nonproportional to the act performed by Party A parties is not the only factor that comes into play (Helm et al., 1972; Youngs, 1986). Party B may in an incivility spiral. Prominent researchers then respond with the first coercive act, perhaps viewing aggression as a social interaction (e.g., a maligning insult, thereby eliciting a similar Bandura, 1973; Baron & Richardson, 1994; cognitive, affective, and behavioral response se- Berkowitz, 1993; Tedeschi & Felson, 1994) have quence from Party A that seems to justify an found that certain characteristics of the individ- even more coercive subsequent move by Party B uals involved in the interaction, as well as cer- (Bies & Tripp, 1995; Kim & Smith, 1993; Youngs, tain features of the social context, can inhibit or 1986). In this manner the spiral escalates. facilitate an aggressive exchange. These re- As implied earlier, reaching the tipping point searchers have studied and proposed a wide is not inevitable. At various points during the array of variables-from physical features and exchange of incivilities, either party can depart personality traits of individuals to the social

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 464 Academy of Management Review July network and physical environment in which the (Berkowitz, 1993; Buss, 1961). Furthermore, indi- aggressive exchange takes place. In summariz- viduals who are rebellious-those who value ing and borrowing from this research, we focus independence, desire self-sufficiency, and resist on two facilitators: (1) the "hot" temperament of group pressures-are more likely to use uncivil an involved party and (2) a workplace climate of or coercive behaviors. Because of experienced informality. We believe these facilitators are societal pressures for conformity and submis- particularly important in determining whether sion, rebellious individuals perceive more irri- an exchange of incivilities will occur and tants in their everyday interactions, thereby in- whether it will escalate into an exchange of creasing the likelihood of an uncivil or coercive coercive actions in today's organizational set- reaction (Buss, 1961). ting. Individuals who are impulsive, emotionally The "hot" temperament. Temperament refers reactive, and rebellious-those who fit the pro- to characteristics of behavior that, through life, file of the hot temperament-tend to handle remain relatively unchanged. One's tempera- stress by reacting discourteously or aggres- ment conveys the way in which one tends to sively (Baumeister et al., 1994; Buss, 1961). In respond to life situations. Classic aggression today's stressful workplace, which is full of cir- researchers (e.g., Berkowitz, 1993; Buss, 1961; cumstances (e.g., diversity, reengineering, Lorenz, 1966) long have recognized several di- downsizing, or temporary work) conducive to mensions of temperament that help to deter- role ambiguity, , situational con- mine the likelihood an individual will respond straints, and heavy , the stimuli for ig- with some form of aggressive behavior when niting those with a hot temperament are numer- provoked or subjected to stressful situations. ous (Carter, 1998; Chen & Spector, 1992; Neuman Likewise, consultants and security experts ana- & Baron, 1997). Moreover, individuals with this lyzing incidents of workplace aggression have hot temperament are more likely to abuse alco- suggested that individuals who perform aggres- hol and drugs while at work, causing them to sive or violent acts at work tend to fit a particu- lose yet more inhibition and to become even lar temperamental profile (e.g., Allcorn, 1994; more likely to use uncivil and coercive behav- Kinney, 1995; Segal, 1994). Self-regulatory capac- iors (Baumeister et al., 1994; Tedeschi & Felson, ity, emotional reactivity, and rebelliousness are 1994). Thus, we propose (as portrayed in Figure three key dimensions of temperament that, to- 2) that if at least one of the parties involved in a gether, can be used to help determine whether social interaction can be characterized by this or not an individual will be likely to use uncivil hot temperament, it is more likely that an inci- and coercive behaviors. vility spiral will occur and/or escalate into an Individuals who are impulsive-those with a exchange of coercive actions: weak capacity to self-regulate their behavior- Proposition 7: The probability of are more likely to use verbal slurs and coercive the occurrence andlor escalation of in- actions than individuals with a strong capacity an civility spiral is to self-regulate (Baumeister et al., 1994; Hynan & enhanced if one or more parties in a social Grush, 1986). Impulsives have less inhibition interaction has a hot temperament. than those with stronger self-regulatory capac- ity to prevent them from acting out their current Climate of informality. Organizational cli- emotional state and intentions (Baumeister et mate refers to the observable practices and pro- al., 1994). Likewise, individuals who are emo- cedures that compose the surface of organiza- tionally reactive-highly sensitive to insults, tional life (Schneider & Reichers, 1983). In easily offended, and who perceive threats in attempting to stimulate creativity and innova- seemingly innocent exchanges-are more likely tion, level status, and promote free-flowing com- to experience feelings of interactional injustice, munication, many organizations today have as- loss of face, and negative emotions, increasing sembled a set of practices and procedures that the likelihood that they will commit uncivil or create a casual and informal workplace-a "cli- coercive acts (Berkowitz, 1993; Buss, 1961; Tede- mate of informality" (Martin, 1996; Morand, 1998). schi & Felson, 1994). These individuals have a The degree of formality/informality in an organ- more intense reaction in response to stimuli that ization can be expressed through dress, word would elicit only mild agitation in most people choice, conversational patterns, postural and

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1999 Andersson and Pearson 465

nonverbal cues, emotional expression, and portrayed in Figure 2) that an incivility spiral modes of decor and architecture (Morand, 1998). will be more likely to occur and/or escalate into For example, one could describe an organiza- an exchange of coercive actions in an organiza- tion in which members wear formal business tion with a climate of informality: attire, address superiors by title, use deliberate Proposition 8: The probability of the and regulated speech patterns, exhibit emo- occurrence and/or escalation of an in- tional restraint, and reside in neat with civility spiral is enhanced if the social businesslike decor as having a formal climate. interaction takes place in an organi- In contrast, one could describe an organization zation that has an informal climate. in which members wear shorts and T-shirts, ad- dress one another by nicknames, engage in lively banter about their personal lives, freely Secondary Spirals express their emotions, and reside in comfort- Norms for civil behavior in an organization able and personally decorated offices as having often become eroded as organizational mem- an informal organizational climate. bers experience or witness incivility spirals Despite the atmosphere of open communica- (Carter, 1998). Frequently, an incivility spiral be- tion and innovation it can foster, a climate of tween two parties spawns secondary incivility informality may inadvertently encourage em- spirals, ultimately spreading incivility through- ployees to behave in ways that are disrespectful out the organization (Masuch, 1985). The incivil- of fellow coworkers (Morand, 1998). When the ity spiral perpetuated by Parties A and B, for organizational climate is more formal, there is example, may be observed by Party C, who, in little ambiguity regarding what is acceptable turn, may model that behavior and initiate an interpersonal behavior, and employees follow incivility spiral with Party D. Or Party A may unspoken rules of politeness and professional- misplace her desire to reciprocate, directing an ism in their relationships with one another incivility at Party C rather than at Party B. These (Elias, 1982; Martin, 1996; Morand, 1998). More- patterns may fuel further incivility, spreading over, when employees have to pay attention to the phenomenon throughout the organization mode of dress, enunciation, and conversational and enabling incivility to become the new or- cues, they are forced to pause and think before ganizational norm. they act. Without the trappings of formality to Secondary spirals are not only spawned by routinize interactions and control for deviations, experienced and witnessed incivilities but also employees may have trouble maintaining their by the general negative response to incivility professional distance and objectivity (Goffman, and coercive acts within the organization. Em- 1967; Morand, 1998). In an informal setting it is ployees become aware of the mounting incivil- more difficult for employees to discern accept- ity, and their response can be increasing levels able behavior from unacceptable, both in others of negative affect, distrust, and fear (Carter, and in themselves, thereby creating greater po- 1998; MacKinnon, 1994). The loss of civility tential for misinterpretation and subsequent changes employees' expectations of one an- deviant behavior (Elias, 1982; Erickson, 1962; other. The irritable, intolerant, or fearful em- Robinson & Bennett, 1997; Wouters, 1990). Fur- ployee is then unwilling to extend the minimal thermore, manifestations of informality, such as courtesies and the tolerance of others that con- casual dress and nicknames, can signify that stitute day-to-day civility, thus creating further employees do not have to "be on their best be- spirals of incivility (LaGrange et al., 1992; havior"-that they can "let down their guard" MacKinnon, 1994). while at work (Martin, 1996; Morand, 1998; We offer the following propositions (shown in Rafaeli & Pratt, 1993). Figure 2): Therefore, in a climate of informality, employ- ees may be more likely to engage in uncivil Proposition 9: Observation of an inci- behaviors. Likewise, researchers have sug- vility spiral by other members of the gested that an informal climate may contribute organization will increase the proba- to escalation to more intense deviant behaviors, bility of a secondary incivility spiral. such as coercive actions (Berkowitz, 1993; Tede- Proposition 10: Observation of nega- schi & Felson, 1994). Therefore, we predict (as tive response (negative affect, distrust,

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 466 Academy of Management Review July

and fear) to uncivil behavior within volved parties with a hot temperament and an the organization will increase the organizational climate of informality may facil- probability of a secondary incivility itate the formation and escalation of such spiral. spirals and that these spirals may spawn sec- ondary spirals, which can permeate an organi- Can so many spirals form that an organiza- zation. Potential relationships between incivil- tion actually becomes an "uncivil organiza- ity and more intense forms of mistreatment, tion?" Again using the analogy of the tipping captured in the present spiral framework, point, this time at the organizational level, we should be of great interest to researchers study- hold that an organization may become "uncivil" ing aggressive behavior in organizations, as once the number of incivility spirals reaches a well as to managers determined to prevent critical threshold. At that critical threshold, "ex- workplace aggression and violence. We now plosive clusters" of incivility may occur, discuss some of the specific research and prac- whereby dozens of incivility spirals are trig- tical implications of this important workplace gered simultaneously, each feeding off the other behavior. (Masuch, 1985). This critical threshold may be reached when employees, involved potentially in multiple spirals at once, perceive loss of iden- Research Implications tity as members of the organization and feel that In alignment with recent research on aggres- the organization is coercive or "out to get them." sion in the workplace (e.g., Folger et al., 1998; When a majority of employees believe that the O'Leary-Kelly et al., 1996; Robinson & O'Leary- organization intends to harm or discount them, Kelly, 1996), our perspective views incivility and the organization itself may become an uncivil coercive actions as stemming from social inter- entity (Kamp & Brooks, 1991). actions. Much of the recent conceptual research on workplace aggression (e.g., Neuman & Baron, 1997; CONCLUSIONAND IMPLICATIONS O'Leary-Kelly et al., 1996), however, focuses on classifying types of aggressive behavior in The overwhelming majority of acts of mis- organizations, rather than on detailing the pro- treatment in organizations are more subtle than cess of how aggression evolves. Moreover, in those involving physical violence (Neuman & much of the recent research on workplace ag- Baron, 1997). Researchers studying deviant and gression, researchers have attempted to model aggressive behavior in the workplace (e.g., Neu- aggression as a single act in time-not as a man & Baron, 1997; Robinson & Bennett, 1995, process systemically linked with social interac- 1997) have given the rude comments, thought- tions occurring prior to the aggressive act (e.g., less acts, insinuating glances, and negative Baron & Neuman, 1996). Our perspective is gestures that transpire within organizations lit- unique in that it not only defines a behavior that tle attention. Here we have introduced a new may be a precursor to aggression but also pro- concept-workplace incivility-to account for poses that the various forms of mistreatment in these lesser forms of mistreatment in organiza- organizations are related, as part of one system. tions in which the intent to harm is ambiguous. The conceptualization of an incivility spiral as a Defined here as behavior characterized by rude- system is important in bridging the gap be- ness and disregard for others in the workplace, tween the behavior of individual participants in in violation of workplace norms for mutual the spiral and the behavior of the organization respect, workplace incivility represents one of as a whole. the less intense forms of deviant workplace The research propositions we offer in this ar- behavior. ticle await empirical testing. Tests of these We have argued that workplace incivility can propositions, however, will require that a valid spiral, beginning with one party's perception of and reliable measure of incivility be con- an incivility and reciprocation with a counterin- structed. We envision the process of develop- civility, which can potentially escalate to an ment of the incivility measure and testing of the exchange of coercive actions when one party propositions to be a multimethod endeavor, con- reaches a tipping point (i.e., perceives an iden- sisting of a combination of inductive and deduc- tity threat). Further, we have argued that in- tive methodologies. First, to grasp the complex-

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1999 Andersson and Pearson 467

ities and subtle ambiguities of workplace time, as the spiral progresses. Perhaps a sample incivility, we believe the use of interviews, field of employees could be asked to track uncivil observations, and small group discussion would incidents that they experience or witness while be beneficial. These techniques would allow ac- at work, maintaining a detailed diary as the cess to the nuances of experience that constitute incidents occur. an uncivil interaction, granting a depth of un- Beyond testing of the propositions, it is our derstanding of the construct and enabling the hope that this article will provoke further re- development of a "laundry list" of behaviors search into the construct of workplace incivility, that employees consider to be uncivil. Next, a its antecedents and consequences, and its facil- survey instrument focusing on critical incidents itators and inhibitors. We have not attempted to of incivility and more intense deviant workplace describe or model the possible antecedents of behaviors could be administered, preferably to incivility that may stem from the organizational several different samples that are known or environment, nor have we addressed how inci- likely to experience such behaviors. This would vility may affect certain organizational out- allow for a preliminary understanding of the comes. For example, the following questions are dimensions and boundaries of the construct. pertinent to incivility in today's workplace: How More rigorous survey research, in which the might the fast-paced and global nature of to- researcher analyzes the dimensions of incivility day's work environment spawn incivility? How espoused in this article (rude and discourteous might incivility affect client/network relation- behaviors, ambiguous intent to harm, and vio- ships and temporary workers? Further, we have lation of workplace norms for mutual respect), not predicted how the facilitators of incivility as well as other dimensions that emerge from and coercive actions (the hot temperament and research, would be warranted before a univer- a climate of informality) may interact, or how sal definition and measure of workplace incivil- other features of the organizational context may ity could be accepted. A key objective at this facilitate or inhibit incivility. It would be inter- stage of research would be to empirically dis- esting, for example, to examine whether an in- tinguish incivility from similar workplace be- dividual with a hot temperament would be more haviors. Laboratory and field experiments, often likely to "explode" in a very informal or a very useful in providing valid first tests of a new formal organizational climate. Finally, the spe- construct, probably would not be useful in the cific behaviors that make up workplace incivil- case of incivility, since it would be difficult to ity likely differ somewhat among cultures and, simulate, and possibly unethical to generate, possibly, even industries and organizations; the intensity of affect and behavior under con- thus, it would be fascinating to examine work- trived conditions. place norms across cultures and discover if Once the incivility measure is honed, one baseline norms for mutual respect in the work- could perform survey research that samples em- place are, indeed, universal. ployees from a diverse range of organizations and hierarchical levels in order to test the prop- Practical Implications ositions. To capture the possibility that incivility spirals occur over varying lengths of time, the There is a market in doing business consider- researchers should employ longitudinal meth- ately: people choose to do business with those ods. Alternatives to self-report measures should who grant them respect and make them feel also be developed, as employees may be hesi- good (Martin, 1996; Solomon, 1998). A certain tant to report on their own use of such a socially level of civility is fundamental to the operation undesirable behavior as incivility. To combat of any business. When civility is absent, work this potential bias, one could use content anal- relations can become frayed. An organizational yses of employee e-mail and telephone conver- climate characterized by rudeness can make sations, for example, to complement self-report workers miserable on the , resulting in ag- measures. Furthermore, one should consider the gressive behavior, higher , lower pro- varying perspectives of the multiple parties in- ductivity, and lost customers (Kamp & Brooks, volved in a uncivil interaction. Ideally, all of the 1991; Neuman & Baron, 1997). Thus, incivility not participants involved in a spiral could be sur- only makes the unpleasant but may neg- veyed and interviewed, at various points in atively impact a company's bottom line.

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 468 Academy of Management Review July

Incivility spirals, like other deviation-amplify- and that it will permeate the organization. In the ing spirals in organizations, can be dangerous: worst case an organization that condones rude- they can damage organizations, careers, and ness and aggressive behavior will attract others people (Masuch, 1985). The spiral framework who act similarly, potentially causing increased presented here provides managers with a start- turnover among the more considerate members ing point for assessing factors in their organiza- of the organization and increased anger among tions that may contribute to incivility and its others. escalation into aggressive behaviors. In addi- Those who instigate uncivil behavior must be tion, the examination of two of the individual held accountable, regardless of their hierarchi- and organizational contextual characteristics cal prestige or special talents. Setting policies that contribute to an incivility spiral is relevant and reinforcing norms that inhibit the occur- to managers, because it suggests the types of rence and escalation of uncivil interaction, such individuals who are more likely to commit un- as zero tolerance for shouting matches, could civil and aggressive acts, as well as the organ- reduce the likelihood of witnessed rudeness and izational policies that might inhibit an uncivil secondary incivility spirals. In addition, the pro- encounter. vision of such stress release options as corpo- To begin, managers might evaluate how their rate fitness centers, human resource hot lines, or own behaviors could contribute to a norm for conflict mediators might encourage employees incivility. Correcting subordinates by pounding to develop means of releasing pent-up emotions one's fist, swearing, or personally debasing without displacing them on other employees. them sets an uncivil tone. Similarly, interactions Incivility is of increasing concern in American between managers that are discourteous, result- society (Carter, 1998; Marks, 1996; Morris, 1996). ing in loss of face, negatively impact not only Incivility in the workplace seems to be spread- those in direct confrontation but also those who ing as the complexities of competition, technol- witness or hear about the incident. As noted, ogy, and globalization intermingle. Workplace these bystanders may then re-enact similar en- incivility, as a negative behavior with moral counters with their own subordinates, peers, or implications and as a potential precursor to in- customers. creasingly aggressive acts, deserves more If curtailing incivility and aggression is im- scholarly attention. The ways in which incivility portant to an organization, the organization affects organizational productivity and should attempt to recruit and hire people whose employee well-being have yet to be tested. characteristics may be expected to facilitate po- Organizations have much to gain by under- lite, courteous interaction. Practical means of standing the factors that disrupt mutual respect achieving this outcome include (1) conducting and prompt aggression; likewise, organizations multiple interviews of applicants by a wide rep- have much to lose when uncivil, tit-for-tat inter- resentation of future associates and then listen- actions occur and escalate. ing to and acting on the feedback from those interviewers; (2) building internship programs during which prospective permanent hires can REFERENCES gain realistic job/culture previews while the po- Allcorn, S. 1994. Anger in the workplace: Understanding tential employer and coworkers gain a more ac- the causes of aggression and violence. Westport, CT: curate sense of prospects' fit; and (3) scrupu- Quorum Books. lously following through on reference checks, Anderson, C. A., Deuser, W. E., & DeNeve, K. M. 1995. Hot including contacts from applicants' more distant temperatures, hostile affect, hostile cognition, and past, from which there are no immediate pres- arousal: Tests of a general model of affective aggres- sion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21: sures on referents to "help" the employee move 434-448. on (Neuman & Baron, 1997). Ashforth, B. E. 1994. Petty tyranny in organizations. Human Finally, organizations that wish to curtail in- Relations, 47: 755-778. civility must address acts of interpersonal rude- Averill, J. R. 1983. Studies on anger and aggression: ness swiftly and justly. To do otherwise corrodes Implica- tions for theories of emotion. American Psychologist, 38: expectations and norms for the organization at 1145-1160. large. Condoning nasty interaction increases Bandura, A. 1973. Aggression: A social learning analysis. the possibility that it will become more intense Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1999 Andersson and Pearson 469

Baron, R. A., & Neuman, J. H. 1996. Workplace violence and workplace. Baltimore: Center for the Applied Study of workplace aggression: Evidence on their relative fre- Ethnoviolence. quency and potential causes. Aggressive Behavior, 22: Elias, N. 1982. The history of manners. New York: Pantheon. 161-173. Erez, M., & Earley, P. 1993. Culture, self-identity, and work. Baron, R. A., & Richardson, D. R. 1994. Human aggression. New York: Oxford University Press. New York: Plenum Press. Erickson, K. 1962. Notes on the sociology of deviance. Social Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. 1994. Losing Problems, 9: 307-314. control: How and why people fail at self-regulation. San Diego: Academic Press. Feldman, D. C. 1984. The development and enforcement of Bellah, R. 1970. Beyond belief: Essays on in a post- group norms. Academy of Management Review, 9: 47-53. traditional world. New York: Harper & Row. Felson, R. B. 1982. Impression management and the escala- Berkowitz, L. 1993. Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and tion of aggression and violence. Social Psychology control. New York: McGraw-Hill. Quarterly, 45: 245-254. Bies, R. J. In press. Interactional (in)justice: The sacred and Felson, R. B., & Steadman, H. J. 1983. Situational factors in the profane. In J. Greenberg & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), disputes leading to criminal violence. Criminology, 21: Advances in organizational behavior. San Francisco: 59-74. New Lexington Press. Folger, R., Robinson, S. L., Dietz, J., McLean Parks, J., & Baron, Bies, R. J., & Moag, J. S. 1986. Interactional justice: Commu- R. A. 1998. When colleagues become violent: Employee nications criteria of fairness. In R. Lewicki & B. Sheppard threats and assaults as a function of societal violence (Eds.), Research on negotiation in organizations, vol. 1: and organizational injustice. Proceedings of the Acad- 43-55. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. emy of Management: Al-A7. Bies, R. J., & Tripp, T. M. 1995. Beyond distrust: "Getting even" Giacalone, R. A., & Greenberg, J. (Eds.). 1997. Antisocial be- and the need for revenge. In R. M. Kramer & T. Tyler havior in organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. (Eds.), Trust in organizations: 246-260. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Gladwell, M. 1996. The tipping point. The New Yorker, 72(14): 32-36. Bies, R. J., Tripp, T. M., & Kramer, R. M. 1997. At the breaking point: Cognitive and social dynamics of revenge in or- Goffman, E. 1967. Interaction ritual. New York: Pantheon. ganizations. In R. A. Giacalone & J. Greenberg (Eds.), Goldstein, A. P. 1994. The ecology of aggression. New York: Antisocial behavior in organizations: 18-36. Thousand Plenum. Oaks, CA: Sage. Gordon, S. L. 1989. Institutional and impulsive orientations Bjorkqvist, K., Osterman, K., & Hjelt-Back, M. 1994. Aggres- in selectively appropriating emotions to self. In D. D. sion among university employees. Aggressive Behavior, Franks & E. D. McCarthy (Eds.), The sociology of emo- 20: 173-184. tions: 115-135. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Brief, A. P., & Motowidlo, S. 1986. Prosocial J. organizational Gouldner, A. W. 1960. The norm of reciprocity: A preliminary behaviors. Academy of Management Review, 11: 710- statement. American Sociological Review, 25: 161-178. 725. Graydon, J., Kasta, W., & Khan, P. 1994. Verbal and physical Brown, D. W. 1978. Arrest rates and crime rates: When does a abuse of nurses. Canadian Journal of Nursing Adminis- tipping effect occur? Social Forces, 57: 671-682. tration, November-December: 70-89. Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. 1987. Politeness: Some universals Hambrick, D. C., & D'Aveni, R. A. 1988. Large corporate fail- in language usage. New York: Cambridge University Press. ures as downward spirals. Administrative Science Quarterly, 33: 1-23. Buss, A. H. 1961. The psychology of aggression. New York: Wiley. Hamilton, K., & Sullivan, S. 1997. Netiquette: A guide to man- ners in the new age. Newsweek, September 1: 14. Carter, S. L. 1998. Civility: Manners, morals, and the etiquette of . New York: Basic Books. Hartman, E. 1996. Organizational ethics. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Chen, C. C., & Eastman, W. 1997. Toward a civic culture for multicultural organizations. Journal of Applied Behav- Helm, B., Bonoma, T. V., & Tedeschi, J. T. 1972. Reciprocity for ioral Science, 33: 454-470. harm done. Journal of Social Psychology, 87: 89-98. Chen, P. Y., & Spector, P. E. 1992. Relationships of work Hynan, D. J., & Grush, J. E. 1986. Effects of impulsivity, de- stressors with aggression, withdrawal, theft, and sub- pression, provocation, and time on aggressive behavior. stance abuse: An exploratory study. Journal of Occupa- Journal of Research in Personality, 20: 158-171. tional and Organizational Psychology, 65: 177-184. Johnson, C. L. 1988. Socially controlled civility. American Donnerstein, E., & Hatfield, E. 1982. Aggression and inequity. Behavioral Scientist, 31: 685-701. In J. Greenberg & R. Cohen (Eds.), Equity and justice in Kamp, J., & Brooks, P. 1991. Perceived organizational climate social behavior: 309-336. San Diego: Academic Press. and employee counterproductivity. Journal of Business & Ehrlich, H. J., & Larcom, B. E. K. 1994. Ethnoviolence in the Psychology, 5: 447-458.

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 470 Academy of Management Review July

Kim, S. H., & Smith, R. H. 1993. Revenge and conflict escala- work value measures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72: tion. Negotiation Journal, 9: 37-43. 666-673. Kinney, J. A. 1995. Violence at work. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Roberts, M. C. 1985. A plea for professional civility. Profes- Prentice-Hall. sional Psychology: Research and Practice, 16: 474. Kramer, R. M. 1994. The sinister attribution error. Motivation Robinson, S. L., & Bennett, R. J. 1995. A typology of deviant and Emotion, 18: 199-231. workplace behaviors: A multidimensional scaling study. Academy of Management Journal, 38: 555-572. LaGrange, R. L., Ferraro, K. F., & Supancic, M. 1992. Perceived risk and fear of crime: Role of social and physical inci- Robinson, S. L., & Bennett, R. J. 1997. : Its vilities. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, definition, its manifestations, and its causes. In 29: 311-334. R. Lewicki, B. Sheppard, & R. Bies (Eds.), Research on negotiation in organizations, vol. 6: 3-27. Greenwich, Lindsley, D. H., Brass, D. J., & Thomas, J. B. 1995. Efficacy- CT: JAI Press. performance spirals: A multilevel perspective. Academy of Management Review, 20: 645-678. Robinson, S. L., & O'Leary-Kelly, A. 1996. Monkey see, mon- key do: The role of role models in predicting workplace Lorenz, K. 1966. On aggression. New York: Harcourt Brace & aggression. Proceedings of the of World. Academy Manage- ment: 288-292. Luckenbill, D. F. 1977. Criminal homicide as a situated trans- Romano, C. 1994. Workplace violence takes a deadly turn. action. Social Problems, 25: 176-186. Management Review, 83(7): 5. MacKinnon, C. 1994. Only words. New York: Basic Books. Sapir, E. 1927. The unconscious patterning of behavior in Marks, J. 1996. The American uncivil wars. U.S. News & World society. In D. Mandelbaum (Ed.), Selected writings of Report, April 22: 66-72. Edward Sapir: 540-559. Berkeley, CA: University of Cal- Martin, J. 1996. Miss Manners rescues civilization. New York: ifornia Press. Crown Publishers. Schlenker, B. R. 1980. Impression management: The self- Masuch, M. 1985. Vicious circles in organizations. Adminis- concept, social identity, and interpersonal relations. trative Science Quarterly, 30: 14-33. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing. Morand, D. A. 1998. Getting serious about going casual on Schneider, B., & Reichers, A. E. 1983. On the etiology of the job. Business Horizons, 41: 51-56. climates. Personnel Psychology, 36: 19-39. Morrill, C. 1992. Vengeance among executives. Virginia Re- Segal, J. A. 1994. When Charles Manson comes to the work- view of Sociology, 1: 51-76. place. HRMagazine, 39(6): 33-40. Shriver, D. Morris, J. 1996. Democracy beguiled. The Wilson Quarterly, W., Jr. 1995. An ethic for enemies: Forgiveness in Autumn: 24-35. politics. New York: Oxford University Press. Neuman, J. H., & Baron, R. A. 1997. Aggression in the work- Shulman, L. S., & Carey, N. B. 1984. Psychology and the place. In R. A. Giacalone & J. Greenberg (Eds.), Antiso- limitations of individual rationality: Implications for the study of cial behavior in organizations: 37-67. Thousand Oaks, reasoning and civility. Review of Educational CA: Sage. Research, 54: 501-524. Skarlicki, D. Northwestern National Life Insurance Company. 1993. Fear P., & Folger, R. 1997. Retaliation in the work- place: The and violence in the workplace. Research report, Minne- roles of distributive, procedural, and interac- apolis, MN. tional justice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82: 434- 443. O'Leary-Kelly, A. M., Griffin, R. W., & Glew, D. J. 1996. Solomon, R. C. 1998. The moral Organization-motivated aggression: A research frame- psychology of business: Care and compassion in the work. Academy of Management Review, 21: 225-254. . Business Ethics Quarterly, 8: 515-533. Organ, D. W. 1988. Organizational citizenship behavior: The Spratlen, L. P. 1994. Workplace good soldier syndrome. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. mistreatment: Its relationship to interpersonal violence. Journal of Psychosocial Nurs- Patterson, G. R. 1982. Coercive family processes. Eugene, OR: ing, 32(12): 5- 6. Castalia. Steinberg, L. 1996. A simple lack of manners imperils civi- Perlow, R., & Latham, L. L. 1993. Relationships of client abuse lized society. The Plain Dealer, January 14: 13. with locus of control and gender: A longitudinal study. Taylor, R. B., & Gottfredson, S. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78: 831-834. 1986. Environmental design, crime, and prevention: An examination of community Pruitt, G., & Rubin, J. Z. 1986. Social conflict: Escalation, dynamics. In A. J. Reiss & M. Tonry (Eds.), Communities stalemate, and settlement. New York: Random House. and crime. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Rafaeli, A., & Pratt, M. G. 1993. Tailored meanings: On the Tedeschi, J. T., & Felson, R. B. 1994. Violence, aggression, & meaning and impact of organizational dress. Academy coercive actions. Washington, DC: American Psycholog- of Management Review, 18: 32-55. ical Association. Ravlin, E. C., & Meglino, B. M. 1987. Effects of values on Tjosvold, D. 1983. Social face in conflict: A critique. Interna- perception and decision making: A study of alternative tional Journal of Group Tensions, 13: 49-64.

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1999 Andersson and Pearson 471

VandenBos, G. R., & Bulatao, E. Q. (Eds.) 1996. Violence on the Wouters, C. 1990. Social stratification and informalization in job: Identifying risks and developing solutions. Wash- global perspective. Theory, Culture & Society, 7: 69-90. ington, DC: American Psychological Association. Youngs, G. A., Jr. 1986. Patterns of threat and punishment Van Dyne, L., Cummings, L. L., & McLean Parks, J. 1995. reciprocity in a conflict setting. Journal of Personality & Extra-role behaviors: In pursuit of construct and defini- Social Psychology, 51: 541-546. tional clarity (a bridge over muddied waters). In B. M. Yukl, G., & Falbe, C. M. 1990. Influence tactics in upward, Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organization- downward, and lateral influence attempts. Journal of al behavior, vol. 17: 215-285. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Applied Psychology, 75: 132-140. Weisinger, H. 1995. Anger at work. New York: William Mor- row. Zimbardo, P. G. 1969. The human choice: Individuation, reason, and order versus deindividuation, impulse, Wilson, J. Q. 1993. The moral sense. New York: Free Press. and chaos. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on Wilson, J. Q., & Kelling, G. L. 1982. Broken windows: The motivation, 237-307. Lincoln: University of Nebraska police and neighborhood safety. The Atlantic, 249: 29-38. Press.

Lynne M. Andersson is an assistant professor of management at St. Joseph's Univer- sity. She received her Ph.D. from the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. Her research interests focus on individual attitudes and behaviors in the workplace, particularly those of a deviant nature and related to pressing business-society issues. Christine M. Pearson is an assistant professor of management at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. She received her Ph.D. from the University of Southern California. Her research interests involve deterring and managing the dark side of organizational life.

This content downloaded from 129.252.86.130 on Mon, 6 Oct 2014 11:55:25 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions