Is There Reason in the Rhyme?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Is There Reason in the Rhyme?

Is There Reason in the Rhyme? Is Rhyme an Effective Tool for Creating Accessible and Engaging Fiction Books for ESOL A0 Level Adult Learners? by

Louise Shepperd

A81006883 Word Count: 5,144

I confirm that this piece of work contains no plagiarised material and that I have read and understood the section on Plagiarism in the School Style Guide 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326

Is There Reason in the Rhyme?

Is Rhyme an Effective Tool for Creating Accessible and Engaging Fiction Books for ESOL A0 Level Adult Learners?

1. Introduction

In a previous study looking at developing fiction books for low-literate immigrant adults, a question arose about the role of rhyme (Shepperd 2011). When writing good fiction for low- literate language learners, a tension exists between the story’s content and linguistic accessibility (Shepperd 2011). It is important that these books are engaging and compelling for an adult audience, to encourage the idea of reading for pleasure. Studies show that the amount one reads influences second language proficiency (Krashen 1989), therefore when learning a second language (L2) it is also important to read outside of class (Rodrigo et al. 2007). A problem that arises in English for Speakers of Other Languages (ESOL) classrooms is that the language learnt in the lesson is often not practiced at home. However, writing better books for low-literate adults could encourage students to read outside of class and provide such practice (Young-Scholten & Maguire 2008).

Engaging content is important and, when it comes to vocabulary, one way to offer something different from word lists. However, such resources are needed for adult learners with low oral proficiency and little to no literacy (A0 level), who need any text to be highly linguistically accessible (Young-Scholten & Maguire 2008). This is where the tension starts to become apparent, as accessibility calls for simple morpho-syntax, vocabulary and phonology. To demonstrate the process, here is an example of text simplification from the fiction book written for this study:

She is awakened from her brief day dream by the ringing telephone.

2 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326

After simplification:

The phone rings. (See appendix 7)

When going through the process of text simplification many creative writing devices must be sacrificed for accessibility. How can quality fiction be written at an appropriate level of accessibility for A0 level learners?

Of prime consideration is phonological awareness, which is key in the acquisition of literacy (e.g. Geudens & Sandra 2002; Goswami & Bryant 1990; Verhoeven et al. 2002) and phonemic awareness is particularly important when using an alphabetic writing system (e.g. Cardoso-Martins 1995; Elbro & Pallesen 2002; Loureiro et al. 2004). A stumbling block for learners of the English writing system is its opaque orthography, which often displays one-to- many phoneme-grapheme correspondences instead of one-to-one correspondence (Yoon et al. 2002). For example, the word thought is made up of seven graphemes but only three phonemes, [Өɔːt], and when the letter t is removed from the written form the remaining word is not pronounced as [Өɔː] but completely changes [ðoʊ].

Phonemes, onsets and rimes are all subsyllabic phonological units, but unlike phonemes, awareness of onsets and rimes exists instinctively, that is, prior to any formal instruction (Kirtley et al. 1989; Loureiro et al. 2004). Alliteration and rhyme are the manipulation of onsets and rimes and are well known poetic devices, used for readers’ enjoyment language and engagement with stories. When it comes to working with English orthography, rhyme has also been shown to aid phonological development and literacy acquisition (Yoon et al. 2002). Can the use of rhyme in low literate fiction not only bridge the gap between pre- literate and literate phonological awareness but also ease the tension between quality of content and accessibility?

3 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326

1.1 Aim

This essay has three aims, firstly, to find out how aware A0 learners are of rimes in written English. Secondly, to find out whether rhyme helps them when decoding English orthography. Thirdly, to find out whether rhyme is an effective device in ESOL fiction by encouraging phonological awareness as well as reading for pleasure.

2. Low literate adults’ reading development

2.1 Importance of phonological awareness in literacy acquisition

Phonological awareness refers to the “awareness of the sounds that make up the words we hear and speak” (Cardoso-Martins 1995: 1) and this includes phonemic awareness, which is the ability to identify individual phonemes in a word (Verhoeven et al. 2002). There is a strong relationship between phonological awareness and literacy acquisition, with many studies supporting the idea that level of phonological awareness directly impacts reading ability (Cardoso-Martins 1995; Elbro & Pallesen 2002; Geudens & Sandra 2002; Goswami & Bryant 1990; Verhoeven et al. 2002).

Phonemic awareness is key in an alphabetic writing system because alphabetic letters represent phonemes (Geudens & Sandra 2002). Phonemes are not always represented perfectly, particularly in English (see above), but this sensitivity enables reading by grapheme-phoneme correspondences (Cardoso-Martins 1995). Elbro & Pallesen (2002:19) also show that any “experience with written language boosts the development of phonological awareness.” Along with experience, explicit instruction in phonemic awareness plays an in important role in successful literacy acquisition (Cardoso-Martins 1995). Loureiro et al. (2004) study of pre-school children and illiterate adults shows both groups of participants improve dramatically in phonemic awareness with instruction. This shows that phonemic awareness does not arise spontaneously, unlike rime and onset awareness.

2.2 Issues surrounding LESSLA1 and English orthography

Writing systems can generally be divided into pictographic/logographic (e.g. Chinese), syllabic (e.g. Japanese) and alphabetic (e.g. most European languages). A widely held view is that different writing systems vary in how difficult they are to learn, but Verhoeven et al. (2002:4) argue that “…all systems represent spoken language at one level or another and that

1 Low Educated Second Language and Literacy Acquisition

4 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326 readers activate speech codes during the decoding process.” For example, even though alphabetic systems have a small number of symbols to learn, when compared to the thousands of symbols in logographic and syllabic systems, they are still difficult as phonemes are hard to perceive (Verhoeven et al. 2002).

Alphabetic writing systems can then be divided according to the depth or transparency of their orthography. An orthography where one phoneme maps onto one grapheme is orthographically transparent or shallow (e.g. Italian, Finnish) (Elbro & Pallesen 2002). An orthography where one phoneme maps onto many graphemes, or vice versa, is orthographically opaque or deep (e.g. English). The Orthographic Depth Hypothesis (Katz & Frost 1992) argues that words with a more shallow orthography encourage phonological processing, whereas this is more difficult in deep orthographies, which encourage visual- orthographic processing.

However, even though English does not have one-to-one mappings for phonemes and graphemes, the system is not completely irregular and there are other ways to phonologically overcome phoneme-grapheme inconsistencies (Pacton et al. 2002). Yoon et al. (2002) suggest that because English orthography lacks transparency, the use of phonological units larger than phonemes is encouraged in reading, for example rime units. Treiman et al. (1995) also emphasize that fact that orthography and phonology are most consistent at the level of the orthographic rime, making this useful for decoding. Sensitivity to linguistic structure is therefore essential in mastering English orthography, due to its opaque nature (Burt & Butterworth 1996).

2.3 Qualities of rhyme in literacy acquisition

As noted above, awareness of rimes (e.g. -at in hat) and onsets (e.g. h- in hat) appears to be instinctive in both children and adults prior to literacy instruction (Young-Scholten & Strom 2006). Yoon et al.’s (2002: 159) study of subsyllabic awareness in the reading of English and Korean (alphabetic system) children found that “children at the early stages of reading instruction come equipped with phonological knowledge of onsets and rimes onto which orthographic recognition units are then mapped.” They also found onset-rime structures are particularly important in providing a structure that is accessible to children before they can access phonemes. This is supported by Kirtley et al.’s (1989) study of preschool children,

5 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326 which showed that children naturally divided syllables into onset and rimes and were only able to isolate single phonemes when they coincided with the onset.

Not only do pre-literate children display a high awareness of rhymes, but so do illiterate adults. Loureiro et al. (2004) studied 97 illiterate and semi-literate Brazilians and found that many participants did well in rhyme identification tasks. This demonstrated that awareness of rhyme was unrelated to alphabetical knowledge and word reading score. They went on to suggest that rhyme identification is a distinct cognitive process from phonemic awareness, which is dependent on alphabetical knowledge (e.g. Morais et al. 1979). “Rhyming is often seen as a first ‘natural’ step into awareness of sub-lexical units” (Elbro & Pallesen 2002: 22) and can ease phonemic development and thus reading development.

English readers in particular parse written language at the onset-rime boundary, using onsets and rimes as orthographic recognition units (Geudens & Sandra 2002). This is likely to be because “when rime units, rather than grapheme-phoneme pairing are the units of analysis, English turns out to be highly reliable” (Yoon et al 2002: 142). However, Burt & Blackwell (2008) point out there are inconsistencies in the pronunciation of rimes, which makes word naming very difficult (e.g. ear in near vs. bear). In spite of this, consistency of the rime is still important in reading patterns of English orthography.

2.4 Qualities of rhyme in reading for pleasure and language play

Rhyme is commonly found in children’s books because it provides the opportunity to ‘play’ with language (Brock & Rankin 2008) and also can provide predictable features that help beginners make associations between spoken and written words (Linse 2007). Alongside the benefits of rhyme in developing phonological awareness, it also has creative writing qualities. Rhyme is widely recognised as a poetic device and has played an important role in adult literature and oral tradition for centuries (McKie 1997). As a poetic device, it is used to engage its audience, make a text or verse more memorable and show versatility of language use (McKie 1997). The use of rhyme by adults is highlighted by Elbro & Pallesen (2002), who note that rhyme is used to a high level by illiterate Brazilian street poets. So, we find rhyme used and enjoyed by children and adults, in written and spoken language, and also in aid of literacy acquisition.

6 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326

Reading for a reason is an important concept in encouraging reading for pleasure and general reading development; therefore high importance needs to be placed on good literature (Sedgwick 2010).

Literacy is not something that you can teach apart from literate behaviour. You don’t learn to read; you learn to read something, and you read something because you want to know something, enjoy a text or participate in a group. (Delpit, 1991: 542)

Hill (2001) explains that stories written in language that learners can understand can be a source of both pleasure and learning. He tells us that stories can “radically improve learners’ attitudes because they find they have an immediate and enjoyable use for the language they are learning” (Hill 2001: 303). They also help develop reading fluency; providing a context for language and extending grasp of lexis and syntax (see also Krashen 1989).

Together with reading for pleasure, there should also be space for language play in the language learning environment. Again, language play is not just relevant for children, as adult literature, poetry and satire demonstrate. In discussing L2 teaching, Cook (1997: 224) points out that “...a good deal of authentic or natural language is playful, in the sense of being focused upon form and fiction rather than on meaning and reality.” Language teaching aims for students to have an authentic grasp of a language and its intricacies, and this is something language play can facilitate. This is not in exchange for emphasis on communication and meaning, but to go alongside it and create an environment sensitive to the complexities of learning a second language (Cook 1997; Linse 2007).

To summarise, phonological awareness is an important element of acquiring literacy and phonemic awareness is particularly necessary for using an alphabetic writing system. A problem posed for ESOL A0 level learners acquiring literacy in English is the opaque nature of English orthography and its frequent one-to-many phoneme-grapheme correspondences.

7 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326

Unlike phonemes, learners are aware of rhyme prior to formal instruction and English orthography is more reliable at the level of rimes. Another problem posed for A0 learners is lack of sufficient engaging fiction books to encourage reading for pleasure. Rhyme is not only a phonological unit, but also a poetic device that is effective in encouraging language play and creating a more engaging and enjoyable use of language.

3. Hypothesis

An explicit hypothesis regarding the outcome of this study was not formulated, as the amount of variables involved was too high. However, it was expected that participants would already have an awareness of rimes; deep orthography would prove more difficult to decode than shallow; and use of rhyme would aid phonological decoding and increase enjoyment of reading.

4. Methodology

4.1 Participant Background Information

Table 1 shows the biographic information for the 9 participants that took part in this study. They were all adult A0 level learners taking English classes at an ESOL centre, Action Language, in Newcastle upon Tyne. Six of the participants were female and three were male, and they were aged between 20 and 45. (Participants from a low-literate background would have been preferable but I did not anticipate how difficult it would be to find ESOL centres willing to co-operate at all, let alone ones with low-literate students.)

Table 1: Participant Background Information Name Country Mother Length of English OG Literacy in L1 of origin Tongue Time in UK Classes Stage 1 AG Portugal Creole 2 months Unknown 1 Unknown 2 MD Romania Romanian 3 months Unknown 1-2 Unknown 3 GA Turkey Turkish 9 months Unknown 1-2 Literate in L1 4 EB Bulgaria Bulgarian 11 months 22 hours 1 Literate in L1, Russian & Turkish 5 MB Gambia Fula 6 years Many 2-3 School until age 12 6 MS Iran Farsi 4 months 8 hours 1 University 7 XT China Mandarin 3 years 54 hours 2 University 8 SP Iran Farsi 5 months 16 hours 1-2 High school and

8 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326

College 9 MK Bulgaria Bulgarian 4 months 30 hours 1-2 University

Even though L1 literacy ranges considerably along with other variables, the data produced was insightful. Diversity, as seen above, is a common situation in most ESOL classrooms, so collecting data from such participants has ecological validity. As six out of nine are literate in their L1, factors such as influence from L1 writing system and more developed phonological and phonemic awareness have to be taken into account. Therefore, looking at the data from the perspective of rhyme aiding phonemic awareness for only low-literates was not possible, but looking at the use of rhyme in regards to acquiring literacy in English for A0 learners more generally is possible.

In Table 1 the term OG stage is used, which refers the morpho-syntactic developmental stages of Organic Grammar (OG) that second language learners pass through (Vainikka & Young-Scholten 2005; Young-Scholten & Ijuin 2006). These stages are offered as a more accurate alternative to standardised assessment of L2 learners. As well as using the tasks outlined below, I also had a brief conversation with each participant to collect data to assess their level of OG.

Table 2: Organic Grammar Criteria for stages in L2 English (Young-Scholten and Ijuin, 2006) Stage Word order in Verb type Verbal Pronouns Complex declaratives inflection syntax 1 Resembles L1, Main verbs None Absent None then L2 only 2 Resembles L2 Main verbs; Little Begin to Formulaic or copula is emerge intonation- appears based Qs 3 Resembles L2 Main verbs, No agreement; More forms, Conjoined modals; copula some tense but can still clauses; Qs forms beyond and aspect be missing formulaic or is forms w/o inversion 4 Resembles L2 Main verbs, Productive Obligatory; Simple modals, and tense, aspect; there and subordination; copula forms agreement w/ existential it Qs, but may be beyond is; be forms emerge uninverted range of auxiliaries 5 Resembles L2 Complex Extension to New uses Complex tense, aspect new forms there and it subordination; forms; Qs inverted passives

9 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326

4.2 Data Collection

Three tasks were conducted, corresponding with the three aims mentioned earlier, but also to observe the idea of triangulation of results. Each participant signed a consent form (see appendix1) and is referred to by either their initials or participant number throughout the essay, for ethical purposes. They then individually worked through each task, whilst I took notes their progress. On a couple of occasions a task proved too difficult for the participant, so we stopped and moved onto the next one.

The first task focused on assessing participants’ awareness of rhyme and on their responses to shallow and deep rhymes in written English. Each participant was asked to read out loud three words and then circle the word that did not rhyme. Most of the participants did not understand the word ‘rhyme’ and actually circle the word they felt was different or the odd one out. This began with an example that demonstrated the aim of the task, followed by fourteen more sets of words (six words with shallow orthography and eight with deep orthography) (see appendix 3).

The second task had two parts, which both looked at whether rhyme affects how A0 level students deal with words with a deep or shallow orthography. The first part was made up of seventeen short sentences, where twelve were rhyming couplets and five had rhyme adjacent pairs (9 sentences with shallow rhymes and 8 sentences with deep rhymes) (see appendix 4). The second part was made up of thirteen longer sentences, where eight were single sentences containing rhyming words and five were rhyming couplets (8 sentences with shallow rhymes and 5 with deep rhymes). Each participant read each sentence aloud whilst the interviewer made notes on frequency of pauses and fluency; need for help or a prompt; inability to read; use of phonemic decoding; and evidence of enjoying the rhyme (see appendix 2).

The third task investigated whether rhyme helps with phonological decoding and enjoyment of reading a fiction book. Each participant read aloud from the book Spotty Boys (see appendix 5), whilst the interviewer took note of problem areas, common mistakes, replacements, phonemic decoding, use of rhyme and evidence of enjoyment. The book was written and illustrated by me, and the process of creating the book will be described below. Another book was also written following the same storyline, but without any rhyme. I originally planned that participants would read both versions of the book so that results of using rhyme could be clearly compared and participants could say which version they

10 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326 enjoyed more. However, during the interviews it became clear that this was too much to ask of the participants.

4.3 Writing the Book

The main focus of this study was to look at the use of rhyme in ESOL fiction books aimed at A0 level adult learners. As mentioned above, rhyme is a poetic device that has been used for many centuries by many cultures to enjoy language and storytelling (McKie 1997). Furthermore, rhymes are phonological units that are more accessible than phonemes (Yoon et al. 2002). So, in theory rhyme could be helpful as an educational and creative device, and something that can be commonly appreciated in a multi-cultural classroom.

My previous essay (Shepperd 2011) gave a detailed description of the process of writing an adult fiction book accessible to A0 learners. The book Spotty Boys was based on the same principles but also took into account what was learnt through field-testing the previous book. This included keeping content as linguistically accessible as possible, choosing material that was relevant to everyday life and including a lot of repetition. Most pages contained two to three short sentences with a clear illustration and characters which were chosen purposefully to demonstrate racial and cultural diversity.

The story was written by following the stages learnt in previous creative writing workshops and then adding rhyme to the simplified version of the story (see appendix 7). The simplification involved reducing the prose to short simple sentences, with no embedded clauses except a few co-ordinating clauses. The story was written in 3rd person singular present so that proper nouns could be used instead of pronouns. Complex vocabulary was eliminated where possible and so were adverbs; two were kept only because they rhymed. Words and phrases that lacked semantic content, idioms and deixis were also avoided to maintain clarity and simplicity.

All remaining words were either mono or bi-syllabic, and orthographically opaque words were kept to a minimum. The original idea was to write the entire book in rhyming couplets but due to the limited vocabulary learners seem to know this proved more difficult than

11 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326 anticipated. Again, tension arises between content and accessibility. To maintain a logical storyline, rhyming couplets were used on eleven out of eighteen pages; nine of these rhymes had a deep orthography and two had a shallow orthography. Part of the motivation for the second task was to look at rhyming couplets and the decoding of words in a way that was not possible through the book. The aim was that the previous rhyme-based tasks and the rhyme throughout the book would help readers decode opaque words and learn more about the orthography and rhythm of English.

5. Results

5.1 Rhyme awareness task

The first task was not fully understood by many of the participants and some found the concept perplexing. They did not understand the purpose of a task that was concerned purely with phonological awareness separate from semantic content (similar issues found in Kurvers et al. 2006). Also many did the task without reading the words out loud, which may have made the process more difficult. Participant 6 did not participate in this task as it proved too difficult. The rest of the participants all attempted the task with varying levels of comprehension2.

Out of the other eight participants only one was able to correctly identify more than half of the non-rhyming words. The average total of correct answers was 42.5% and, on average, 38.8% of shallow rhymes and 44.4% of deep rhymes were correctly identified. The graph below shows which particular word sets were problematic and whether there was any distinction between rhymes with deep or shallow orthography.

Figure 1: Results from rhyme awareness task

Word set 8 (day, tree, key) was only answered correctly by one participant, with the majority circling tree as the odd-one-out (indicate which participants). The next highly problematic word set was 12 (hat, rat, hot), which was only correctly answered by two participants, with the majority circling rat as the odd-one-out, highlighting alliteration over rhyme. Word set 8

2 Young-Scholten and Strom (2006) gave their tasks in the learners’ L1 first but as I had eight different L1s, compared to their two L1s, this was not replicable.

12 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326 was orthographically opaque, whereas 12 was transparent, also, word lists 11,14 and 15 were all opaque but were answered correctly by over 60% of participants. Overall, depth of orthography did not make a difference. In many cases where rhymes were not correctly identified matching onsets or orthographically similar words were, which still showed a degree of phonological awareness.

5.2 Reading list of rhyming sentences

The second task proved more successful as all participants were familiar and comfortable with reading aloud short sentences. In the first half of the data set, involving shorter sentences, most sentences were read with a relatively high level of fluency and many participants developed a sense of rhythm as they went along. Sentences 5 (couplet: skirt, hurt) proved the most difficult with only four able to read it relatively fluently and over half needing prompting. Sentences 6 (couplet: best, test), 10 (adjacent: blue, shoe) and 17 (couplet: blue, shoe) proved easiest and were read fluently by all, except participant 7 who needed help with blue and shoe. Some even showed that they enjoyed a few of the rhymes, including: best and test; and sick chick.

In the second half of the data set, involving longer sentences and more complex vocabulary, most sentences were again read with a relatively high level of fluency and sense of rhythm. Sentences 11 (couplet: cough, off) were read fluently by all participants with only participant 6 sounding out phonemes and needing a prompt. In the shorter sentences cough was found difficult by over half (exact number) of the participant but read fluently in the longer sentences, suggesting modification from the previous set of sentences. Sentences 2 (adjacent: dirt, hurt) and 4 (adjacent: shows, clothes, froze) were found most difficult, both with one participant unable to read them correctly, more than half making mistakes and at least 25% needing a prompt and sounding out of phonemes. Sentence 7 (adjacent: call, fall, wall) was the next most problematic with only 50% reading fluently. The difficulty with opaque sentences 2 and 4 but then also transparent sentence 7 demonstrates again that there is little distinction between deep and shallow orthography. Finally, 25% of participants showed that they enjoyed the rhyme jelly and belly in sentences 10.

13 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326

Figure 2 demonstrates distinctions between deep and shallow orthography and also rhyming words’ position in rhyming couplets and adjacent within the same sentence.

Figure 2: Results from rhyming sentences reading task

The main result shown in figure 2 is that most sentences were read fluently with few problems, irrespective of depth of orthography or position of rhyming pair. On average, sentences with orthographically deep rhymes did entail more prompting, short pauses and phonemic decoding. However, again the data does not indicate rhymes with a deep orthography are more difficult. One the other hand, these results might be demonstrating that rhyme does help overcome difficulty with obscure orthography. Regarding position of the rhyming words, couplets are highest for fluency and lowest for mistakes and difficulty, but there is insufficient data to indicate a relationship between position of rhyme and participants ability read.

5.3 Book reading task

The aim of this final task was to examine how well the book as a whole would be received by A0 learners as well as effect of rhyme (see appendix 5). Each student read the story aloud whilst I made notes on difficult areas and gave prompts where necessary. Every participant was able to read the book with few difficulties and the majority clearly comprehended the content of the story. There were problem areas, but these varied for each participant, exemplifying the diversity of abilities within the A0 level classroom. The influence of rhyme on phonological decoding was subtle but became more apparent in the more orthographically opaque examples. Table 3 displays the results from each participant’s reading of the book.

A common problem area that was not anticipated was number symbols. A conscious decision had been made to use number symbols (e.g. 1, 2, 3) instead of number words (e.g. one, two, three), because they were assumed to be more accessible than opaque words, such as eight. Even though the meaning of the symbol is clear, there are no phonological clues for the phonetic realisation of the symbol. I decided to keep the number symbols in the final copy of the book because they provided an opportunity to practise and improve grasp of numbers in English. Also, the difficulties with these decreased as the students progressed through the book, due the amount of repetition.

14 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326

Difficulty with words that had opaque orthography, consonant clusters and/or digraphs was an anticipated problem. Interestingly, only three words consistently caused problems for participants; these were cries, irons and sighs. These were all orthographically opaque with silent letters and consonant clusters. Cries and sighs not only stood out as problematic, but also as examples where some participants used rhyme to overcome difficulties. Three of the participants adjusted or corrected their reading of sighs to rhyme with cries, one of whom adjusted to their incorrect decoding of cries, but this still shows development of phonological decoding in spite of highly opaque orthography. Other participants showed use of rhyme by self-correcting their reading or sore to rhyme with door or reading with a sense of rhythm.

Overall, this task gave positive and encouraging results, as the book succeeding in being linguistically accessible and comprehensible. The illustrations proved important as a few participants overtly referred to them to better understand the meaning of the text. Also, rhyme did appear to positively affect the reading experience for the reader. It aided phonological decoding in areas of difficulty, gave the text a sense of rhythm and in places participants expressed overt enjoyment of the story.

Student Problem Replacements Too Drops Reads letter Uses rhyme Enjoy General areas Difficult ‘-s’ by letter story ability 1. AG starts, quick; boy(s); ‘cry’ [cri:] and crisps nurse give(s) ‘sigh’ [si:] rhyme; correctly guesses ‘sore’ 2. MD Number irons No use rhyme to Difficult start symbols guide decoding; but competent Corrects ‘sore’ and fluent to rhyme with ‘door’ 3. GA Number chips (crisps) uses; Some rhymes Competent symbols; irons picked up and and fluent, cries used for hardly any [cri:ɛs] decoding, but issues not all. 4. EB Number baby (boy) cough ring(s) late, apples, Decoding of Quite symbols more, irons, ‘sighs’ is not competent, cries helped by ‘cries’ with some problem areas

15 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326

and mistakes 5. MB Reads with Very rhythm competent and fluent, no problems. 6. MS irons- chips (crisps); cream irons ‘Cry’ [craɪ] used Quite [i:]rons; cars (cries) to correctly competent and sick; decode ‘sigh’ fluent, with spotty [saɪ]; some self- some problem correction, but areas mainly guessing 7. XT cry [cri:] late; give(s) sighs, worse Reads with a yes Quite socks; rhythm Competent sighs; and fluent, rings; uses cream illustrations to clarify meaning 8. SP Number irons; Competent, symbols; spot; medium feels- worse; fluency, uses f[ɛ]ls; sighs illustrations cream- cr[ɛ]m 9. MK chew; Reads with yes Competent Mrs. rhythm; uses and fluent, ‘Cry’ [craɪ] to very few correctly guess pauses decoding of ‘Sigh’ [saɪ]

6. Discussion

As mentioned in the hypothesis, I was expecting participants to demonstrate a high awareness of rimes and a distinction between shallow and deep orthography. The results of the first task could show this to be incorrect, as they indicate low rhyme awareness and a lack of distinction between shallow and deep orthography. However, a more likely conclusion is that lack of understanding of the task and an inappropriate data collection method is responsible for the results.

The second task demonstrated that participants were able to read sentences involving rhyme fluently and with little difficulty. The lack of difficulty when decoding words, and use of rhyme to develop a rhythm, implies that the participants are more aware of rimes than the first task would indicate. However, similarly to the previous results, little distinction is made between rhyming words with deep and shallow orthography. A possible conclusion could be that the rhyme is effective in aiding the decoding of opaque orthography, but it could also be

16 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326 related to sight word repertoire. Another possible factor is that participants learnt from their mistakes as they went through the reading tasks. This again could be a factor in lack of consistent problems with opaque rhymes. To clarify this result, further testing is needed on words with opaque orthography, without the influence of rhyme and repetition.

When discussing the tasks with the participants’ teachers, they predicted their students would have a lot of trouble with the book and suggested I should work with the next higher level, the A1 level group. Out of all the tasks, I found the students responded best to the book reading and had very few problems regarding accessibility. Due to the teachers’ sceptiscm towards the book, they may have given me students from the top of their A0 level class instead of a selection that would represent the average level of ability. This could be a factor in why there was a consistently high level of fluency and competence in the reading tasks.

An aspect of the reading tasks that proved particularly difficult was trying to test participants’ ‘enjoyment’. Certain outward displays were noted, such as smiling after reading particular sentences and comments made during the interview, demonstrating the participant had engaged with the story. The fact that any of the participants overtly expressed enjoyment or engagement with the text is a very positive outcome and could indicate that rhyme does indeed improve content and encourage language play. However, this is far from a conclusive finding and further investigation is needed.

Throughout the tasks, the participants demonstrated low oral ability but a relatively high level of competence when reading. As mentioned in the literature review, any formal instruction in an alphabetic script will result in increased phonemic awareness. Most of the participants were literate in their L1’s (all of which use an alphabetic system, except Mandarin) meaning their phonemic awareness was likely to be well developed and they were comfortable with text and reading tasks. To further develop this study and see if rhyme could be used to bridge the gap between pre-literate and illiterate phonological awareness L1 low-literate adults would be needed.

7. Conclusion

The first aim of this study was to find out how aware A0 level learners were of rimes in written English. The results from the first task proved inconclusive, but across the other two tasks participants did show an awareness of rhyme. This was demonstrated by self-correcting and correctly guessing opaque words in rhyming couplets and sentences, reading with rhythm

17 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326 and expressing enjoyment after certain rhyming combinations. The next aim was to find out whether rhyme helps participants to decoding English orthography. The level of consistent fluency and competence in both reading tasks, irrespective of shallow or deep orthography, indicates that rhyme does prove effective. Also, the evidence of participants’ correctly guessing opaque words in rhyming couplets that they had previously decoded incorrectly showed that rhyme helps overcome problems with English orthography.

The third and final aim was to find out whether rhyme was an effective device in ESOL A0 level fiction writing, not only encouraging students’ phonological awareness but also reading for pleasure. There were instances in both reading task where participants expressed enjoyment of the text, demonstrating the use of rhyme in encouraging language play. Also, the response to the book was very positive overall, with some participants overtly engaging with the text, not just as a reading exercise but as a story. These results show that rhyme is accessible and effective in making a text or story more engaging for low level learners.

Overall, the findings of this study indicate that rhyme aids decoding of opaque words in English, in a way that is accessible to A0 learners. The results also show that rhyme makes text more engaging for this level learner by introducing rhythm and an element of language play. Therefore, rhyme can be used to ease the tension between content and accessibility in fiction books aimed at ESOL A0 level learners. However, there are areas of this study which need to be investigated further, before these findings can be considered fully conclusive.

18 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326

Bibliography

Brock, A. & C. Rankin, 2008. Communication, Language and Literacy from Birth to Five. London: SAGE Ltd.

Burt, J. & P. Butterworth. 1996. ‘Spelling in Adults: Orthographic transparency, learning new letter strings and reading accuracy’. The European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 8 (1): 3- 44.

Burt, J. & P. Blackwell. 2008. ‘Sound-spelling consistency in adults’ orthographic learning’. Journal of Research Reading 31 (1): 77-96.

Cardoso-Martins, C. 1995. ‘Sensitivity to rhymes, syllables, and phonemes in literacy acquisition in Portuguese’. Reading Research Quarterly 30 (4): 808-828.

Cook, G. 1997. ‘Language Play, Language Learning’. ELT Journal 51 (3): 224-231.

Delpit, L. 1991. ‘Interview with William H. Teale, editor of Language Arts’. Language Arts 68: 541-547.

Elbro, C., & B. Pallesen. 2002. ‘The quality of phonological representations and phonological awareness: a causal link?’ in L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro & P. Reitsma (eds) Precursors of Functional Literacy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

Geudens, A. & D. Sandra. 2002. ‘The role of orthographic onset-rime units in Dutch beginning readers’ in L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro & P. Reitsma (eds) Precursors of Functional Literacy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

19 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326

Goswami U. & P. Bryant. 1990. Phonological Skills and Learning to Read. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Hill, D. 2001. ‘Survey: Graded Readers’. ELT Journal 55 (3): 300-24.

Katz, L. & R.Frost. 1992. ‘The reading process is different for different orthographies: the orthographic depth hypothesis’ in R. Frost & L. Katz (eds) Orthography, Phonology, Morphology, and Meaning. Amsterdam: Elsevier North Holland Press: 67-84.

Kirtley, C., P. Bryant, M. MacLean & L. Bradley. 1989. ‘Rhyme, rime and the onset of reading’. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 48: 224-245.

Krashen, S. 1989. ‘We Acquire Vocabulary and Spelling by Reading: Additional Evidence for the Input Hypothesis’. The Modern Language Journal 73 (4): 440-460.

Kurvers, J., R. van Hout & T. Vallen. 2006. ‘Discovering features of language: metalinguistic awareness of adult literates’ in I. van de Craats, J. Kurvers & M. Young-Scholten (eds) Low- Educated Adult Second Language and Literacy Acquisition. Netherlands: 70 -88.

Linse, C. 2007. ‘Predictable books in the children’s EFL classroom’. ELT Journal 61 (1): 46- 54.

Loureiro, C., L. Braga, L. Souza, G. Filho, E. Queiroz & G. Dellatolas. 2004. ‘Degree of illiteracy and phonological and meta-phonological skills in unschooled adults’. Brain and Language 89 (3): 499-502.

20 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326

McKie, M. 1997. ‘The origins and early development of rhyme in English verse’. The Modern Language Review 92 (4): 871-831.

Morais, J., L. Cary, J. Alegria & P. Bertlson. 1979. ‘Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously?’ Cognition 7: 323-331

Pacton, S., M. Fayol & P. Perruchet. 2002. ‘The acquisition of untaught orthographic regularities in French’ in L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro & P. Reitsma (eds) Precursors of Functional Literacy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

Rodrigo, V., D. Greenberg, V. Burke, R. Hall, A. Berry, T. Brink, H. Joseph & M. Oby. 2007. ‘Implementing an extensive reading program and library for adult literacy learners’. Reading in a Foreign Language 19 (2): 106-119.

Sedgwick, F. 2010. Inspiring Children to Read and Write for Pleasure: Using literature to inspire literacy learning for 8-12. London: Routledge.

Shepperd, L. 2011. Phonological Awareness and Text Simplification in Fiction Books Aimed at Low-Literate Immigrant Adults. Attached to SEL 3012: Low Educated Language and Literacy Acquisition. Newcastle University.

Treiman, R., J. Mullennix, R. Bijeljac-Babic & E. Richmond-Welty. 1995. ‘The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography’. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124 (2): 107-136.

Vainikka, A. & M. Young-Scholten. 2005. ‘The roots of syntax and how they grow: Organic Grammar, the Basic Variety and Processability Theory’. Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 39: 77-106

21 081006883 Extended Study SEL 3326

Verhoeven, L., C. Elbro & P. Reitsma. 2002. ‘Functional literacy in a developmental perspective’ in L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro & P. Reitsma (eds) Precursors of Functional Literacy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

Yoon, H., D. Bolger, O. Kwon & C. Perfetti. 2002. ‘Subsyllabic units in reading: A difference between Korean and English’ in L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro & P. Reitsma (eds) Precursors of Functional Literacy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

Young-Scholten, M. & C. Ijuin. 2006. ‘How can we better measure adult ESL student progress?’ AEIS Newsletter 4: 2.

Young-Scholten, M. & N. Strom. 2006. First-time adult L2 readers: Is there a critical period? In I. van de Craats, J. Kurvers, & M. Young-Scholten (Eds.) Low-educated adult second language and literacy acquisition Utrecht: LOT: 45-68

Young-Scholten, M & D. Maguire. 2008. ‘Stories for extensive reading for LESLLA learners’ in I. van de Craats & J. Kurvers (eds) Low-Educated Second Language and Literacy Acquisition: Proceedings of the 4th Symposium. Utrecht: LOT.

22

Recommended publications