NATURAL VARIATION AND GENE EXPRESSION IN THE MALE COURTSHIP PROGRESSION OF MELANOGASTER

BY

ELIZABETH ANNE RUEDI

B.A., Lake Forest College, 2001

DISSERTATION

Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Biology in the Graduate College of the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 2007

Urbana, Illinois

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. UMI Number: 3290367

INFORMATION TO USERS

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleed-through, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

® UMI

UMI Microform 3290367 Copyright 2008 by ProQuest Information and Learning Company. All rights reserved. This microform edition is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code.

ProQuest Information and Learning Company 300 North Zeeb Road P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. C e r t i f i c a t e o f C o m m i t t e e A p p r o v a l

University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Graduate College

July 2, 2007

We hereby recommend that the thesis by:

ELIZABETH ANNE RUEDI

Entitled:

NATURAL VARIATION AND GENE EXPRESSION IN THE MALE COURTSHIP PROGRESSION OF DROSOPHILA MELANOGASTER

Be accepted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of:

Doctor of Philosophy

Signatures:

— ____ Director of Research - Kimberly A. Hughes Head o/Qepart/ient Jeffrey - D. Brawn

Committee on Final Examit/afti

iberly A. Hughes Commitu ’.mber - Hugh M. Robertson

- t r -

Co} M em ber - Gene E. Robinson Committee M ember - Andrew V. Suan/z

Committee Member - Committee Member -

* Required for doctoral degree but not for master’s degree

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. © 2007 by Elizabeth Anne Ruedi. All rights reserved.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Abstract

Natural genetic variation in the temporal sequence of male courtship behavior can

be one target of selection. The goals of my dissertation research were to identify

variation in the sequence of male behavior using a natural population, and investigate the

genetic underpinnings of this variation. I developed a new, high-throughput method of

male courtship analysis; this method focuses on the progression of male courtship over

time (MCP), and is more sensitive than previously used metrics of measurement. I used

this method to examine differences in MCP between males from full-sibling families,

using either virgin or previously mated females as targets. There were significant genetic

differences in MCP for families placed with either female target, and hence there was

natural genetic variation for MCP in that population.

To investigate the genetic underpinnings of this behavioral variation, I examined

previously identified male courtship candidate genes for alterations in expression due to

changes in age or experience, and then investigated if the previously identified candidates

were also involved in natural behavioral variation. Candidate courtship genes identified

from previous research were examined for non-genotypic changes in gene expression

using genetically identical males of different ages, with different levels of exposure to

females. I found that several of these genes had altered expression due to age

(reproductive maturity) but no genes had alterations due to experience with a female.

I then performed a microarray analysis on reproductively mature males from

families with the most extreme differences in MCP from the first experiment. This

allowed me to determine which genes exhibited differential baseline expression

associated with variation in MCP. I identified fourteen candidate genes with differential

iii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. expression, and none of these had been previously associated with male courtship

behavior. I suggest potential roles for these fourteen new candidates, and a factor pattern

analysis revealed that several of them might be co-regulated. The culmination of this

research brings us one step closer to understanding the genetic basis for natural

behavioral variation in male courtship; this information can now be used to determine

what sequence differences are associated with expression differences in candidate genes,

and how selection might be acting on this variation.

iv

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Acknowledgements

This research is the culmination of six years of hard work. My time here at the

University of Illinois has been the time of my life. I have enjoyed the challenges and the

friendship, and my time in the lab. There are many people who have worked with me

and supported me; I will try to mention them all, but if I leave someone out it is entirely

unintentional. I would like to thank my advisor, Kim Hughes, for challenging me and

assisting me. Thanks to my thesis committee, Hugh Robertson, Gene Robinson, and

Andy Suarez, for their time and helpful comments. Thank you to Anne Houde for giving

me the encouragement and advising that it took to get me here in the first place. I would

like to thank Alison Bell for her stimulating conversations and suggestions for my first

manuscript. Thanks to the undergraduates who have helped me over the years, including

Karin Stokke, Rachael O’Callahan, Gavin Davis, and Amy Peterson. I would have never

survived without Melissa Reedy, the best laboratory assistant, wrangler, and friend.

Thanks to Silvia Remolina, Katelyn Michelini, Ashley Johnson, Rose Reynolds, and

Jenny Dmevich for being lab ladies and having fabulous ladies nights. Jenny has also

been my mentor in countless ways; thanks to her for helping me through the microarray

protocols. Thanks to Tom Newman for his assistance with qRT-PCR, extracting high-

quality DNA, and other molecular processes. Thanks to Rodrigo Valarde for helping me

troubleshoot.

I can never begin to say enough about Rose Reynolds, Amy Toth, Lynn

Anderson, and Fernando Miguez. Not only are they some of my best friends, they have

helped me survive graduate school. Whenever I needed help or comments or support or a

joke, they have been there. I hope it continues to be the same for the rest of my life.

v

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Thanks to my other ladies, all of whom have made huge impacts on my life at

some point during this experience: Jan Murray (winner of the best cook, support system,

and Canadian awards), Andrea Gschwend, Rachel Knepp, Lisa Ainsworth, Amy Cash,

Lydia Wraight, Corey Tarwater (winner of the best dance partner in the world award),

Molly Tranel, Eli Saetnan, Moni Berg-Binder (aka Binderberg), Jinelle Hutchins, Orla

Dermody, Maggie Prater, Melissa Chipman, Karen Cavey Rowe, Kelly Anderson, Maria

Villamil, and Yulia Tkachuk.

Thanks to my guys, who have kept me sane, made me laugh, hugged me, and let

me drink whiskey: Ian Kirwan, Paul Nabity, Paul Henne, Richard Webster, Brad

Cosentino (winner of the best movie watching partner award), Nano Tellez, Andy

Leakey, Dylan Maddox, Jeff Foster, Ken Reynolds (winner of the best baker award),

Matt Rambert, Sameer Bharwani, and Timmy Daugherty (winner of the best little brother

in the world award).

Most importantly, thank you to my family. Thank you to Melody Glaub for

giving me life, thank you to Marilyn and Mel Franklin for always being there, thanks to

Gagy and Grandpa for supporting me and making fly jokes, thanks to Grandpa and

Grandma Ruedi (I miss you). But especially, thank you to my parents. Mom and Dad,

you have been there through everything, supporting me through it all, giving me love,

encouragement, and confidence in my capabilities (even when I had lost it). I love you.

vi

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Table of Contents

Chapter 1: Overview of male courtship in the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogasler...... l

Chapter 2: Natural variation in the courtship progression of male Drosophila melanogasler...... 27 TABLES...... 50 FIGURES...... 55

Chapter 3: Alteration in expression of courtship genes due to age and experience in maleDrosophila melanogasler...... 57 TABLES...... 79 FIGURES...... 88

Chapter 4: Contribution of differential gene expression to natural variation in maleDrosophila melanogasler courtship...... 94 TABLES...... 112 FIGURES...... 116

ChapterS: Conclusion...... 118

Appendix A: Statistical tests for univariate models of single behaviors...... 121

Appendix B: Mean MCP scores for all VT families...... 122

Appendix C: Mean MCP scores for all MT families...... 125

Appendix D: Least square means of gene expression due to age, experience, or age*experience...... 128

Curriculum Vitae...... 137

vii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Chapter 1: Overview of male courtship in the fruit fly,

Drosophila melanogaster

Courtship behavior is vital to male fitness in many species because it is

necessary for successful reproduction. In species with no paternal care, mating success is

the primary determinant of adult male fitness; it can signal species identity and can be the

object of intersexual selection via direct benefits, indirect benefits, or sensory bias

modalities (Andersson 1994). Consequently, courtship success is a major fitness

component and should be under strong selection. However, like other fitness

components, this trait exhibits substantial heritable variation within populations of many

species (Stirling et al. 2002). Because variation in courtship behavior can lead to

reproductive isolation and speciation, evolutionary effects of such variation are

particularly interesting (Etges 2002; Sawamura and Tomaru 2002; Kulathinal and Singh

2004). To successfully understand the causes and consequences of this variation, we

must 1) identify the presence of genetic differences in a natural population, 2) investigate

the molecular mechanisms underlying variation in courtship behavior, and 3) identify the

evolutionary processes responsible for maintenance of variation.

My dissertation explores the first two of these questions using variation in male

courtship behavior in the fruit fly,Drosophila melanogaster. This is a well characterized

behavioral pattern in a model system, and thus is ideal for examining the behavior

genetics of natural variation. D. melanogaster male courtship behaviors are sequentially

ordered and include orienting toward the female, following/chasing the female,

performing a “courtship song” using wing vibrations, licking the female’s genitalia,

1

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. attempting copulation, and successful copulation (Bastock and Manning 1955). Visual,

auditory, and chemosensory cues are an integral part of successful courtship, and each

behavior plays an essential role in species recognition or assessment of the sex and

receptivity of potential partners (reviewed in Greenspan and Ferveur 2000).

Additionally, this behavioral sequence is under substantial genetic control, as revealed by

mutant screening (Hall 1994a). However, it is unknown if the genes identified by mutant

screening (or other genes) are involved in variation in male courtship behavior in natural

populations. Studies of variation in gene expression using non-mutant stocks of

Drosophila for traits such as response to alcohol (Morozova et al. 2006), response to heat

stress (Sorensen et al. 2005), and learning/memory (Dubnau et al. 2003) have produced

candidate gene lists that have very little overlap with gene lists previously identified

using mutant screening. This is true even for a study that identified candidates using

gene expression vs. mutant screening from the same source population (Dubnau et al.

2003).

Below, I review several studies investigating natural inter- and intra-specific

variation in components of male courtship behavior. I then detail the visual, auditory,

and chemosensory components contributing to male courtship behavior, as well as sex-

determination and learning. I also focus on several genes involved in these processes that

have been discovered using mutant screening; these candidate genes are important for the

production of normal courtship behavior. This is not an exhaustive list of candidates, but

rather identifies potential contributors to natural variation in behavior.

2

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Natural variation in Drosophila male courtship behavior

The complex courtship behavior in male Drosophila exhibits inter- and intra­

specific variation in several factors, including auditory and chemosensory components,

mating speed, and courtship vigor. Certain aspects of courtship song, such as interpulse

interval, show species-specificity (inter-specific variation) thought to be an important

factor in reproductive isolation (Wheeler et al. 1991; Ritchie and Gleason 1995; Gleason

and Ritchie 2004). Gleason et al. (2002) identified chromosome regions containing

quantitative trait loci (QTL) for intra-specific variation in courtship song using a panel of

recombinant inbred lines derived from a cross between populations. These QTL had large

effects, but the regions did not contain genes previously suspected to affect courtship

behavior. Within population variation in courtship song also appears to have a genetic basis

in D. melanogaster, because it can respond to selection (Ritchie and Kyriacou 1994). This

does not always hold true for o th e r Drosophila species (Aspi and Hoikkala 1993).

Variation in female cuticular hydrocarbon (CHC) profiles can contribute to

species recognition and courtship interactions between males and females (Antony et al.

1985; Scott 1986; Schaner et al. 1989; Ferveur and Jallon 1996; Ferveur and Sureau

1996). Inter-specific (Coyne et al. 1994) and intra-specific variation in CHC profiles has

been reported (Tompkins and Hall 1984). The variation in CHC profiles parallels

variation in male response to CHCs both across and within species. To my knowledge

there have been no studies focused on intra-population variation in either female CHC

profile or variation in male response to differences in chemosensory cues.

Many studies of variation have focused on latency to copulation, or mating speed.

There is a great deal of inter-specific variation in mating speed due to differences in

3

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. female re-mating speed; if females of a species have high re-mating frequency, male

courtship behavior in that species is typically short and presumably not as important as

sperm competition (Markow 2002). There is also intra-specific variation in mating

speed; the trait responded to selection for both fast and slow mating speeds and genes

were identified as contributing to this phenotypic difference using both QTL (Moehring

and Mackay 2004) and micoarray analyses (Mackay et al. 2005). However, the majority

of the variation between these lines is due to female willingness to mate (Mackay et al.

2005). Casares and collegues verified the importance of female phenotype on variation

in mating speed within a population (Casares et al. 1993). However, other studies of

intra-population variation demonstrate significant effects of male phenotype

(Stamenkovicradak et al. 1992; Hoffmann 1999), suggesting that males may also have an

important role in mating speed.

Finally, there have been documented cases of within-population variation in

general courtship vigor (Gromko 1987), total time spent courting and performing

courtship song (Boake and Konigsberg 1998), and copulation duration (Boake and

Konigsberg 1998). None of the inter-specific, intra-specific, and intra-population studies

of variation in male courtship has examined the entire behavioral repertoire, or the pattern

of male behavior over time. Additionally, although genes that have effects on courtship

have been identified in mutant screening, it is not known whether these genes or an

entirely new set of genes are contributing to naturally occurring variation in behavior

(Hall 1994b; Greenspan 1997). Genes identified using mutant screening fall into several

categories based on mutant phenotype: general courtship vigor/success; courtship song;

olfaction; sex-determination; and learning/memory.

4

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Components of behavior and candidate genes

Several genes have been identified as playing a role in overall courtship drive and

vigor, couch potato (cpo) is one of several genes whose mutation causes a general

reduction in male courtship behavior, but does not disrupt one specific aspect of male

courtship, cpo is located on chromosome III and encodes a putative RNA recognition

motif protein, which is an RNA binding protein involved in the regulation of gene

expression at the post-transcriptional level (Lasko 2000). Mutations in cpo have revealed

that it is necessary for normal embryonic development of the peripheral nervous system

(PNS), and that it is also expressed in the central nervous system (CNS), PNS, and

salivary glands of adults (Bellen et al. 1992). It is also required for normal adult

behavior, as hypomorphic mutants have slow anesthesia recovery, decreased phototactic

and geotactic responses, and decreased flight ability—the are physically capable of

doing these behaviors, but are sluggish (Bellen et al. 1992). Mutants for this gene also

have severely reduced male courtship indices (Hall 1994a). cpo is an especially

intriguing candidate for this study because although not much is known about its specific

effects on male courtship, hypomorphic expression of this gene could have detrimental

effects on the success of a male in a natural population. Differential expression for cpo is

found in other behavioral traits, mating speed (Mackay et al. 2005) and male aggression

(Edwards et al. 2006), which indicates that its expression could be an important factor in

other behaviors such as male courtship.

Mutations in a locus on the X chromosome, technical knockout (tko), also have an

effect on the success of a male’s courtship, tko mutant males do not usually succeed in

initiating copulation with wild-type females, and when placed with tko mutant females,

5

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. the number of successful copulations is greatly reduced compared to wild-type males

(Toivonen et al. 2001). Additionally, when tko mutant males are placed with tko mutant

females the latency to copulation is significantly longer than that of wild-type males,

indicating impairment in the general courtship ability of tko males (Toivonen et al. 2001).

tko encodes a ribosomal protein (Royden et al. 1987; Shah et al. 1997) that is part of the

redox reactions in the mitochondria; mutants have greatly decreased levels of redox

reactions, which are needed for normal energy output (Toivonen et al. 2001). tko is also

part of a class of “bang-sensitive” mutants that result in temporary paralysis after a

physical jolt (such as light vortexing) (Ganetzky and Wu 1982; Engel and Wu 1994;

Pavlidis and Tanouye 1995). This bang-sensitivity is due to a combination of hyper- and

hypoactivity in the sensory responses of the fly. Additionally, there is some indication

that thetko mutants are hearing impaired (Toivonen et al. 2001). Differences in the

expression of tko could influence behavior, because significant differential expression

was present between males in lines selected for fast and slow mating speed (Mackay et al.

2005).

Courtship song

Auditory cues are arguably the most important sensory aspect of courtship for

females in many species of Drosophila (Markow 1987). The male courtship song has

been implicated in species recognition (Wheeler et al. 1991). In D. montana, male

courtship song frequency correlates with the fitness of his progeny, and may be used as

an indicator of “good genes” by the female (Hoikkala et al. 1998). The song consists of

several distinct parts, including a series of pulses and a sinusoidal humming ‘sine song’

6

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. (von Schilcher 1976). The overall mean interval between pulses (interpulse interval, IPI)

is species specific, as are the IPI oscillation periods (IPIs vary slightly between pulse

songs, and oscillate sinusoidally; (Kyriacou and Hall 1982)). For D. melanogaster, the

overall mean IPI is 34 milliseconds and the IPI oscillation period is 55 seconds (Kyriacou

and Hall 1982). Prior exposure to the pulse song of the appropriate species enhances

female receptivity to male courtship (Kyriacou and Hall 1984), and both the pulse and

sine songs decrease female locomotor activity to promote copulation (von Schilcher

1976).

Mutations in the atonal (ato) locus on chromosome III result in abnormal

courtship song production. Originally isolated as a developmental mutation, ato encodes

a basic helix-loop-helix protein involved in DNA binding (Jarman et al. 1995). ato is the

proneural gene responsible for the proper development of the chordotonal organs and

photoreceptors (Jarman et al. 1994; Jarman et al. 1995). Mutants for ato lack Johnston’s

Organ, which is responsible for hearing (Eberl et al. 2000). Without the ability to hear,

the courtship song produced by atonal mutants is significantly different than that of

wildtype flies, possibly due to the lack of sensory feedback mechanisms available to

normal individuals (Tauber and Eberl 2001). ato is expressed in adult male heads (Loop

et al. 2004), and thus the product must be used after development. Subtle differences in

the expression of this gene during adulthood could result in the production of an altered

courtship song in males, reducing their courtship intensity and lowering their fitness.

The dissonance allele of no-on-transient A (nonA

gene involved in the production of the male courtship song as well as influencing normal

vision (Kulkarni et al. 1988; Jones and Rubin 1990). nonA is on the X chromosome and

7

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. encodes an RNA binding protein that resembles a transcription factor and/or an RNA

splicing protein (Rendahl et al. 1992; Stanewsky et al. 1993; Rendahl et al. 1996). The

gene has two potential RNA recognition motifs (RRMs) that are common in RNA

binding proteins (Stanewsky et al. 1993; Rendahl et al. 1996). Only one of these RRMs

is important in controlling both the visual and song production phenotypes (Rendahl et al.

1996). nonAdlss mRNA is expressed in the CNS throughout development and continues

into adulthood. Turning on the expression of nonA during development, adulthood, or

both results in the rescue of normal visual and song production phenotypes, indicating

that thenonA protein product is used in the CNS throughout the entirety of the fly’s life

(Rendahl and Hall 1996).

nonAd,ss mutants have significantly lower courtship indices than wild-type males,

presumably due to both the visual and song irregularities. The male courtship song of

these mutants has a highly irregular pulse song phenotype—the pulse cycles in the

beginning of the pulse song resemble those of wildtype males, but as the song continues

the pulses become polycyclic and have irregular frequencies (Kulkarni et al. 1988). The

role of nonA in the production of courtship song has been supported by a study that

introduced the nonA transcript from D. virilis into nonAdlss mutant D. melanogaster males.

The D. virilis nonA transcript rescued the visual phenotype of the transformed D.

melanogaster, and resulted in the production of a courtship song that closely resembled

that of wild-type D. virilis (Campesan et al. 2001). This implicates nonA as an important

component of the species-specificity of male courtship song. Any differences in the

expression of this gene could result in an irregular courtship song, which could

dramatically affect the successful courtship of the male. Expression differences have

8

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. already been detected in nonA for lines of flies selected for fast vs. slow mating speed

(Mackay et al. 2005) or high vs. low male aggression (Edwards et al. 2006). These

selection regimes both began with base populations collected from the wild; hence, nonA

could have differential expression in natural populations.

Olfactory cues

Chemosensory cues are used in male assessment of the receptivity of a potential

partner, and olfactory cues have been well described. The courtship stimulating

compounds of mature females were identified as cis cis 7,11 heptacosadiene and cis cis

7,11 nonacosadiene (Antony et al. 1985). Immature males have courtship stimulating

alkenes, cis 13 tritriacontene and cis 11 tritriacontene (Schaner et al. 1989), that are not

counterbalanced by an inhibitory compound found on mature males, cis 7 tricosene (Scott

1986). The duration of male courtship depends on the balance of male inhibitory and

female stimulatory pheromones, and each of these seem to be independently processed

(Ferveur and Sureau 1996). Recent findings indicate that the female hydrocarbons are

used to modulate male responses to ancestral pheromones common to several species,

and that sexual isolation might occur in part because the males learn which modulatory

pheromones are associated with female acceptance of courtship attempts (Savarit et al.

1999).

One of the only known genes expressed in the brain that influences olfaction and

courtship behavior is paralytic (para). Certain mutations in para, smellblind (sbl) and

olfaction-deficient (olf-D), alter the responses of both larvae and adults to olfactory cues

(Tompkins et al. 1980; Tompkins et al. 1982; Gailey et al. 1986; Lilly and Carlson 1990);

9

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. these were later mapped to the same region on the X chromosome as para, which had

originally been isolated due to mutants displaying reversible paralysis at high

temperatures (Lilly et al. 1994). para encodes a voltage-sensitive sodium channel protein

a subunit, which is thought to play an important role in mediating neural membrane

excitability (Loughney et al. 1989). para is alternatively spliced, with mRNA from

several transcripts detected in the CNS (Loughney et al. 1989; Amichot et al. 1993).

These alternatively spliced regions are highly conserved across D. melanogaster and D.

virilis, indicating that para proteins are important for normal function in the fly

(Thackeray and Ganetzky 1995). Thesbl and olf-D alleles exhibit deficiencies in

olfaction, while other para alleles do not; one possible explanation for this is that sbl is

an alternative splice variant of para, and that this sodium-channel variant is important for

olfaction (Lilly et al. 1994).

sbl and olf-D mutant males have significantly reduced courtship indices when

paired with virgin females and immature males, but have significantly increased

courtship indices when presented with mated females (Tompkins et al. 1980; Gailey et al.

1986). Their inability to detect cuticular hydrocarbon profiles impairs their ability to

court well, and their orientation and courtship pattern is often incorrect when compared to

wild-type males (Tompkins et al. 1980; Markow 1987). Decreased expression of this

particular alternative splice variant in adults could significantly influence male courtship

intensity, as any variability in the number or quality of this sodium-channel a subunit

could have an effect on olfactory capabilities.

10

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Sex-determination pathway

TheDrosophila sex-determination cascade involves many genes that are

alternatively spliced and expressed in a sex-specific manner, beginning with the

determination of the X: autosomal chromosome ratio during development. The reading of

the X:A ratio begins a male- or female-specific pathway—if thesex-lethal gene is turned

on by the genes responding to the X:A ratio, the female pathway has been chosen. The

transcription factor produced by sex-lethal then turns on female-specific splicing of the

transformer (tra) and transformer-2 (tra-2) genes, while simultaneously preventing

expression of male-specific fruitless splice variants. The tra gene then prevents the

expression of the male-specific form of doublesex. The male pathway is thus the default;

if sex-lethal is not expressed, tra and tra-2 are not activated, and the male-specific forms

of doublesex and fruitless are turned on, resulting in male behavior and appearance

(reviewed in MacDougall et al. 1995; Wolfner 2003).

Thefruitless gene on chromosome III is involved in the sex-determination

cascade. Mutations in fruitless (fru) result in many different phenotypes, ranging from

bisexual orientation to a complete abolition of courtship behavior (Gailey and Hall 1989).

Some fru mutants also have abnormal courtship songs (Villella et al. 1997).

Additionally, it seems to establish sex-specific aggressive behavior patterns and

dominance relationships (Vrontou et al. 2006). fru is alternatively spliced under the

control of tra and tra-2 (Heinrichs et al. 1998), and yields sex-specific transcripts; the

male-specific form of fruitless encodes a BTB zinc-finger protein, which is a putative

transcription factor, indicating that fru is involved in the regulation of a gene or genes

further down the sex-determination cascade (Ito et al. 1996; Ryner et al. \996). fru is also

11

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. expressed in the central nervous system of adult male flies, suggesting that it is involved

in gene regulation after development is complete (Goodwin et al. 2000; Lee et al. 2000).

The expression of the male-specific FRU protein also coincides with the presence of

octopamine in several locations in the male CNS, and fru seems to have implications for

controlling the switch from performing aggressive and courtship behaviors (Certel et al.

2007). Additionally, yh/ has highly variable expression amongst adult D. melanogaster

lines isogenic for the third chromosome (Hughes et al. 2006), and exhibits differential

expression in lines selected for fast and slow mating behavior (Mackay et al. 2005).

doublesex (dsx ) is also located on the third chromosome and is involved in the late

stages of the sex-determination cascade. Dependent on the presence of tra and tra-2, the

dsx gene can be alternatively spliced—the use of the default splice site results in male-

specific dsx expression, while tra and tra-2 allow the splicosome to use an alternate

splice-site, resulting in the expression of the female-specific dsx product (Burtis and

Baker 1989). Both dsx products encode transcription factors; DSXM actively suppresses

the expression of female-specific products such as yolk proteins (Coschigano and

Wensink 1993; An and Wensink 1995), while simultaneously activating the expression of

male-specific characteristics such as sex-comb development (Jursnich and Burtis 1993).

Peak dsx expression occurs during mid-pupal development, but expression continues

throughout adulthood in the CNS and the thoracic ganglia (Lee et al. 2002). This

expression is also sexually dimorphic, with males expressing much higher levels of dsx

during adulthood (Lee et al. 2002). There may be natural variation in expression of dsx,

as expression alters in response to selection for both mating speed (Mackay et al. 2005)

and male aggression (Edwards et al. 2006).

12

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Learning and memory

Male courtship behavior is modified by experience and environment. Males who

have been housed with other males (and exposed to homosexual courtship) achieve

successful copulation with females more quickly than naive males (Mcrobert and

Tompkins 1988). Males also learn to associate cis 7 tricosene (produced both by mature

males and mated females) with rejection behavior (Tompkins and Hall 1984), and modify

their courtship behavior in response to this association. This modification has been

termed “experience-dependent courtship conditioning”: males previously placed with

mated females show depressed mating activity (Siegel and Hall 1979). This conditioning

represses male courtship towards virgin females for 3-4 hours after contact with an

unreceptive female (Siegel and Hall 1979), but males retain the aversion to mated

females for approximately 24 hours (Gailey et al. 1984), supporting the idea that

courtship conditioning may exist to help males conserve energy and avoid courting

unreceptive females. However, the repression of courtship towards virgin females is

obviously not beneficial to the conditioned male. There does seem to be an adaptive

benefit of courtship conditioning conferred to males raised in an environment where they

encounter females in various states of receptivity. Males kept in constant contact with

many other flies for 21 generations showed enhanced courtship conditioning relative to

males with little exposure to females for the same number of generations (Reif et al.

2002).

Experience-dependent courtship conditioning has been identified as a form of

associative learning (Gailey et al. 1984), and the plasticity of learning and memory plays

13

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. an important role in the retention of this conditioning (Griffith et al. 1993). The gene

encoding calcium!calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKlt) on chromosome IV

was first identified due to its functional homology to rat CaM kinase, which has been

implicated in learning (Griffith et al. 1993). After placement with a mated female for an

hour, males expressing an inhibitor of CaMKII protein do not retain courtship

conditioning—the time spent courting a virgin female after conditioning is significantly

greater than that of wild-type males who have been through the same training (Griffith et

al. 1993). The level of expression of CaMKII product has a direct effect on the intensity

of the resistance to conditioning (Griffith et al. 1993). CaMKII product potentially

phosphorylates the Ether-a-gogo (EAG) potassium-channel subunit, which can modulate

and regulate the post-synaptic potential of a repeatedly stimulated neuron by allowing

proper repolarization of the nerve and regulation of neurotransmitter release (Griffith et

al. 1994). Learning itself seems to occur due to an increase in postsynaptic potential

(modulated by potassium current) in neurons during sensitization (Schwartz and

Greenberg 1987).

Mutations at the X-chromosome locus dunce (dnc ) result in males deficient in

normal learning, including experience-dependent courtship conditioning (Byers et al.

1981; Ackerman and Siegel 1986; Dauwalder and Davis 1995). dnc encodes an

alternatively spliced cyclic-AMP phosphodiesterase (cAMP PDE), which helps

metabolize cAMP (Chen et al. 1986; Qiu et al. 1991; Qiu and Davis 1993). Experiments

indicate that cA M P must be present in the mushroom bodies of the fly in order for normal

associative and nonassociative learning to occur. There is speculation that without

certain dnc products available to keep cAMP to a minimum when it is not needed, the

14

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. presence of cAMP during training stages fails to properly modulate the neurons used in

learning (Davis and Dauwalder 1991). This can be detrimental to the mating success of a

male in a large population, as wild-type males who have learned to avoid courting

immature males have higher courtship indices and shorter latency to copulation with

virgin females than dnc mutants (Gailey et al. 1985).

Conclusion

I Use of natural variation to investigate the relationship between genes and

behavior has been advocated recently (Greenspan 1995; Sokolowski 2001; Ben-Shahar et

al. 2002; Emmons and Lipton 2003; Fitzpatrick et al. 2005). The approach used in my

dissertation integrates molecular techniques and detailed examination of naturally

occurring behavior to investigate within-population variation in behavior. Although

within-population variation is the raw material for evolution, the identity and nature of

contributing genes are largely unknown. With my dissertation, I have begun to dissect

genetic and molecular mechanisms that underlie natural variation in a complex

phenotype. The objectives of my research are included below.

Most of the studies of natural variation in courtship within populations of D.

melanogaster have focused on one component of behavior (i.e. song production).

However, valuable information can be lost when the details of the entire courtship

progression are not examined, because individual have subtle differences in their

patterns of behavior over time (Markow and Hanson 1981).

Objective 1: Develop a high-throughput assay of male courtship behavior that captures the details of courtship progression for a sample size large enough for genetic and

15

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. genomic studies. Use this male courtship progression (MCP) assay to determine the presence of genetic variation in the whole sequence of courtship behavior within a natural population.

After the presence of variation in MCP within a natural population was detected, I

then wanted to investigate the possible molecular contributions to natural variation. I

focused on differential gene expression, as alteration of the expression of a gene can be

one target of selection (Harris-Warrick 2000; Insel and Young 2000). Part of this

alteration can be due to non-genotypic factors, such as age or experience.

Objective 2: Examine courtship genes previously identified in the literature for alteration in gene expression due to age or experience with a female. Genes were considered static if age, experience, or the interaction between age and experience had no effect on expression. Genes were considered dynamically expressed if any of these factors had a significant effect on expression.

In the set of previously identified courtship genes, I found that any dynamic

changes in gene expression are correlated with age (reproductive maturity), not

experience with females. Thus, I continued my investigation of genes contributing to

natural variation in male courtship progression using mature males. I wanted to

determine which genes exhibited differential baseline expression between families with

the most extreme differences in courtship progression from Objective 1.

Objective 3: Use full-genome microarray analysis to find candidate genes associated with High and Low male courtship progression. Compare these candidates with

16

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. previously identified candidates, and examine functional trends in the new set of candidates.

I discovered 14 candidate genes; none were previously associated with male courtship

behavior. In future research, I will confirm that these candidates are directly associated

with male courtship progression by using functional mutants with decreased (or silenced)

expression to examine their effects on behavior.

List of References

Ackerman SL, Siegel RW (1986) Chemically reinforced conditioned courtship in drosophila - responses of wild-type and the dunce, amnesiac and don giovanni mutants. J Neurogenet 3(2): 111-123.

Amichot M, Castella C, Berge JB, Pauron D (1993) Transcription analysis of the para gene by insitu hybridization and immunological characterization of its expression product in wild-type and mutant strains of Drosophila. Biochem Molec 23(3): 381-390. An WQ, Wensink PC (1995) Integrating sex-specific and tissue-specific regulation within a single Drosophila enhancer. Gene Dev 9(2): 256-266. Andersson MB (1994) Sexual Selection. Princeton, N. J.: Princeton University Press. 599 Antony C, Davis TL, Carlson DA, Pechine JM, Jallon JM (1985) Compared behavioral- responses of male Drosophila-melanogaster (canton-s) to natural and synthetic aphrodisiacs. J Chem Ecol 11(12): 1617-1629.

Aspi J, Hoikkala A (1993) Laboratory and natural heritabilities of male courtship song characters in Drosophila montana and D. Littoralis. Heredity 70(4): 400-406.

Bastock M, Manning A (1955) The courtship ofDrosophila melanogaster. Behaviour 8: 7-110. Bellen HJ, Vaessin H, Bier E, Kolodkin A, Develyn D et al. (1992) The Drosophila couch-potato gene - an essential gene required for normal adult behavior. Genetics 131(2): 365-375.

17

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Ben-Shahar Y, Robichon A, Sokolowski MB, Robinson GE (2002) Influence of gene action across different time scales on behavior. Science 296(5568): 741-744.

Boake CRB, Konigsberg L (1998) Inheritance of male courtship behavior, aggressive success, and body size in Drosophila silvestris. Evolution 52(5): 1487-1492. Burtis KC, Baker BS (1989) Drosophila doublesex gene controls somatic sexual- differentiation by producing alternatively spliced messenger-RNAs encoding related sex-specific polypeptides. Cell 56(6): 997-1010. Byers D, Davis RL, Kiger JA (1981) Defect in cyclic-amp phosphodiesterase due to the dunce mutation of learning in Drosophila-melanogaster. Nature 289(5793): 79-81. Campesan S, Dubrova Y, Hall JC, Kyriacou CP (2001) The nonA gene in Drosophila

conveys species-specific behavioral characteristics. Genetics 158(4): 1535-1543. Casares P, Carracedo MC, Miguel ES, Pineiro R, Garciaflorez L (1993) Male mating speed in Drosophila-melanogaster - differences in genetic architecture and in relative performance according to female genotype. Behavior Genetics 23(4): 349-358. Certel SJ, Savella MG, Schlegel DCF, Kravitz EA (2007) Modulation of Drosophila male behavioral choice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104(11): 4706-4711. Chen CN, Denome S, Davis RL (1986) Molecular analysis of cdna clones and the corresponding genomic coding sequences of the Drosophila dunce+ gene, the structural gene for cAMP phosphodiesterase. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 83(24): 9313-9317. Coschigano KT, Wensink PC (1993) Sex-specific transcriptional regulation by the male and female doublesex proteins of Drosophila. Gene Dev 7(1): 42-54. Coyne JA, Crittenden AP, Mah K (1994) Genetics of a pheromonal difference contributing to reproductive isolation in Drosophila. Science 265(5177): 1461- 1464. Dauwalder B, Davis RL (1995) Conditional rescue of the dunce learning memory and female fertility defects withDrosophila or rat transgenes. Journal of Neuroscience 15(5): 3490-3499.

18

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Davis RL, Dauwalder B (1991) The Drosophila dunce locus - learning and memory genes in the fly. Trends Genet 7(7): 224-229. Dubnau J, Chiang AS, Grady L, Barditch J, Gossweiler S et al. (2003) The staufen/pumilio pathway is involved in Drosophila long-term memory. Current Biology 13(4): 286-296. Eberl DF, Hardy RW, Kernan MJ (2000) Genetically similar transduction mechanisms for touch and hearing in Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience 20(16): 5981-5988. Edwards AC, Rollmann SM, Morgan TJ, Mackay TFC (2006) Quantitative genomics of aggressive behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Plos Genetics 2(9): 1386-1395. Emmons SW, Lipton J (2003) Genetic basis of male sexual behavior. J Neurobiol 54(1): 93-110.

Engel JE, Wu CF (1994) Altered mechanoreceptor response in Drosophila bang-sensitive mutants. J Comp Physiol A 175(3): 267-278. Etges WJ (2002) Divergence in mate choice systems: Does evolution play by rules?

Genetica 116(2-3): 151-166. Ferveur JF, Sureau G (1996) Simultaneous influence on male courtship of stimulatory and inhibitory pheromones produced by live sex-mosaic Drosophila melanogaster. P Roy Soc Lond B Bio 263(1373): 967-973. Ferveur JF, Jallon JM (1996) Genetic control of male cuticular hydrocarbons in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetical Research 67(3): 211-218. Fitzpatrick M, Ben-Shahar Y, Smid H, Vet L, Robinson G et al. (2005) Candidate genes for behavioural ecology. Trends Ecol Evol 20(2): 96-104. Gailey DA, Hall JC (1989) Behavior and cytogenetics of fruitless in Drosophila-

melanogaster - different courtship defects caused by separate, closely linked lesions. Genetics 121(4): 773-785. Gailey DA, Jackson FR, Siegel RW (1984) Conditioning mutations in Drosophila- melanogaster affect an experience-dependent behavioral-modification in courting males. Genetics 106(4): 613-623. Gailey DA, Hall JC, Siegel RW (1985) Reduced reproductive success for a conditioning mutant in experimental populations of Drosophila-melanogaster. Genetics 111(4): 795-804.

19

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Gailey DA, Lacaillade RC, Hall JC (1986) Chemosensory elements of courtship in normal and mutant, olfaction-deficient Drosophila-melanogaster. Behavior Genetics 16(3): 375-405. Ganetzky B, Wu CF (1982) Indirect suppression involving behavioral mutants with altered nerve excitability in Drosophila-melanogaster. Genetics 100(4): 597-614. Gleason JM, Ritchie MG (2004) Do quantitative trait loci (qtl) for a courtship song difference between Drosophila simulans and D. sechellia coincide with candidate genes and intraspecific QTL? Genetics 166(3): 1303-1311. Gleason JM, Nuzhdin SV, Ritchie MG (2002) Quantitative trait loci affecting a courtship signal in Drosophila melanogaster. Heredity 89: 1-6. Goodwin SF, Taylor BJ, Villella A, Foss M, Ryner LC et al. (2000) Aberrant splicing and altered spatial expression patterns in fruitless mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics 154(2): 725-745. Greenspan RJ (1995) Understanding the genetic construction of behavior. Sci Am 272(4):

72-78. Greenspan RJ (1997) A kinder, gentler genetic analysis of behavior: Dissection gives

way to modulation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 7(6): 805-811. Greenspan RJ, Ferveur JF (2000) Courtship in Drosophila. Annu Rev Genet 34: 205-232. Griffith LC, Wang J, Zhong Y, Wu CF, Greenspan RJ (1994) Calcium/calmodulin- dependent protein-kinase-II and potassium channel subunit eag similarly affect plasticity in Drosophila. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 91(21): 10044-10048. Griffith LC, Verselis LM, Aitken KM, Kyriacou CP, Danho W et al. (1993) Inhibition of

calcium calmodulin-dependent protein-kinase in Drosophila disrupts behavioral plasticity. Neuron 10(3): 501-509. Gromko MH (1987) Genetic constraint on the evolution of courtship behavior in Drosophila-melanogaster. Heredity 58: 435-441. Hall JC (1994a) The mating of a fly. Science 264(5166): 1702-1714. Hall JC (1994b) Pleiotropy of Behavioral Genes. In: Greenspan RJ, Kyriacou CP, editors. Flexability and constraint in behavioral systems: John Wiley & Sons, LTD. pp. 15-27.

20

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Harris-Warrick RM (2000) Ion channels and receptors: Molecular targets for behavioral evolution. J Comp Physiol A 186(7-8): 605-616. Heinrichs V, Ryner LC, Baker BS (1998) Regulation of sex-specific selection of fruitless 5 ' splice sites by transformer and transformer-2. Mol Cell Biol 18(1): 450-458. Hoffmann AA (1999) Is the heritability for courtship and mating speed in Drosophila (fruit fly) low? Heredity 82: 151-157. Hoikkala A, Aspi J, Suvanto L (1998) Male courtship song frequency as an indicator of male genetic quality in an insect species, Drosophila montana. P Roy Soc Lond B Bio 265(1395): 503-508. Hughes KA, Ayroles JF, Reedy MM, Drnevich JM, Rowe KC et al. (2006) Segregating variation in the transcriptome: Cis regulation and additivity of effects. Genetics

173(3): 1347-1364. Insel TR, Young LJ (2000) Neuropeptides and the evolution of social behavior. Curr Opin Neurobiol 10(6): 784-789. Ito H, Fujitani K, Usui K, ShimizuNishikawa K, Tanaka S et al. (1996) Sexual orientation in Drosophila is altered by thesatori mutation in the sex- determination gene fruitless that encodes a zinc finger protein with a btb domain. Proceedings of the National Academy o f Sciences of the United States of America 93(18): 9687-9692. Jarman AP, Sun Y, Jan LY, Jan YN (1995) Role of the proneural gene, atonal, in formation of Drosophila chordotonal organs and photoreceptors. Development 121(7): 2019-2030. Jarman AP, Grell EH, Ackerman L, Jan LY, Jan YN (1994) Atonal is the proneural gene for Drosophila photoreceptors. Nature 369(6479): 398-400. Jones KR, Rubin GM (1990) Molecular analysis of no-on-transient-A, a gene required for normal vision in Drosophila. Neuron 4(5): 711-723. Jursnich VA, Burtis KC (1993) A positive role in differentiation for the male doublesex protein of Drosophila. Dev Biol 155(1): 235-249. Kulathinal RJ, Singh RS (2004) The nature of genetic variation in sex and reproduction- related genes among sibling species of the Drosophila melanogaster complex. Genetica 120(1-3): 245-252.

21

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Kulkarni SJ, Steinlauf AF, Hall JC (1988) The dissonance mutant of courtship song in Drosophila-melanogaster - isolation, behavior and cytogenetics. Genetics 118(2): 267-285.

Kyriacou CP, Hall JC (1982) The function of courtship song rhythms in Drosophila. Animal Behaviour 30(AUG): 794-801. Kyriacou CP, Hall JC (1984) Learning and memory mutations impair acoustic priming of mating-behavior in Drosophila. Nature 308(5954): 62-65. Lasko P (2000) The Drosophila melanogaster genome: Translation factors and RNA binding proteins. J Cell Biol 150(2): F51-F56. Lee G, Hall JC, Park JH (2002) Doublesex gene expression in the central nervous system of Drosophila melanogaster. J Neurogenet 16(4): 229-248. Lee G, Foss M, Goodwin SF, Carlo T, Taylor BJ et al. (2000) Spatial, temporal, and sexually dimorphic expression patterns of the fruitless gene in the Drosophila central nervous system. J Neurobiol 43(4): 404-426. Lilly M, Carlson J (1990) Smellblind - a gene required for Drosophila olfaction. Genetics 124(2): 293-302. Lilly M, Kreber R, Ganetzky B, Carlson JR (1994) Evidence that theDrosophila olfactory mutant smellblind defines a novel class of sodium-channel mutation. Genetics 136(3): 1087-1096. Loop T, Leemans R, Stiefel U, Hermida L, Egger B et al. (2004) Transcriptional signature of an adult brain tumor in Drosophila. BMC Genomics 5: 24.

Loughney K, Kreber R, Ganetzky B (1989) Molecular analysis of the para locus, a sodium-channel gene in Drosophila. Cell 58(6): 1143-1154.

MacDougall C, Harbison D, Bownes M (1995) The developmental consequences of alternate splicing in sex determination and differentiation in Drosophila. Dev Biol 172(2): 353-376. Mackay TFC, Heinsohn SL, Lyman RF, Moehring AJ, Morgan TJ et al. (2005) Genetics and genomics of Drosophila mating behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102: 6622-6629.

22

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Markow TA (1987) Behavioral and sensory basis of courtship success in Drosophila- melanogaster. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 84(17): 6200-6204. Markow TA (2002) Perspective: Female remating, operational sex ratio, and the arena of sexual selection in Drosophila species. Evolution 56(9): 1725-1734. Markow TA, Hanson SJ (1981) Multivariate analysis of Drosophila courtship. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 78(1): 430-434. Mcrobert SP, Tompkins L (1988) 2 consequences of homosexual courtship performed by Drosophila-melanogaster and Drosophila-affinis males. Evolution 42(5): 1093-1097. Moehring A, Mackay T (2004) The quantitative genetic basis of male mating behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics 167(3): 1249-1263. Morozova TV, Anholt RRH, Mackay TFC (2006) Transcriptional response to alcohol exposure in Drosophila melanogaster. Genome Biology 7(10): 10. Pavlidis P, Tanouye MA (1995) Seizures and failures in the giant fiber pathway of

Drosophila bang-sensitive paralytic mutants. Journal of Neuroscience 15(8): 5810-5819. Qiu YH, Davis RL (1993) Genetic dissection of the learning memory gene dunce of Drosophila-melanogaster. Gene Dev 7(7B): 1447-1458. Qiu YH, Chen CN, Malone T, Richter L, Beckendorf SK et al. (1991) Characterization of the memory gene dunce of Drosophila-melanogaster. J Mol Biol 222(3): 553-565. Reif M, Linsenmair KE, Heisenberg M (2002) Evolutionary significance of courtship conditioning in Drosophila melanogaster. Animal Behaviour 63(Part 1): 143-155. Rendahl KG, Hall JC (1996) Temporally manipulated rescue of visual and courtship abnormalities caused by a nonA mutation in Drosophila. J Neurogenet 10(4): 247-256. Rendahl KG, Jones KR, Kulkarni SJ, Bagully SH, Hall JC (1992) The dissonance mutation at the no-on-transient-A locus of Drosophila-melanogaster - genetic- control of courtship song and visual behaviors by a protein with putative RNA- binding motifs. Journal of Neuroscience 12(2): 390-407.

23

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Rendahl KG, Gaukhshteyn N, Wheeler DA, Fry TA, Hall JC (1996) Defects in courtship and vision caused by amino acid substitutions in a putative RNA-binding protein encoded by the no-on-transient A (nonA ) gene of Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience 16(4): 1511-1522. Ritchie MG, Kyriacou CP (1994) Genetic-variability of courtship song in a population of Drosophila-melanogaster. Animal Behaviour 48(2): 425-434.

Ritchie MG, Gleason JM (1995) Rapid evolution of courtship song pattern in Drosophila-willistoni sibling species. J Evolution Biol 8(4): 463-479. Royden CS, Pirrotta V, Jan LY (1987) The tko locus, site of a behavioral mutation in Drosophila-melanogaster, codes for a protein homologous to prokaryotic ribosomal-protein sl2. Cell 51(2): 165-173. Ryner LC, Goodwin SF, Castrillon DH, Anand A, Villella A et al. (1996) Control of male sexual behavior and sexual orientation in Drosophila by thefruitless gene. Cell 87(6): 1079-1089. Savarit F, Sureau G, Cobb M, Ferveur JF (1999) Genetic elimination of known pheromones reveals the fundamental chemical bases of mating and isolation in Drosophila. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 96(16): 9015-9020.

Sawamura K, Tomaru M (2002) Biology of reproductive isolation in Drosophila: Toward a better understanding of speciation. Population Ecology 44(3): 209-219. Schaner AM, Dixon PD, Graham KJ, Jackson LL (1989) Components of the courtship- stimulating pheromone blend of young male Drosophila-melanogaster - (z)-13- tritriacontene and (z)-ll-tritriacontene. J Insect Physiol 35(4): 341-345. Schwartz JH, Greenberg SM (1987) Molecular mechanisms for memory - 2nd-messenger induced modifications of protein-kinases in nerve-cells. Annu Rev Neurosci 10: 459-476. Scott D (1986) Sexual mimicry regulates the attractiveness of mated Drosophila- melanogaster females. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 83(21): 8429-8433. Shah ZH, O'Dell KMC, Miller SMC, An X, Jacobs HT (1997) Metazoan nuclear genes for mitoribosomal protein sl2. Gene 204(1-2): 55-62.

24

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Siegel RW, Hall JC (1979) Conditioned responses in courtship behavior of normal and mutant Drosophila, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 76(7): 3430-3434.

Sokolowski MB (2001) Drosophila'. Genetics meets behaviour. Nat Rev Genet 2(11): 879-890. Sorensen JG, Nielsen MM, Kruhoffer M, Justesen J, Loeschcke V (2005) Full genome gene expression analysis of the heat stress response, in Drosophila melanogaster. Cell Stress & Chaperones 10(4): 312-328. Stamenkovicradak M, Partridge L, Andjelkovic M (1992) A genetic correlation between the sexes for mating speed in Drosophila-melanogaster. Animal Behaviour 43(3):

389-396. Stanewsky R, Rendahl KG, Dill M, Saumweber H (1993) Genetic and molecular analysis of the x-chromosomal region 14bl7-14c4 in Drosophila-melanogaster - loss of function in nonA, a nuclear-protein common to many cell-types, results in specific physiological and behavioral defects. Genetics 135(2): 419-442. Stirling DG, Reale D, Roff DA (2002) Selection, structure and the heritability of behaviour. J Evolution Biol 15: 277-289. Tauber E, Eberl DF (2001) Song production in auditory mutants of Drosophila : The role of sensory feedback. J Comp Physiol A 187(5): 341-348. Thackeray JR, Ganetzky B (1995) Conserved alternative splicing patterns and splicing signals in the Drosophila sodium-channel gene para. Genetics 141(1): 203-214. Toivonen JM, O'Dell KMC, Petit N, Irvine SC, Knight GK et al. (2001) technical knockout, a Drosophila model of mitochondrial deafness. Genetics 159(1): 241-254. Tompkins L, Hall JC (1984) Sex-pheromones enable Drosophila males to discriminate between conspecific females from different laboratory stocks. Animal Behaviour 32(MAY): 349-352. Tompkins L, Hall JC, Hall LM (1980) Courtship-stimulating volatile compounds from normal and mutant Drosophila. J Insect Physiol 26: 689-697.

25

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Tompkins L, Gross AC, Hall JC, Gailey DA, Siegel RW (1982) The role of female movement in the sexual-behavior of Drosophila-melanogaster. Behavior Genetics 12(3): 295-307. Villella A, Gailey DA, Berwald B, Ohshima S, Barnes PT et al. (1997) Extended reproductive roles of the fruitless gene in Drosophila melanogaster revealed by behavioral analysis of new fru mutants. Genetics 147(3): 1107-1130.

von Schilcher F (1976) The role of auditory stimuli in the courtship of Drosophila melanogaster. Animal Behavior 24: 18-26. Vrontou E, Nilsen SP, Demir E, Kravitz EA, Dickson BJ (2006) fruitless regulates aggression and dominance in Drosophila. Nature Neuroscience 9(12): 1469-1471. Wheeler DA, Kyriacou CP, Greenacre ML, Yu Q, Rutila JE et al. (1991) Molecular transfer of a species-specific behavior from Drosophila-simulans to Drosophila- melanogaster. Science 251(4997): 1082-1085.

Wolfner MF (2003) Sex determination: Sex on the brain? Current Biology 13(3): R101-R103.

26

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Chapter 2: Natural variation in the courtship progression of

male Drosophila melanogaster

Co-authored by Kimberly A. Hughes1

Abstract

Courtship behavior is a main component of male fitness, especially in species

with no parental care. Variation in this behavior can thus be a target for mate choice and

sexual selection, and can lead to evolution. The fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster, has

well-documented complex male courtship comprised of a progressive sequence of

distinct behaviors, and is an ideal model for behavior-genetic analysis. In order to

evaluate genetic differences in the temporal pattern of behavior, I have developed a

simple, high-throughput method that allows the documentation of the progression of male

courtship using an ordinal scale (male courtship progression scale, MCP). Using this

method, I document natural genetic variation in the temporal pattern of behavior that was

not detected using other metrics. This method was robust enough to detect genetic

variation in this trait for males placed with both virgin and mated female targets.

Introduction

Courtship behavior can be a key determinant of male fitness in animal species that

have little or no male parental care. It can signal species identity and can be the object of

intersexual selection via direct benefits, indirect benefits, or sensory bias modalities

(Andersson 1994). Despite potentially strong directional selection, which should erode

1 Ruedi, E.A. and Hughes, K.A. In review. Natural variation in the courtship progression o f male Drosophila melanogaster. Submitted to Behavior Genetics.

27

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. variation, courtship behavior exhibits substantial genetic variation within populations of

many species (Stirling et al. 2002). Because variation in courtship behavior can lead to

reproductive isolation and speciation, evolutionary effects of such variation are

particularly interesting (Etges 2002; Sawamura and Tomaru 2002; Kulathinal and Singh

2004). To successfully understand the causes and consequences of this variation, we

must 1) identify the presence of genetic differences in a natural population, 2) investigate

the molecular mechanisms underlying variation, and 3) identify the evolutionary

processes responsible for maintenance of variation. In this study I address the first of

these points. I also present a high throughput method for obtaining robust, detailed

measures of courtship behavior and compare this method with others currently available.

Drosophila melanogaster is a model system for genetic analysis of behavior. Flies

in the genus Drosophila have complex male courtship that is characterized by a

progressive sequence of distinct behaviors that culminates in copulation (Hall 1994). In

D. melanogaster males, the courtship progression includes orienting toward the female,

following her, tapping her abdomen, performing a courtship “song” via wing vibration,

licking her genitalia, attempting copulation, and copulation (Hall 1994). This behavioral

progression involves visual, auditory and chemical communication between males and

females, as well as aspects of learning and memory (Hall 1994; Yamamoto and Nakano

1998; Greenspan and Ferveur 2000).

Variation in male courtship has been demonstrated between closely related

species ofD rosoph ila (Ritchie and Gleason 1995; Markow etal. 2002; Gleason and

Ritchie 2004), between populations within species (ex. Gleason and Ritchie 2004;

Moehring and Mackay 2004; Mackay et al. 2005) and within populations (ex.

28

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Stamenkovicradak et al. 1992; Aspi and Hoikkala 1993; Ritchie and Kyriacou 1994;

Boake and Konigsberg 1998; Hoffmann 1999). However, many of these studies have

focused on one or a few specific behaviors in the courtship repertoire. Valuable

information can be lost when the details of the entire courtship progression are not

examined, because individual animals have subtle differences in their patterns of

behavior over time (Markow and Hanson 1981). Also, most previous studies have

assayed only male behavior towards virgin females. It is unknown if variation in

courtship towards virgin females is representative of courtship towards mated females.

In nature and in most laboratory culture conditions, most encounters between sexes will

involve previously-mated female targets (Bouletreau 1978; Partridge et al. 1987). Thus

the most evolutionarily relevant variation in courtship behavior may be that between

males when they encounter non-virgin females (Gromko and Markow 1993; Reif et al.

2002).

Most analyses of fly courtship have used metrics that either focus on a specific

behavior in the sequence or use a method that does not discriminate between different

behaviors in the sequence. For example, the commonly-used Courtship Index (Cl)

measures the proportion of the total observational trial that the male spends performing

any courtship behavior (eg. Siegel and Hall 1979). Other metrics include latency to

copulation (Moehring and Mackay 2004; Mackay et al. 2005), and copulation occurrence

and duration (e.g. and Giebultowicz 2004; Moehring and Mackay 2004). A

notable exceptionis the approach of Markow and Hanson(1981), w ho used detailed

analysis of focal animal samples to characterize temporal patterns of behavior. A more

detailed metric is more sensitive and more likely to pick up small, but critically important

29

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. behavioral variation that has a genetic basis; however, the focal animal approach is labor

intensive and difficult to apply to the large sample size needed in genetic and genomic

experiments. Therefore, we need to refine behavioral methods to compare differences

between individuals and keep pace with the genetic and genomic methods that require

high throughput phenotypic analysis.

Here, I developed a novel approach for quantifying the patterns of male courtship

that is sensitive to differences in both overall courtship activity and to temporal patterns

in activity and can be used for high throughput phenotypic analysis. I developed an

ordinal scale that mirrors the normal progression of courtship in D. melanogaster males,

and applied this method to analysis of courtship of males placed either with virgin or

mated females. I detected significant genetic differences between families from a single

wild-type population for males exposed to both kinds of females. Using this method, I

can quantify the extent and structure of genetic variation in courtship behavior and

examine differences resulting from the mating history of the female target.

Methods

Courtship Assay

D. melanogaster courtship typically follows the temporal sequence specified in

Table 2.1, culminating in successful copulation. I therefore assigned each behavior to an

ordinal value ranging from 1 (motionless) to 8 (copulation). The ordinal nature of this

male courtship progression (MCP) scale implies a qualitative measure of distance

between behaviors (e.g., licking is “closer” to copulation than is orienting) without

specifying a quantitative measure of distance between the behaviors (Martin and Bateson

30

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 1993). This ordinal scale, combined with repeated scan sampling during behavior

observations, can be applied to many mating trials simultaneously. When used with

appropriate statistical analysis, the technique can be used to quantify the intensity,

duration, and temporal pattern of a complex behavior, and can be efficiently used in

large-scale experiments requiring the observation of many individuals.

Experimental Flies

I used flies descended from wild-caught ancestors collected from a site in

Terhune, New Jersey by Valerie Pierce. The founding population consisted of

approximately 8,000 offspring from 400 females collected in 1999. This population was

maintained in the laboratory of Dr. Allen Gibbs at a population size of 1000-1500 for 45

generations (A. Gibbs, pers. comm.) until the Hughes lab obtained ~ 500 individuals in

2003. Since then, we have maintained the population with overlapping generations at a

size of -12,000 individuals. This population (subsequently, the NJ population) is kept on

standard commeal media in 8-dram vials, transferred to new media every 14 days, and

housed at 25°C on a 12h:12h light:dark cycle. Flies are randomly mixed in bottles and

redistributed to vials every 28 days to maintain random mating.

I chose the NJ population for this study because it has been in the lab long enough

(>100 generations) that novel-environment effects will not substantially bias estimates of

genetic variation (Service and Rose 1985), but not so long that variation has been

dramatically eroded. Indeed,the population has responded vigorously to selection for

desiccation resistance (Gefen et al. 2006) and lifespan (K. Hughes, unpublished data),

indicating that it harbors substantial genetic variability.

31

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. To detect genetic differences in courtship, I used a full-sib mating design, using

160 different full-sib families in two different experiments (80 families for each

experiment). Because of the large number of families in each experiment, families were

assayed in four blocks of 20 families each for each experiment. Parents of experimental

families were reared from controlled-density vials, collected as virgins 0-4 hours post-

eclosion, and housed in single-sex vials for 5-7 days. I produced single-pair matings by

randomly pairing males and females from this collection, and introducing each pair to a

fresh rearing vial. Pairs were allowed to produce offspring for four days, transferred to a

new vial for four more days, and were then discarded. Collecting males from these two

different rearing vials reduced the possibility of confounding environmental effects on

behavior with the effect of family. I collected 5-10 virgin male offspring from both vials

for each single-pair mating at 0 to 4 hours post-eclosion over a three-day period, and the

males were kept in single-sex vials separated by family and by date of emergence. These

virgin males comprised my full-sib families. I used 80 of these full-sib families for

courtship assays in which virgin females were the mating targets (virgin target, or VT

experiment); I used another 80 families for courtship assays in which previously-mated

females were the mating targets (mated target, or MT experiment). Up to eight males per

family were used in behavior trials (mean= 7, range = 5-8). The VT and MT experiments

were performed at different times with different females.

Females used as mating targets were collected from a stock in which a

spontaneously-occurringebony (e) mutation had been introgressed into a non-inbred wild

type genetic background (Ives background), ebony females were used because of their

dark body color, which aids in observer discrimination between male and female flies in

32

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. the behavioral trials. This stock, which is homozygous for thee mutation, is maintained

in a large, randomly-mating population with overlapping generations and reared under

the same conditions as the NJ population. The e/e females used in the behavioral trials

were collected as virgins 0-4 hours post-eclosion and kept in single-sex vials separated by

date of emergence. For each block of an experiment, 180 virgin females were collected

over a three-day period. For the VT experiment, the females were kept in their original

collection vials, at a density of 25 flies per vial, until the behavioral trial. Females from

the same vial were assigned to different male families in the behavior trial to avoid

confounding environmental effects shared among females with genetic effects of male

families. For the MT experiment, 180 virgin females were collected over a three day

period. 24 hours prior to the beginning of behavioral trials, subsets of 45 females were

placed in a half-liter bottle with media and with 50 e/e males for two hours, and then

housed separately from males in vials at a density of 25 females per vial. To ensure that

all females for the MT experiment had mated successfully with e/e males, any females

not observed in copula after twenty minutes of exposure to e/e males were removed via

aspiration and discarded.

Behavioral Observations

Behavior observations were conducted in devices modeled after Drapeau and

Long (2000), see Figure 2.1. Behavioral observations were carried out between July and

Decem ber, 2004. For each block of the VT and MT experiments, twomales from each

family were observed per day, and there were four days of observation per block. No

33

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. male was observed more than once, and observations were blind with respect to

genotype. All males were 5-6 days old when behavioral observations were conducted.

On the day before their trial, males were randomly assigned to one of forty arenas

(four separate devices with ten chambers each were used each day). Males were lightly

anesthetized with CO2 , and then placed in a chamber that comprised the bottom half of

each arena. After all males were added, the upper chambers were placed onto the

apparatus over opaque divider sheets. Females were lightly anesthetized and quickly

transferred over to the upper chambers (1 female/chamber). Finally, the clear plexiglass

top was attached, and the flies were allowed to recover from anesthesia overnight in an

incubator at 25°C.

All observations took place between 8 am and 10:30 am and were conducted at

24°C ± 1 °C, in a room isolated from all other activities. At the start of observations, the

opaque plastic sheet separating the sexes was removed; thus a single male was exposed to

a single female. The behavior of each male was then recorded via instantaneous scan

sampling (Martin and Bateson 1993) every 30 seconds for a total of 30 minutes. At each

scan, the male was scored as performing one of the behaviors in Table 2.1. At the end of

a trial, each male was thus characterized by 60 scores in a temporal sequence. All

observations were conducted by the same observer (E.A.R.). With this scheme, a total of

40 males could be observed per day in a two-hour time span (4 Vi-hour trials per day),

and each block consisted of four days of observation (160 males per block).

34

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Statistical Analyses

Families in the VT experiment were analyzed separately from families in the MT

experiment. All statistical analyses were conduced using SAS Statistical Analysis

Software v. 9.1 (SAS Institute 2006). Unless otherwise stated, dependent variables met

the assumptions of general linear models (normally and homoscedastically distributed).

Block effects and interactions of blocks with other factors were not significant in any

analyses; therefore, I report analyses with those effects removed.

In this experiment, I limited our attention to variation in male behavior, and I

tested males from each family using different, unrelated females. However, differences

among females can contribute to differences in male mating behavior (Reif et al. 2002). I

therefore interpret differences in male behavior between families as differences among

male genotypes, averaged over a random sample of female genotypes. In this experiment,

interaction between male and female genotype contributes to the residual variation in the

statistical analysis, and is thus not confounded with familial differences in male behavior.

Male Courtship Progression

The eight-point ordinal scale used to characterize male behavior resulted in a

multinomially distributed variable, with repeated measures on individuals at each 30-

second time interval. To analyze temporal patterns and family-by-time interactions for

this variable, I used a generalized linear model modified for repeated measures. This

model is a non-parametric alternative to ANOVA, and the generalized linear model

procedure SAS PROC GENMOD implements Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE)

to evaluate repeated-measures data structures (Littell et al. 2002). I used the cumulative

35

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. logit link function in PROC GENMOD to model the cumulative probabilities of the

ordinal MCP scores, and applied a repeated-measures analysis with individual flies as the

subject and the time-specific samples for each fly as the repeated measure. Only the first

30 time points of each trial (the first fifteen minutes) were used in the analysis, because

nearly all males had ceased locomotor activity after that time, and therefore had the same

MCP score: “motionless”. The model included both linear and quadratic effects of time

as predictor variables because preliminary inspection of the data indicated an

approximately quadratic change in MCP values over time (see Figure 2.2).

I first fit a model containing all terms and interactions of interest, and

subsequently removed interaction terms that were not significant. In the initial model,

the probability that an observation is a particular response out of the eight ordered

response categories of the MCP scale was modeled as: logit(Pj) = [i + f+ d + a(d) + t +

(t*t) + (f*t) +(f*t*t), where P, was the cumulative probability of the ordinal response i ,

and the other terms represent the fixed effects of family (f), day (d), trial (a) within day,

linear and quadratic effects of time (t) within the courtship trials (in 30-second intervals),

and interactions between family and time; fi, was the intercept for a particular ordinal

category. Because this is a repeated-measures analysis, a significant family effect

indicates that families are significantly different at time 0, and significant interactions

between family and time variables indicates that families have different temporal patterns

of behavior.

I was interested in detecting whether full-sib families differed from one another in

indices of courtship behavior, and also in identifying families with relatively high and

low levels of courtship behavior. This is a useful approach to identify families for studies

36

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. of the genetic basis of behavioral differences. Additionally, multinomial data is modeled

such that each observation has more than two possible outcomes (in this case, each

observation has 8 possible outcomes), and the expected mean for each observation is not

scalar. Because of this, variance is represented by a covariance matrix; thus it is not

possible to use the generalized linear model to calculate a single measure of “variance”

for MCP. For these reasons, I treated families as fixed effects in the analyses of MCP

and in derived metrics.

Comparing MCP to Other Metrics

I compared MCP to several commonly used metrics, including Cl, copulation

latency, occurrence, and duration, and a multivariate factor pattern analysis. Unless

otherwise noted, I analyzed these data using mixed linear models in SAS PROC MIXED.

The effects of family, day, and trial(day) were modeled as fixed effects. All univariate

metrics were compared by calculating the Spearman rank correlation between the metrics

across families. MCP can also be used to examine individual behaviors in the courtship

repertoire, as is seen in some literature (Gromko 1987; Gromko et al. 1991;

Stamenkovicradak et al. 1992; Aspi and Hoikkala 1993; Casares et al. 1993; Ritchie and

Kyriacou 1994; Boake and Konigsberg 1998). Results for this analysis are reported in

appendix A.

C l Proxy

I calculated a proxy for the most-commonly used composite measure of courtship

behavior, Cl, which is typically measured as the proportion of time during a trial that a

37

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. male spends performing any behavior related to courtship (eg. Siegel and Hall 1979;

Ferveur and Jallon 1993; Hall 1994; Ryner et al. 1996; Balakireva et al. 1998; Mackay et

al. 2005). Because each focal male was not observed continuously during his mating trial,

I could not calculate a true Cl from our data, but I could estimate Cl by dividing the

number of observations during which a male performed any of the MCP categories 3

through 8 (“orienting” through “copulation”) by the total number of observations for the

male. The dependent variable was transformed by taking the arcsine square root, as is

appropriate for proportions (Zar 1996).

Latency to copulation

“Mating speed” or “latency to copulation” has also been used as an index of

courtship behavior (Moehring and Mackay 2004; Mackay et al. 2005). I calculated an

equivalent metric for the VT experiment only, because very few families achieved

copulation in MT families. I recorded the latency to copulation as the observation period

that an individual reached the MCP score of 8 (copulation). In this analysis, all 60 time

periods of a mating trial were used to calculate latency. Any individuals that did not

achieve copulation during the trial were assigned a latency of “61”. Latency scores were

log transformed before analysis to improve fit to normality assumptions.

Copulation Duration and Occurrence

Some studies have evaluated mating behavior using measures of copulation

occurrence and duration (e.g. Beaver and Giebultowicz 2004; Moehring and Mackay

2004). To compare MCP to copulation occurrence, I tested for among-family differences

38

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. in copulation occurrence using a generalized linear model (PROC GENMOD) with

binomially distributed error and logit link function. The model was: logit(jt) = p + f + d +

a(d), where it was the probability of copulation for an individual, and the predictor

variables are abbreviated as above. I tested for differences in copulation duration by

fitting the linear model described above to the proportion of observations during which

an individual was observed copulating (transformed with arcsine square root).

Multivariate Analysis: Factor Pattern

I performed a factor pattern analysis to compare my results to the multivariate

analysis of Drosophila courtship developed by Markow and Hanson (1981). This

analysis is used to detect if particular behaviors typically co-occur. Markow and Hanson

(1981) conducted a factor pattern analysis of male behavior data collected on 15 pairs of

courting flies, and then calculated transition matrices documenting behavioral changes

over the course of the trial. Replication of the transition matrix analysis is not possible

with my data because I did not continuously monitor each focal animal. However, I

could perform a maximum-likelihood (canonical) factor pattern analysis (PROC FACT),

and also obtain factor-loading scores for each male for analysis of genetic variation. The

model for this analysis was: yy= xubij + Xj2b2j + ... + xiqbqj + ey, where yy was the

proportion of time individual i spent performing behavior j, Xjk, k=i..q is the value of theith

observation on the k h common factor, bkj is the regression coefficient of the lth factor for

predicting the j ,h behavior, and ey is the value of the i"‘ observation loaded on a unique

factor for the j th observed behavior; q is the number of common factors. I used a Scree

plot analysis to determine the number of significant factors (Stevens 1992), and the

39

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. factors underwent oblique promax rotation. I then estimated the factor loading scores for

the significant factors (two factors for both VT and MT experiments) using the factor

scoring coefficient determined by PROC FACTOR. Scores were calculated for each

individual fly by multiplying the score coefficient by the raw data (PROC SCORE). I fit

a linear model to these scores for each significant factor with family, day, and trial nested

in day as independent variables.

Results

VT Experiment

Male Courtship Progression

Full-sibling families differed in their temporal patterns of courtship during the

behavioral trials. In the repeated-measures analysis of males exposed to virgin females,

the family*time interaction was significant, and the family* time2 effect was marginally

non-significant (Table 2.2). The family effect was not significant, indicating that families

did not have significant genetic differences at the beginning of the trial. Both linear and

quadratic effects of time were significant, indicating a robust quadratic temporal pattern

of behavior variation during the trials. Figure 2.2a illustrates this pattern by showing the

mean MCP scores for eight families (mean MCP scores for all VT families are provided

in appendix B). Day and trial-within-day effects were also significant, demonstrating that

male courtship behavior was influenced by daily environmental effects and within-day

temporal effects.

40

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Comparing MCP to Other Metrics

There were significant family differences for Cl proxy, and for copulation

occurrence, latency, and duration (Table 2.3). The temporal effect of day was significant

for Cl as well as copulation latency and duration; trial-within-day was significant only for

copulation occurrence. The composite measure of Cl was significantly related to

copulation occurrence (rs = 0.51, n=80, p< 0.001), copulation duration (rs = 0.76, n=80,

p< 0.001), and latency to copulation (rs = -0.58, n=80, p< 0.001).

The Scree analysis indicated two important canonical factors for VT families

(eigenvalues >1.5, total variance explained: 58.6%). Factor 1 was dominated by positive

loadings of wing vibration, licking, and attempted copulation and a moderate negative

loading for motionless. Factor 2 was dominated by a large positive loading for

copulation, and a strong negative loading for motionless (Table 2.4). Families differed

significantly for Factor 2, and the effect of day was also significant; however, none of the

effects were significant for Factor 1 (Table 2.3).

MT Experiment

Male Courtship Progression

Results of the MCP analysis for males exposed to mated females were similar to

those for virgin females. Families differed significantly in how behavior changes over

time, but not in the baseline behavior at time 0 (Table 2.2). In this experiment, families

differed in the quadratic and in the linear effects o f time (Figure 2.2b, mean MCP scores

for all MT families are provided in appendix C). As in the VT experiment, time effects

41

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. were highly significant. Day was not significant in this analysis, although trial within day

was marginally significant.

Comparing MCP to Other Metrics

In contrast to the results for the VT experiment, there were no significant

differences between families for any of the univariate metrics (Table 2.3). This is also

very different from the results of the MCP analysis, as that metric successfully detected

significant behavioral differences between families when these other metrics did not.

Day was significant for copulation occurrence, as in the VT experiment. The effect of

trial within day was not significant for any analyses.

The Scree analysis from the factor analysis indicated two important factors

(eigenvalues >0.5, total variance explained: 78.4%). As in the VT experiment, wing

vibration, licking, and attempted copulation loaded strongly positively on Factor 1 (Table

2.4). However, Factor 2 is mainly influenced by positive loadings of following and wing

vibration and negative loadings of motionless and copulation. The difference in Factor 2

loadings from those seen in the VT experiment (where Factor 2 reflected a strong positive

loading of copulation) is likely due to rarity of copulations in the trials with previously

mated females. Also unlike the VT experiment, families did not differ significantly for

either Factor (Table 2.3). The only significant effect in the analysis of MT factors was a

significant effect of day on Factor 1. For a summary of results for MCP compared to

other analyses, refer to Table 2.5.

42

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Discussion

I found substantial genetic variation in male courtship behavior when males are

exposed either to virgin or to previously mated females. This is a rare demonstration of

natural genetic variation in male courtship behavior towards previously mated females in

Drosophila. Families differed mainly in the rate at which high levels of courtship

intensity are achieved in both mated- and virgin female experiments. The primary

difference between the two experiments is that in mated-female experiments, even the

families with the highest-intensity behavior did not have many individuals advance

beyond “licking” on the MCP scale. This result suggests that female receptivity is

necessary for males to progress to high levels of courtship intensity. Females in the MT

experiments had mated ~ 24 hours previous to their mating trial, and were likely not

receptive to mating (Markow 2002).

In the MT experiment, genetic variation in behavior reported here does not seem

to reflect variation in the occurrence of courtship conditioning (“Courtship conditioning”

refers to the substantial decrease in Drosophila male courtship behavior after extended

exposure to non-receptive females (Siegel and Hall 1979; Gailey et al. 1984; Reif et al.

2002)). Rather, there appears to be more variation in the rate of increase in courtship

intensity early in the mating trial than in the rate of decline in intensity after 20 minutes

(see Figure 2.2b). This could potentially correspond to variation in the ability of different

families to detect the presence of a female, receive and process stimuli, and respond

appropriately in a timely manner.

Although all behavioral trials were confined to two hours in the morning, and

environmental conditions for all males were highly controlled, I detected significant

43

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. among-day and within-day variation in behavior in both experiments. Among-day

variation suggests that subtle effects such as daily fluctuations in relative humidity in the

laboratory (not controlled in these experiments) can have significant effects on behavior.

Within-day effects suggest that circadian variation in male courtship behavior may be

occurring over quite short time scales. In fact, in both the virgin- and mated-female

target experiments, the average MCP score decreases by approximately 0.5 from trial 1

(approximately 8 AM-8:30 AM) to trial 4 (approximately 9:30 AM-10 AM), indicating a

decrease in courtship behavior in late morning. This demonstrates the importance of

accounting for the effects of environmental and circadian variation when performing

statistical analyses of behavior, even when efforts are made to experimentally control for

them.

My method appears robust for quantifying differences among individuals in the

pattern of courtship behavior in Drosophila. The MCP scale was more successful at

detecting these differences than other metrics, especially in the mated-female experiment

(Table 2.5). For example, although a metric such as “latency to copulation” can provide

insight into temporal patterns of behavior (Moehring and Mackay 2004; Mackay et al.

2005), it is not useful when very few males actually achieve copulation, as in our MT

experiment. This result highlights the importance of quantifying subtle differences in

courtship behavior. The MCP was the only metric able to detect significant variation for

males placed with mated females, which is usually what males would encounter in the

wild, and hence where variation in courtship influencing mating success may have a

significant impact on evolution of behavior.

44

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. One limitation of this new method is that the 30-second interval between

instantaneous scan sampling will miss some behavioral transitions. In Bastock and

Manning’s report of male courtship behavior (1955), orienting behavior had a durational

range of 6-15 seconds, while wing vibration had a range of 2-16 seconds. Markow and

Hanson (1981) reported that chasing behavior had a mean duration of 3 seconds.

However, the MCP method still successfully captures variation in behavioral patterns,

and can do so for a much larger number of flies than any other method that would capture

all of these transitions. This assay is straightforward and scalable to the large sample

sizes that are needed for genetic analysis.

This experiment reveals the importance of examining overall patterns of courtship

behavior as opposed to using summary metrics or focusing on specific behaviors from a

sequential series. Using the appropriate analysis of behavioral patterns allowed me to

detect significant genetic differences in courtship when other methods would have

detected no difference, especially in experiments with mated females as targets. This

method is also relatively easy to use and provides a detailed account of behavior in a

high-throughput assay. Using the MCP scale, I detected the presence of genetic variation

in behavior in a natural population using a sample size large enough to extend this to

behavior-genetic analysis. I can now use this information to move forward in the process

of understanding the genetics of courtship behavior by examining the molecular basis of

this variation, identifying why this variation is being maintained, and looking for

mechanisms which may be under sexual selection.

45

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Acknowledgements

I would like to thank the University of Illinois Program in Ecology, Evolution, and

Conservation and the Francis M. and Harlie M. Clark research grant for financial support

for this research. I would also like to thank M. M. Reedy and the members of the Hughes

lab for help with the flies, and R. M. Reynolds, J. M. Dmevich, A. L. Toth, A. M. Bell,

R. Fuller, H. M. Robertson, A. V. Suarez, and G. E. Robinson for their useful comments

on this manuscript.

List of References

Andersson MB (1994) Sexual Selection. Princeton, N. J.: Princeton University Press. Aspi J, Hoikkala A (1993) Laboratory and natural heritabilities of male courtship song characters in Drosophila montana and D. Littoralis. Heredity 70(4): 400-406. Balakireva M, Stocker RF, Gendre N, Ferveur JF (1998) Voila, a new Drosophila courtship variant that affects the nervous system: Behavioral, neural, and genetic characterization. Journal o f Neuroscience 18(11): 4335-4343. Bastock M, Manning A (1955) The courtship of Drosophila melanogaster. Behaviour 8: 7-110. Beaver LM, Giebultowicz JM (2004) Regulation of copulation duration by period and timeless in Drosophila melanogaster. Current Biology 14(16): 1492-1497. Boake CRB, Konigsberg L (1998) Inheritance of male courtship behavior, aggressive success, and body size in Drosophila silvestris. Evolution 52(5): 1487-1492. Bouletreau J (1978) Ovarian activity and reproductive potential in a natural population of Drosophila melanogaster. Oecologia (Berl) 35: 319-342. Casares P, Carracedo MC, Miguel ES, Pineiro R, Garciaflorez L (1993) Male mating speed in Drosophila-melanogaster - differences in genetic architecture and in relative performance according to female genotype. Behavior Genetics 23(4): 349-358.

46

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Drapeau MD, Long AD (2000) The Copulatron, a multi-chamber apparatus for observing Drosophila courtship behaviors. Drosophila Information Services 83: 194-196. Etges WJ (2002) Divergence in mate choice systems: Does evolution play by rules? Genetica 116(2-3): 151-166. Ferveur JF, Jallon JM (1993) nerd, a locus on chromosome-III, affects male reproductive-behavior in Drosophila-melanogaster. Naturwissenschaften 80(10): 474-475. Gailey DA, Jackson FR, Siegel RW (1984) Conditioning mutations in Drosophila- melanogaster affect an experience-dependent behavioral-modification in courting males. Genetics 106(4): 613-623. Gefen E, Marlon AJ, Gibbs AG (2006) Selection for desiccation resistance in adult Drosophila melanogaster affects larval development and metabolite accumulation. The Journal o f Experimental Biology 209: TBA. Gleason JM, Ritchie MG (2004) Do quantitative trait loci (QTL) for a courtship song difference between Drosophila simulans and D. sechellia coincide with candidate

genes and intraspecific QTL? Genetics 166(3): 1303-1311. Greenspan RJ, Ferveur JF (2000) Courtship in Drosophila. Annu Rev Genet 34: 205-232. Gromko MH (1987) Genetic constraint on the evolution of courtship behavior in Drosophila-melanogaster. Heredity 58: 435-441. Gromko MFI, Markow TA (1993) Courtship and remating in field populations of Drosophila. Animal Behavior 45(2): 253-262. Gromko MFI, Briot A, Jensen SC, Fukui HH (1991) Selection on copulation duration in Drosophila-melanogaster - predictability of direct response versus unpredictability of correlated response. Evolution 45(1): 69-81. Hall JC (1994) The mating of a fly. Science 264(5166): 1702-1714. Hoffmann AA (1999) Is the heritability for courtship and mating speed in Drosophila (fruit fly) low? Heredity 82: 151-157. Kulathinal RJ, Singh RS (2004) The nature of genetic variation in sex and reproduction- related genes among sibling species of the Drosophila melanogaster complex. Genetica 120(1-3): 245-252.

47

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Littell RC, Stroup WW, Freund RJ (2002) SAS for Linear Models, Fourth Edition. Cary, N. C.: SAS Institute Inc. Mackay TFC, Heinsohn SL, Lyman RF, Moehring AJ, Morgan TJ et al. (2005) Genetics and genomics of Drosophila mating behavior. Proceedings o f the National Academy o f Sciences o f the United States o f America 102: 6622-6629. Markow TA (2002) Perspective: Female remating, operational sex ratio, and the arena of sexual selection in Drosophila species. Evolution 56(9): 1725-1734. Markow TA, Hanson SJ (1981) Multivariate analysis of Drosophila courtship. Proceedings o f the National Academy o f Sciences o f the United States o f America 78(1): 430-434. Markow TA, Castrezana S, Pfeiler E (2002) Flies across the water: Genetic

differentiation and reproductive isolation in allopatric desert Drosophila. Evolution 56(3): 546-552. Martin P, Bateson P (1993) Measuring Behaviour: An introductory guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Moehring A, Mackay T (2004) The quantitative genetic basis of male mating behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics 167(3): 1249-1263. Partridge L, Hoffmann A, Jones JS (1987) Male size and mating success in Drosophila- melanogaster and Drosophila-pseudoobscura under field conditions. Animal Behaviour 35: 468-476. Reif M, Linsenmair KE, Heisenberg M (2002) Evolutionary significance of courtship conditioning in Drosophila melanogaster. Animal Behaviour 63(Part 1): 143-155. Ritchie MG, Kyriacou CP (1994) Genetic-variability of courtship song in a population of Drosophila-melanogaster. Animal Behaviour 48(2): 425-434. Ritchie MG, Gleason JM (1995) Rapid evolution of courtship song pattern in Drosophila-willistoni sibling species. J Evolution Biol 8(4): 463-479. Ryner LC, Goodwin SF, Castrillon DH, Anand A, Villella A et al. (1996) Control of male sexual behavior and sexual orientation in Drosophila by thefruitless gene. Cell 87(6): 1079-1089. SAS Institute (2006) SAS online documentation v. 9.1. Available: http://support.sas.com/91doc/docMainpage.isp. Accessed 2006.

48

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Sawamura K, Tomaru M (2002) Biology of reproductive isolation in Drosophila: Toward a better understanding of speciation. Population Ecology 44(3): 209-219. Service P, Rose M (1985) Genetic covariation among life-history components- the effect of novel environments. Evolution 39(4): 943-945. Siegel RW, Hall JC (1979) Conditioned responses in courtship behavior of normal and mutant Drosophila. Proceedings o f the National Academy o f Sciences o f the United States o f America 76(7): 3430-3434. Stamenkovicradak M, Partridge L, Andjelkovic M (1992) A genetic correlation between the sexes for mating speed in Drosophila-melanogaster. Animal Behaviour 43(3): 389-396. Stevens J (1992) Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences. Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates. Stirling DG, Reale D, Roff DA (2002) Selection, structure and the heritability of behaviour. J Evolution Biol 15: 277-289. Yamamoto D, Nakano Y (1998) Genes for sexual behavior. Biochemical & Biophysical Research Communications 246(1): 1-6. Zar JH (1996) Biostatistical Analysis. Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Prentice Hall.

49

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. TABLES

Table 2.1. Male Courtship Progression Scale

Score Behavior

1 Motionless

2 M oving

3 Oriented toward female

4 Following/chasing female

5 Wing vibration

6 Licking genitalia

7 Attempted copulation

8 Copulation

50

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 0.015 67.58 (48) 0.174 0.033 16.39(12) 35.3 (1) Effect 17.39(1) 10.45(1) 49.28 (14) 71.61 (48) <0.0001 <0.0001 Family Family*Time Family*(Time)2 Time Time*Time Day Trial(Day) 0.633 0.012 0.042 67.72 (77) 105.12 (79) 97.74 (79) 70.26 (75) 108.9 (78) 100.9 (78) 84.27 (1) 2.2. Generalized linear model analysis ofMCP VT ExperimentVT P-valueMTExperiment 0.766P-value 0.026 0.075 <0.0001 0.001 <0.0001 X2 (df) X(df) Table

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Table 2.3. Statistical tests for univariate and multivariate models

E ffect1

Family Day Trial(Day)2

Trait

VT Experiment

Cl 2.0 (77,451) *** 1.81 (14,451)* 0.94 (48,451)

Copulation Occurrence2 105.8 (79) * 17.4(14) 86.5 (48) **

Copulation Duration 1.5 (77,374) * 1.89(14,374)* 1.31 (48,374)

Latency to Copulation 1.4 (77,450) * 2.24(14,450) ** 1.22 (48,450)

Factor 1 1.0 (77,451) 0.74(14,451) 0.97 (48,451)

Factor 2 1.8 (77,451)** 2.22 (14,451)* 1.28 (48,451)

MT Experiment

Cl 0.98 (75,388) 1.46(12,388) 0.92 (48,388)

Copulation Occurrence 88.3 (78) 34.3 (12) *** 50.3 (48)

Copulation Duration 1.1 (2,5) 0.20 (3,5) N/A3

Factor 1 0.94 (75,388) 1.85 (12,388)* 1.02 (48,388)

Factor 2 1.2 (75,388) 0.89 (12,388) 0.98 (48,388)

* p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

1 - Family, day, and trial(day) effects tested using Type III F-statistics

2 - Copulation occurrence tested usingy2

3 - Model possessed infinite likelihood with trial(day) effect;

52

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Table 2.4. Multifactorial factor pattern analysis

VT Experiment

Behavior Factor 1 Factor 2

Motionless -0.4806 -0.7057

Moving 0.0149 -0.3832

Orienting 0.3151 -0.4454

Following 0.405 0.0437

Wing Vibration 0.8235 -0.187

Licking 0.8608 -0.2143

Attempted Copulation 0.8271 -0.0622

Copulation -0.2886 0.9413

MT Experiment

Behavior Factor 1 Factor 2

Motionless -0.6645 -0.4949

Moving -0.6604 0.3268

Orienting -0.0009 0.1167

Following -0.0563 0.6823

Wing Vibration 0.648 0.5704

Licking 0.7381 0.1466

Attempted Copulation 0.6117 -0.2621

Copulation 0.0063 -0.5013

53

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Table 2.5. Summary of success of MCP and other methods for detecting presence of

genetic variation

Method VT Experiment MT Experiment

MCP * *

Cl Proxy *** N.S.

Copulation Occurrence * N.S.

Copulation Duration * N/A

Latency to Copulation * N/A

Factor Pattern 1/ Factor Pattern 2 N.S./** N.S./N.S.

* p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** pO.OOl

54

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. FIGURES

Figure 2.1. Courtship observation device modeled after Drapeau and Long (2000). Top

row of device shows male and female chambers separated by thin opaque sheet. Bottom

row of device shows full mating arenas comprised of both male and female chambers.

Smaller side chambers are filled with media to prevent dessication during trials.

55

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Figure 2.2. As an example of male courtship progression, average male courtship progression score is plotted over course of trial for four “High MCP” and four “Low MCP” families (a) placed with virgin female targets and (b) placed with mated female targets. A total of eighty families were analyzed for each experiment. Only the first fifteen minutes of each trial were used in generalized linear model analysis of MCP (see text).

a.

o L _ _------0.5 3.5 6,5 9.5 12,5 15,5 18,5 21,5 24,5 27.5 30

Time (minutes)

b.

Family

Time (minutes)

56

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Chapter 3: Alteration in expression of courtship genes due to

age and experience in maleDrosophila melanogaster

Abstract

Differences in gene expression can be one target of selection. Genes can exhibit static

expression, where baseline expression levels do not change over the course of adult life.

They may also exhibit dynamic expression, where expression levels change in response

to a non-genotypic factor; for example, age or experience. Here I examine whether genes

previously identified through mutation screening as being associated with male courtship

behavior in the fruit fly,Drosophila melanogaster, have static or dynamic expression in

the heads of genetically identical flies placed in different age/experience scenarios. This

information can give us an understanding of the complexities of expression for genes

potentially involved in variation of male behavior. For this genotype, I found that 16

genes were statically expressed, and 9 genes were dynamically expressed with changes in

response to age. No genes exhibited dynamic expression changes due to experience or

age*experience interactions.

Introduction

Evolution via alterations in gene expression and mutations in regulatory regions

has been proposed as a major contributor to phenotypic variation between species (King

and Wilson 1975). Differences in gene expression both between and within species show

evidence of positive and stabilizing selection (Ferea et al. 1999; Oleksiak et al. 2002;

57

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Rifkin et al. 2003; but see Khaitovich et al. 2004). Substantial variation in expression has

been identified within populations (Oleksiak et al. 2002; Wayne et al. 2004; Hughes et al.

2006); therefore, changes in gene expression could be an underlying cause of phenotypic

variation within populations. One such phenotype is behavior, which can be critical for

individual fitness.

Alteration of the expression of a gene can be one target of selection (Harris-

Warrick 2000; Insel and Young 2000); and in fact, several cases of variation in behavior

can be attributed to differential gene expression. Both baseline differences in statically

expressed genes between individuals and changes in dynamically expressed genes can be

important for the evolution of behavior. One of the most well characterized examples is

theforaging gene. In the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster, theforaging gene has two

alleles, f o f and fo rs, which correspond to the rover and sitter foraging phenotypes of

both larvae and adults. The rover allele results in consistently higher mRNA levels of the

fo r gene and higher levels of PKG activity than the sitter allele, in both the juvenile and

adult phases. These differences in mRNA abundance and PKG activity are static, but are

under active selective pressure depending on the ecological conditions that the population

is experiencing (Fitzpatrick et al. 2007).

Like statically expressed genes, dynamically expressed genes can also affect

behavior with important fitness consequences. The ortholog of the foraging gene in the

honeybee, Apis mellifera, is again a well-characterized example. In A. mellifera,

individuals go through a nursing stage during which they care for the hive and brood.

Typically, at 2-3 weeks of age, an individual will transition from nursing to foraging

tasks. Expression of the ortholog to the D. melanogaster gene foraging, Amfor, is

58

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. significantly higher in foraging individuals than in nurses. In addition, increased PKG

activity causes accelerated transition from nurse to forager (Ben-Shahar et al. 2002).

These dynamic expression changes are integral to the social roles of individuals, and thus

are crucial for the fitness of the hive.

Another example of selection on a dynamically expressed gene is in the cichlid,

Haplochromis burtoni. In this species, the expression of a gene for gonadotropin-

releasing hormone ( GnRH l ) is socially regulated. Dynamic changes in the expression of

GnRHl influence the switch from aggressive to submissive behaviors associated with

social status; these changes in expression can affect the reproductive success of the fish

(White et al. 2002).

Because selection can act on both statically and dynamically expressed genes, a

complete understanding of how gene expression can cause variation in behavior requires

knowledge of how genes are regulated by changes in environment or physiology due to

age or experience. To examine the effects of age and experience on changes in gene

expression, I used male courtship behavior in Drosophila melanogaster , which is one of

the best characterized behavioral patterns in a model organism. Male courtship in the

fruit fly consists of a suite of complex behaviors that are genetically modulated (Hall

1994b). Mutation screening has revealed many genes that are necessary for normal

courtship. However, the alleles produced by this process usually result in null mutations

that have many pleiotropic effects in addition to the disruption of normal courtship

behavior, and alleles with such severe mutations are unlikely to persist at an appreciable

frequency in natural populations (Hall 1994a; Greenspan 1997a).

59

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Genes that have been identified by mutant screening are known to influence male

courtship behavior, but their temporal expression patterns are often unknown.

Understanding the expression pattern of these genes (further referred to as courtship

deficient, or CD genes) in response to age and environment is critical to investigating

their contributions to the natural variation in male courtship behavior. Here, I was

interested in changes in gene expression of CD genes in the head of adult male flies due

to age or experience. In this situation, if the expression of a CD gene changed either due

to age (temporal regulation) or experience (environmental regulation), I considered it

dynamically expressed. I was particularly interested in up- or down-regulation of CD

genes in reproductively mature males that had experienced courtship; changes in gene

expression only in this age/experience category would indicate that the CD gene

specifically responds to courtship. If a CD gene did not change expression due to age or

experience, I considered it statically expressed. Static expression could indicate that the

CD gene is involved in the development and/or maintenance of the organism. However,

CD genes that appear to be statically expressed in this experiment might have spatially

dynamic expression (for instance, tissue-specific expression), dynamic expression in

response to repeated exposure to females, dynamic expression resulting from copulation,

etc. Thus, I cannot extend my claim of static expression to scenarios outside these

particular age/experience combinations.

I conducted an experiment to investigate age- and experience-dependent

regulation of gene expression involved with courtship. Age and experience are key

factors in male courtship success (Pitnick et al. 1995; Reif et al. 2002), but it is not

known how expression of genes originally identified by mutation screening changes with

60

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. either variable. There is evidence for alterations of neural physiology due to age or

experience (Strambi et al. 1999; Devaud et al. 2003), and for genetic variation in age-

related male reproductive physiological maturity (Pitnick et al. 1995; Promislow and

Bugbee 2000). Therefore, age and experience do have profound effects on the expression

of genes related to these phenotypes (which may also feasibly be related to courtship

behavior). For CD genes, it would be not be unreasonable to expect expression

differences related to age, experience, or a combination of those factors. I found that age,

but not experience, altered the expression of 9 CD genes, and that the other 16 genes

examined were statically expressed.

Methods

Flies

Flies used in this study were derived from the New Jersey (NJ) stock of D.

melanogaster. This stock originated from 8,000 offspring of 4,000 wild-caught females

collected in Terhune, New Jersey, in 1999 by Valerie Pierce. The stock was maintained

in a large, randomly mating outbred population in the laboratory of Dr. Allan Gibbs. We

obtained 500 of these flies in 2003, where they have been maintained at a population size

of approximately 12,000 individuals with overlapping generations.

To create isogenic lines, virgin females were randomly chosen from the NJ base

stock, and the X, II, and III chromosomes from these flies were extracted by Dr. Jenny

Dmevich using balancer chromosomes and standard Drosophila crossing schemes

(Greenspan 1997b). The balancers used in creation of these lines were FM7a and

T(2;3)A1-W, In(2L)Cy, In(2R)Cy, Cy1 L1: TM2/T(2;3)UbxB18, In(2LR)bwvl, bwvl Sb'.

61

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Using microsatellite analysis, we confirmed that the lines used in this study were

homozygous at 7 highly variable microsatellite loci spread out across the genome.

Two of the isogenic NJ lines (17 and 77) were crossed to produce genetically

identical, non-inbred males for this experiment. Experimental flies were reared under

controlled density to minimize maternal and common-environment effects. Virgin

females from line 17 were crossed to males from line 77 in plastic bottles containing 25

flies of each sex. First instar larvae were collected from each of 30 different bottles on

agar plates supplied with yeast paste. Two groups of 25 larvae were collected per bottle,

and each group was transferred to a rearing vial supplied with standard commeal media.

The line 17 x line 77 cross produced progeny that were genetically identical, but not

inbred, for the majority of the genome.

Age and experience categories

My aim was to investigate gene expression differences due to age and experience.

Hence, I examined gene expression differences in flies from four categories, representing

two different ages and two different levels of experience. Age/experience categories

included: 3-day old, reproductively mature males (Pitnick et al. 1995) with courtship

experience (mature and experienced, “ME”) or without courtship experience (mature but

naive, “MN”); and 1-1.5 hour old, immature males who had been exposed to mature

females (immature having encountered a female, “IE”) or who had no experience with

females (immature and nai've, “IN”). In the NJ population, reproductively immature

males can begin to exhibit courtship behavior as early as two hours post-eclosion

(E.A.R., personal observation); however, their degree of progression through the

62

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. courtship behaviors or their subsequent success with females at this age has not been

determined.

To control for non-experimental environmental or circadian differences, tissue

from flies for all categories was collected simultaneously, within a single half-hour

period on a single day. This collection was made at 9:30 AM (2 hours after the light

cycle begins for our stocks), when genes exhibiting circadian expression are relatively

stable (Leloup and Goldbeter 2000) and flies are active (Rosato et al. 1997). Males for

the ME and MN categories were collected as virgins 0-1 hours post-eclosion from the

constant larval density vials on Day 1 using light CO 2 anesthesia, and were housed

individually so that they had no contact with other flies. On Day 2, ME males were

lightly anesthetized with CO2 and placed into the bottom chambers of a mating device.

Previously-mated females from an outbred stock of flies with ebony body coloration were

placed into the top chambers, and the male and female chambers were separated by an

opaque plastic sheet. Previously-mated females were chosen because they are less

receptive to male courtship (Markow 1996), and thus males should be able to experience

courtship, but not copulation. Four devices with ten male chambers each were set up in

this manner, and were placed in incubators overnight so the flies could recover from

anesthesia (N=40). MN males were kept isolated (N=44).

On Day 3 ,1 collected newly emerged virgin males (0-1 hours post eclosion) for

the immature categories using light CO 2 anesthesia. These males were placed

immediately into individual vials (IN males, N=44) or into vials containing one

previously-mated female from the ebony stock (IE males, N=44). As soon as this was

done, the opaque plastic sheet separating the ME males from their females was removed.

63

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. The ME males and IE males were allowed to interact with a female for 30 minutes. 30

minutes was chosen because variation in male courtship progression can already be

detected after a 30-minute observational period (See Chapter 2). If any ME males were

observed copulating, they were not included in further analysis, since I did not want gene

expression differences due to courtship experience to become confounded with

differences due to copulation. After this 30-minute period, all males from all categories

were frozen using dry ice.

ME males endured one additional exposure to light C 02 anesthesia than the other

categories. A significant age*experience effect for a CD gene could thus reflect

expression changes due to C 02 exposure. However, no effect of age*experience was

identified, and thus I do not think this was a confounding factor.

Gene expression

I identified 25 CD genes using both a literature search and a query of FlyBase

(Drysdale et al. 2005). From FlyBase, I extracted 65 genes matching the Gene Ontology

(www.geneontology.org) category of “male courtship behavior” and daughter categories

such as “courtship song”. Using the extensive literature (including Hall 1994b;

Fitzpatrick 2004; Moehring and Mackay 2004), I narrowed this list to 25 genes for which

expression in adult male heads has been documented. I limited this experiment to

expression in the head because I wanted to narrow my focus on genes involved in

nervous system function. The CD genes, their molecular functions, their role in male

courtship behavior, and key citations are listed in Table 3.1. If genes showed no

significant change in expression between any of the age/experience categories, I

64

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. considered them static. If genes displayed significant changes in expression between age

groups, between males housed individually or with females, or with any age/experience

interaction, I considered them dynamic.

I used quantitative real-time PCR to measure abundance of mRNA for each CD

gene in males from each age-experience category. Forward and reverse primer sets were

designed for each of the 25 CD genes using Primer Express software v2.0 (Applied

Biosystems) with default “Taqman Primer and Probe Sets” settings, or using published

qPCR primers (transformer-2 and doublesex Tarone et al. 2005) (Table 3.2). If a gene

had known male-specific exons or transcripts, primers were designed to specifically

amplify those. Primers were checked for specificity using NCBI BLAST searches.

Heads were dissected from the frozen flies in each age/experience category.

Heads from four individual flies were pooled to yield sufficient total RNA collected for

analysis The number of independent RNA pools assayed (biological replicates) was:

MN=11; ME=9; IN= 11; IE=11. Total RNA was extracted using PicoPure RNA

extraction kits (Arcturus), using the manufacturer’s protocol including treatment with

DNasel (Qiagen). RNA was quantified and checked for purity using a spectrophotometer

(Nanodrop) at 260nm. All samples had a 260/280 nm ratio of 1.8 or greater. 200 ng

RNA was reverse transcribed using a mixture of 2pL lOx first strand Arrayscript buffer

(Ambion), lpL lOmM dNTP mix (Applied Biosystems), 0.2pL RNase inhibitor (Applied

Biosystems), and 0.2pL 200U/pL Arrayscript (Ambion) in DEPC-treated water. As an

exogenous control, I spiked lOng of plant Root Cap Protein I cRNA (RCP1, obtained

from Dr. Thomas Newman) into each reaction. Reactions were incubated at 42°C for 60

minutes, then at 95°C for 5 minutes. The cDNA from one RT reaction could be used to

65

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. complete qRT-PCR reactions for 9-10 genes. Therefore, three separate RT reactions

were performed for each biological replicate in order to yield enough cDNA to complete

qRT-PCR reactions for all 25 genes.

For each gene, a mixture of 5 pL 2x SYBR Green Master Mix (Applied

Biosystems), 1 pL lOpM F primer, and lpLlO pM R primer was added to 3 pL of 6x

diluted cDNA from each sample. This reaction was performed in triplicate for each

sample (technical replicates). Specific transcripts from each sample were quantified

using the ABI Prism HT7900 sequence detection system (Applied Biosystems). Each

reaction was performed with the default PCR cycle settings for 40 cycles, and a

dissociation curve was added to the final cycle to confirm the absence of primer-dimers

for each gene. A five-fold log-scale dilution standard curve was generated for each gene

using D. melanogaster genomic DNA in order to determine absolute quantification for

each sample. A qRT-PCR reaction for the exogenous RCP1 control gene was performed

for each of the three separate RT reactions used to generate cDNA. This allowed for

standardization of the absolute quantity of expression of the CD genes by accounting for

differences between samples in the RT reactions. A five-fold log-scale dilution standard

curve was generated for RCP1 using RPC1 cRNA.

Statistical Analyses

To standardize the absolute quantities of cDNA for each gene (determined from

the standard curve, in nanograms) I used the exogenous RCP1 cRNA control to account

for differences in efficiency of reverse transcription between samples (for details about

RCP1 and molecular methods, see above). This standardization was accomplished by

66

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. dividing the raw absolute quantity of a gene (in nanograms) for each sample by the raw

absolute quantity of RCP1 (in nanograms) for that sample. After standardization with the

exogenous control, all expression data were log-transformed to normalize the residuals in

order to meet the necessary assumptions for the statistical tests.

To evaluate the overall effects of age and experience, I performed a multivariate

analysis of variance (MANOVA) on expression of all genes simultaneously using R

statistical software v. 2.5 (R Development Core Team, 2007). I tested for the effects of

age, experience, and their interaction as in the ANOVA for individual genes. I used the

Pillai trace statistic to approximate the Type-III F-statistic in my MANOVA, which uses

eigenvalues to compare the error sums of squares to the sums of squares of the

hypotheses (Johnson 1998).

To examine the ability of expression profiles to appropriately classify flies into

the age/experience categories, I used a linear discriminant analysis (Everitt 2005). The

linear discriminant analysis is equally as effective as alternative parametric and non-

parametric versions of classification analyses (Meyer et al. 2002), especially in cases

where only a few canonical factors explain most of the variation (Johnson 1998). In

addition, it can allow the researcher to interpret variables and visualize distances between

populations on a plane (Johnson 1998), which many other classification methods cannot

do. To determine the number of significant linear discriminant factors in my expression

data, I used SAS PROC CANDISC (SAS Institute, 2007). The eigenvalues showed that

LD1 was highly significant (F75_28= 5.62; P<0.0001), while LD2 was marginally non­

significant (F48> 2o= 1.88; P= 0.062) and LD3 was not significant (F23,u=1.37; p=0.3).

Thus, any discussion of LD3 should be interpreted with caution.

67

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. I next performed linear discriminant analyses using the “Ida” function in R

(MASS package). I then used the “predict” function to classify individual flies based on

resubstitution using the linear discriminate scores for all three discriminant factors, and

cross-validated the classification using the “CV” command in the “Ida” function. CV

performs a leave-one-out cross-validation of the ability of the LD loading scores to

accurately predict the class of each individual, and is a more conservative estimate than

resubstitution. Finally, I visualized the expression patterns of the age/experience

categories using single-factor hierarchical clustering on normalized data with Genesis

software (Stum et al. 2002).

To determine the significance of effects of age, experience, and the interaction

between these two factors on the expression of individual genes, I performed a fixed-

effect ANOVA (SAS PROC MIXED, SAS Institute, 2007) on the normalized data. Each

gene was fit to a model: y = p + a + x + a*x + e, where y was the log-transformed

standardized quantity of cDNA for that a particular sample, a was the fixed effect of age,

x was the fixed effect of experience, a*x was the interaction effect between age and

experience, and e was the random error (Type III SS). False discovery rate (FDR) was

calculated using the method described in Benjamini and Hochberg (1995) for age,

experience, and age*experience to control for multiple hypothesis tests.

Results

The MANOVA showed a highly significant effect of age, a non-significant effect

of experience, and a marginally non-significant interaction of age*experience on gene

expression differences between the age/experience categories (Table 3.3). The linear

68

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. discriminant analysis yielded three linear discriminate factors. LD1 accounted for 93.3%

of the total variance, while LD2 and LD3 accounted for 4.8% and 1.9%, respectively (for

linear discriminant loadings see Table 3.4).

Plotting the linear discriminant scores for individuals in each age/experience

category revealed that the majority of the variation in LD1 is explained by differences in

gene expression related to age (Figure 3.1 and 3.2). However, LD1 also appears to be

using variation due to both age and age*experience to appropriately classify flies into

categories. Genes with positive LD1 loading scores represent either higher expression in

mature males, or a more complex interaction of age*experience. Genes with negative

LD1 loading scores tend to represent higher expression in immature males; in fact, the

genes with the largest negative LD1 scores all have higher expression in immature

categories. LD2 appears to discriminate between MN/IE males and ME/IN males,

indicating that certain genes are expressed differently depending on interactions between

age and experience (Figure 3.1). Finally, LD3 discriminates between expression patterns

of males who have and have not encountered females (Figure 3.2). LD2 and LD3 should

be interpreted cautiously, as they account for very little of the overall variance in gene

expression. Predicting classes of individuals using resubstitution of the linear

discriminate scores had an extremely high success rate (ME 100%; MN 100%; IE 100%;

IN 88.9%). The more conservative cross-validation of the linear discriminate

classification yielded similar results for the mature age groups (ME 71.4%; MN 70%),

but was less reliable for the immature age groups (IE 54.5%; IN 44.4%). The combined

resubstitution and cross-validation results confirm that the linear discriminant loadings

69

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. are appropriately classifying individuals based on their genes expression profiles in the

majority of instances.

The single factor hierarchical clustering showed a tree separating experimental

groups first by age, then by experience (Figure 3.3). There were three interesting gene

clusters. Genes that had higher expression in immature vs. mature males had a distinct

cluster, while genes that had higher expression in mature vs. immature males separated

into two other clusters. One cluster grouped genes with differences mostly due to age,

much like the immature v. mature male cluster. The other cluster exhibited subtle

expression differences between categories that were not associated with just age or

experience, perhaps picking up on the marginally non-significant effect of the

age*experience interaction.

Analysis of variance on individual genes revealed that age had a significant effect

on expression of nine of the genes, but that there were no significant effects of experience

or age*experience interaction before correction for multiple tests (Table 3.5; raw means

and standard deviations for each CD gene are displayed in Figure 3.4). The genes

exhibiting static expression do not show appear to show communality, i.e. they do not

have uniformly lower (or higher) absolute expression levels than the significant genes

(log-transformed expression levels for all genes is reported in appendix D). Therefore,

the lack of effect of age, experience, or a combination of both is probably not due to a

lack of statistical power or unreliable quantification of genes with low (or high)

expression.

70

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Discussion

I found that courtship deficient genes are both statically and dynamically

expressed. Nine CD genes contributing to normal male courtship behavior had altered

temporal expression during the first three days of adult life. This was apparent whether I

analyzed genes individually or as a group. Whether or not a male had encountered (or

even courted) a female did not significantly alter gene expression, although the

MANOVA and linear discriminant analysis demonstrated a slight trend indicating that

the interaction of age and experience might alter the expression of several of the genes.

My results indicate that these genes do not exhibit dynamic expression due to the

immediate effects of experience with a female. However, it is possible that some of these

genes may exhibit altered expression several hours after encountering a female, or after

repeated exposure to a female.

The genes that are contributing most heavily to the age-related variation in

expression are: dsx, eg, fru, K r-hl, pie, pros, tko, to, andy. These genes exhibit dynamic

temporal expression, but do not all change in the same direction or magnitude. Three of

these genes are down-regulated in later ages (eg, pie, y); this could indicate that they are

important in early adult development, or that their down-regulation is necessary for

normal adult function, y and pie are involved in pigmentation (reviewed in Wittkopp et

al. 2003), which perhaps accounts for their heightened expression in immature males.

Those genes that are up-regulated in mature males (dsx, fru, Kr-hl, pros, tko, to)

may require increased expression for normal courtship behavior. Many of these genes

are transcription factors, indicating that there may be genes downstream of these CD

genes that are also involved in male courtship. Three of these genes, dsx, fru . and to, are

71

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. part of the sex determination cascade, most of which is completed by adulthood

(reviewed in Greenspan and Ferveur 2000; also see Dauwalder et al. 2002). However,

for each of these genes, expression has been found in heads of wild-type adult males (fru:

Ryner et al. 1996; to: Dauwalder et al. 2002; dsx: Lee et al. 2002). Presence of

expression in adults, which I have now determined to by dynamically altered by age,

supports the idea that fru, dsx, and to exhibit as yet unknown pleitropic effects during

adulthood.

The sixteen remaining genes were statically expressed across age and experience

level. It is possible that subtle differences in expression of these genes within individuals

does not affect courtship behavior, but rather it is the absence of a normal protein product

during development and/or in adulthood that alters male courtship (hence the

identification of the gene using mutation screening procedures). The static expression of

these genes could indicate that they are functionally constrained. Although these genes

are statically expressed, differences in baseline levels of expression in individual flies

may have a profound effect on courtship behavior. Additional testing on individuals with

different genetic backgrounds is necessary to examine the possibility that different

genotypes have different baseline expression for these static genes. Additionally, as

discussed before, these genes may exhibit dynamic expression under different

circumstances or in different tissues.

Courtship deficient genes involved in male courtship behavior in D. melanogaster

appear to be mainly statically expressed. This could perhaps be related to constraints in

expression of genes involved with nervous system function (i.e. Galis 1999; Smith 2006);

however, genes expressed in the brain that exhibit altered expression associated with

72

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. behavioral modification are less constrained than originally thought (i.e. Whitfield et al.

2003; Velarde et al. 2006). The genes that were dynamically expressed changed with age

and perhaps corresponded to the physiological, reproductive maturity of the males; it

would be interesting to see how reproductive maturity alters male courtship behavior.

Finally, there does not appear to be immediate dynamic changes in expression related to

exposure to stimuli from a female or from courtship itself. Thus, for these CD genes,

experience with a female does not have an immediate effect on expression. It is possible

that genotypes other than this one could have stronger age*experience changes in

expression; if so, and if this contributes to phenotypic differences, it could be a target of

selection related to phenotypic plasticity.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank the National Science Foundation (NSF DEB 0296177), University

of Illinois Program in Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, the Animal Biology Banks

Award, and the Francis M. and Harlie M. Clark Research Grant for funding for this

project. Thanks to G. E. Robinson for the use of his laboratory and equipment. I am

indebted to F. E. Miguez and A. L. Toth for their description of linear discriminant

analysis and R code. I would especially like to thank M. M. Reedy for her help with the

flies, T. Newman for his help with qRT-PCR troubleshooting, and G. Davis for help with

primer design. Thanks to G. E. Robinson, H. M. Robertson, A. V. Suarez, A. L. Toth and

R. M. Reynolds for their helpful commentson this manuscript.

73

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. List of References

Anholt RRH, Mackay TFC (2004) Quantitative genetic analyses of complex behaviours in Drosophila. Nat Rev Genet 5(11): 838-849. Baker BS, Taylor BJ, Hall JC (2001) Are complex behaviors specified by dedicated regulatory genes? Reasoning from Drosophila. Cell 105(1): 13-24. Ben-Shahar Y, Robichon A, Sokolowski MB, Robinson GE (2002) Influence of gene action across different time scales on behavior. Science 296(5568): 741-744. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal o f the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological) 57(1): 289-300. Billeter JC, Goodwin SF, O'Dell KMC (2002) Genes mediating sex-specific behaviors in Drosophila. Advances in genetics, vol 47. San Diego: Academic Press Inc. pp. 87-116.

Dauwalder B, Tsujimoto S, Moss J, Mattox W (2002) The Drosophila takeout gene is regulated by the somatic sex-determination pathway and affects male courtship behavior. Gene Dev 16(22): 2879-2892. Devaud JM, Acebes A, Ramaswami M, Ferrus A (2003) Structural and functional changes in the olfactory pathway of adult Drosophila take place at a critical age. J Neurobiol 56(1): 13-23. Diagana TT, Thomas U, Prokopenko SN, Xiao B, Worley PF et al. (2002) Mutation of Drosophila homer disrupts control of locomotor activity and behavioral plasticity. Journal o f Neuroscience 22(2): 428-436.

Drapeau MD (2003) A novel hypothesis on the biochemical role of the Drosophila yellow protein. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications 311(1): 1-3. Drapeau MD, Cyran SA, Viering MM, Geyer PK, Long AD (2006) A cw-regulatory sequence within theyellow locus o f Drosophila melanogaster required for normal male mating success. Genetics 172(2): 1009-1030. Drysdale R, Crosby M, Consortium TF (2005) Flybase: Genes and gene models. Nucleic Acids Research 33: D390-D395 http://flvbase.org/.

74

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Emmons SW, Lipton J (2003) Genetic basis of male sexual behavior. J Neurobiol 54(1): 93-110. Everitt B (2005) An R and S-Plus companion to multivariate analysis', Casella G, Fienberg S, Olkin I, editors. London: Springer-Verlag London Limited. Ferea TL, Botstein D, Brown PO, Rosenzweig RF (1999) Systematic changes in gene expression patterns following adaptive evolution in yeast. Proceedings of the National Academy o f Sciences o f the United States o f America 96(17): 9721-9726. Fitzpatrick M (2004) Pleiotropy and the genomic location of sexually selected genes. American Naturalist 163(6): 800-808. Fitzpatrick MJ, Feder E, Rowe L, Sokolowski MB (2007) Maintaining a behaviour polymorphism by frequency-dependent selection on a single gene. Nature 447(7141): 210-U215. Gaines P, Tompkins L, Woodard CT, Carlson JR (2000) quick-to-court, a Drosophila mutant with elevated levels of sexual behavior, is defective in a predicted coiled- coil protein. Genetics 154(4): 1627-1637. Galis F (1999) Why do almost all mammals have seven cervical vertebrae? Developmental constraints, hox genes, and cancer. Journal o f Experimental Zoology 285(1): 19-26. Greenspan RJ (1997a) A kinder, gentler genetic analysis of behavior: Dissection gives way to modulation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 7(6): 805-811. Greenspan RJ (1997b) Fly pushing: The theory and practice o f drosophila genetics. Plainview, N.Y.: Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press. Greenspan RJ, Ferveur JF (2000) Courtship in Drosophila. AnnuRev Genet 34: 205-232. Grosjean Y, Savy M, Soichot J, Everaerts C, Cezilly F et al. (2004) Mild mutations in the pan neural gene prospero affect male-specific behaviour in Drosophila melanogaster. Behav Process 65(1): 7-13. Grozinger CM, Sharabash NM, Whitfield CW, Robinson GE (2003) Pheromone- mediated gene expression in the honey bee brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100: 14519-14525.

75

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Hall JC (1994a) Pleiotropy o f behavioral genes. In: Greenspan RJ, Kyriacou CP, editors. Flexability and constraint in behavioral systems: John Wiley & Sons, LTD. pp. 15-27. Hall JC (1994b) The mating of a fly. Science 264(5166): 1702-1714. Harris-Warrick RM (2000) Ion channels and receptors: Molecular targets for behavioral evolution. J Comp Physiol A 186(7-8): 605-616. Hughes KA, Ayroles JF, Reedy MM, Dmevich JM, Rowe KC et al. (2006) Segregating variation in the transcriptome: Cis regulation and additivity of effects. Genetics 173(3): 1347-1364. Insel TR, Young LJ (2000) Neuropeptides and the evolution of social behavior. Curr Opin Neurobiol 10(6): 784-789. Johnson DE (1998) Applied multivariate methods for data analysts. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company. Khaitovich P, Weiss G, Lachmann M, Hellmann I, Enard W et al. (2004) A neutral model of transcriptome evolution. Plos Biology 2(5): 682-689. King M-C, Wilson AC (1975) Evolution at two levels in humans and chimpanzees. Science 188(4184): 107-116. Lam BJ, Bakshi A, Ekinci FY, Webb J, Graveley BR et al. (2003) Enhancer-dependent 5' -splice site control of fruitless pre-mRNA splicing. Journal o f Biological Chemistry 278(25): 22740-22747. Lee G, Hall JC, Park JH (2002) doublesex gene expression in the central nervous system of Drosophila melanogaster. J Neurogenet 16(4): 229-248. Leloup JC, Goldbeter A (2000) Modeling the molecular regulatory mechanism of circadian rhythms inDrosophila. Bioessays 22(1): 84-93. Markow TA (1996) Evolution o f drosophila mating systems. In: Hecht MKM, R. J.; Clegg, M. T., editor. Evolutionary biology. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 73-106. Meyer D, Leisch F, Homik K (2002)Benchmarking support vector machines. Vienna, AU: Vienna University of Economics and Business Administration. 78. Moehring A, Mackay T (2004) The quantitative genetic basis of male mating behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics 167(3): 1249-1263.

76

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Oleksiak MF, Churchill GA, Crawford DL (2002) Variation in gene expression within and among natural populations. Nature Genetics 32(2): 261-266. Orgad S, Rosenfeld G, Greenspan RJ, Segal D (2000) courtless, the Drosophila ubc7

homolog, is involved in male courtship behavior and spermatogenesis. Genetics 155(3): 1267-1280. Pitnick S, Markow TA, Spicer GS (1995) Delayed male maturity is a cost of producing large sperm in Drosophila. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States o f America 92(23): 10614-10618. Promislow DEL, Bugbee M (2000) Direct and correlated responses to selection on age at physiological maturity in Drosophila simulans. J Evolution Biol 13(6): 955-966. Reif M, Linsenmair KE, Heisenberg M (2002) Evolutionary significance of courtship conditioning in Drosophila melanogaster. Animal Behaviour 63(Part 1): 143-155. Rifkin SA, Kim J, White KP (2003) Evolution of gene expression in the Drosophila melanogaster subgroup. Nature Genetics 33(2): 138-144. Rosato E, Piccin A, Kyriacou CP (1997) Circadian rhythms: From behaviour to molecules. Bioessays 19(12): 1075-1082. Ryner LC, Goodwin SF, Castrillon DH, Anand A, Villella A et al. (1996) Control of male sexual behavior and sexual orientation in Drosophila by thefruitless gene. Cell 87(6): 1079-1089. Smith KK (2006) Craniofacial development in marsupial mammals: Developmental origins of evolutionary change. Developmental Dynamics 235(5): 1181-1193. Sokolowski MB (2001) Drosophila: Genetics meets behaviour. Nat Rev Genet 2(11):

879-890. Strambi C, Cayre M, Strambi A (1999)Neural plasticity in the adult insect brain and its hormonal control. International review of cytology - a survey of cell biology, vol 190. San Diego: Academic Press Inc. pp. 137-174. Stum A, Quackenbush J, Trajanoski Z (2002) Genesis: Cluster analysis of microarray data. Bioinformatics 18(1): 207-208. Tarone AM, Nasser YM, Nuzhdin SV (2005) Genetic variation for expression of the sex determination pathway genes in Drosophila melanogater. Genetical Research 86(1): 31-40.

77

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Tauber E, Eberl DF (2001) Song production in auditory mutants of Drosophila'. The role of sensory feedback. J Comp Physiol A 187(5): 341-348. Toivonen JM, O'Dell KMC, Petit N, Irvine SC, Knight GK et al. (2001) technical knockout, a Drosophila model of mitochondrial deafness. Genetics 159(1): 241-254. Velarde RA, Robinson GE, Fahrbach SE (2006) Nuclear receptors of the honey bee: Annotation and expression in the adult brain. Insect Molecular Biology 15(5): 583-595. Villella A, Ferri SL, Krystal JD, Hall JC (2005) Functional analysis of fruitless gene expression by transgenic manipulations of Drosophila courtship. Proceedings of the National Academy o f Sciences o f the United States of America 102(46): 16550-16557. Wayne ML, Pan YJ, Nuzhdin SV, McIntyre LM (2004) Additivity and trara-acting effects on gene expression in male Drosophila simulans. Genetics 168(3): 1413-1420. White SA, Nguyen T, Femald RD (2002) Social regulation of gonadotropin-releasing hormone. J Exp Biol 205(17): 2567-2581. Whitfield CW, Cziko AM, Robinson GE (2003) Gene expression profiles in the brain predict behavior in individual honey bees. Science 302(5643): 296-299. Wittkopp PJ, Carroll SB, Kopp A (2003) Evolution in black and white: Genetic control of pigment patterns in Drosophila. Trends Genet 19(9): 495-504.

78

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 2,3,4 5 3 2,3 Citation(s) Sex determination 2,3,7,9 Courtship conditioning Courtship conditioning Courtship vigor 6 Courtship conditioning G-protein-coupled receptor binding, cyclic-AMP phosphodiesterase activity Courtship conditioning 2,3,4 DNA binding, mRNA binding, neuropeptide hormone activity DNA binding, transcription factor activity Song transmembrane receptor activity Courtship latency 1 mRNA binding, nucleotide bindingubiquitin-protein ligase activity Courtship drive 2,3,4,7,8 transcription factor activity two-component voltage-gatedsensor activity, potassium channel activity ATP binding, calmodulinprotein serine/threonine binding, kinase activity amn dnc ato dsx eag Gene name Gene abbr. Putative Function Aspect Male Courtship 18 wheeler 18w dunce ether ago-go CaMKII* TABLES Table 3.1. Courtship deficient genes amnesiac atonal potatocouch cpo courtless crl doublesex Continued on next page; * Calcium/Calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 2 7 2,7,14 9 2,3,7 Sex discrimination Sex determination/Song/ Sex discrimination Courtship conditioningSong Song 10,11 Courtship conditioning/ Courtship latency, occurance 1 ligand-dependent nuclear receptorsequence-specific activity, DNA binding, transcription factor activity transcription factor activity receptorbinding calcium ion binding, voltage-gated sodium channel activity transcription factor activitymRNA binding, nucleotide binding, Possibly pheromone detectiontranscription factor/regulator activity 12 Courtship drive 13 iron ionbinding, tyrosine 3-monooxygenase activity protein binding, transcriptionregulator activity unknown Courtship latency/ Sex discrimination Courtship deficient genes Gene abbr. Putative Function Aspect Male Courtship Citation(s) homer eg pros pie Gene name quick-to-court qtc Continued on nextpage; * Kruppel homolog 1; f no on or offtransient A eagle Table 3.1 (Con’t). homer nonAi Kr-hl* paralytic para prospero pale fruitless fru

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 18 7,20,21 3,15 Citation(s) Sex determinationSex determination/Song/ 19 Courtship conditioningCourtship conditioningCourtship latency/ Courtship Vigor 2,3,4 2 Male mating success 15- (Toivonen et al. 2001) 18- (Villella et al. 2005) 19- (Lam et al. 2003) 16- (Dauwalder et al. 2002) 17- (Anholt and Mackay 2004) 20- (Drapeau 2003) 21- (Drapeau et al. 2006) 11 - (Fitzpatrick 11 2004) 12- (Grozinger et al. 2003) 10- (Diagana et al. 2002) 13- (Grosjean et al. 2004) 14- (Gaines et al. 2000) 8- (Orgad et al. 2000) 9- (Baker et al. 2001) adenylate cyclase activity, calcium- and receptor binding, structural molecule activity, voltage-gated potassium channel activity structural constituent ofribosome unknownmRNA binding, protein binding Sex determination 16,17 calmodulin-responsive adenylate cyclase activity ribulose-bisphosphate carboxylase activity nucleic acid binding, transcrption factor, mRNA splicing Sex determination/Song protein binding, Courtship deficient genes to tko Sh y 1- 1- (Moehring and Mackay 2004) takeout transformer tra transformer2 tra2 rutabaga rut 2- (Greenspan and Ferveur 2000) 3- (Sokolowski 2001) 4- (Emmons and Lipton2003) 5- (Tauber and Eberl 2001) 6 -(Hall 1994b) 7- (Billeteret al. 2002) Table 3.1 (Con’t). Gene name Gene abbr. Putative Function Aspect Male Courtship technical Shaker knockout yellow 00

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. GG AAATGGGCGACGTGG GTTC TCCATCGGATCAACCAGACA CTCCACACGCTTCTGTTGGAT R-primer GGCTACCCAGCGCTT AAA GT TCT GG AGT CGGT GG AC AAATTCTAGTC GGACCGGTGG GGGCGTTGCCGGATCT CCGACCGATCGCGAAGT AGCGGTGTCCCTGTATCCACT AAGACCGAAAATAATGCCCC AACCCGCTACCC AAGGTCT GATGCTTCACCCGCAAATGGCCGTCT T CGATTG GTTGAT GCGGATGTGCAATGAGAA T TTCGAAGGAGATTTCAGCAATCAAGGCTTAAGCCACCAT G CGT GCGTGCACATCCTCATCCT CGACCAGGAGGT AGTGAGTAC CGAACACCGACAGGGAGAA GAGGAGCCGCTCGTAGGAT CGGTTTT GGGCCAACACTT AACGTGGCTTCCGTTGGT ATGCTGTGGTAGATGTGCTCTGAGT AC TTCGTGAGCACCTTAGCGCATTTCGCGTTTCATACC AGGA ATCGCAGGTACGCTTATG CGCCGCGC GAGA AAATTTATT CT AACTGTTTATTCCGCCAGGT C AT ACTGCCCGGGACTTCCTT AGTTAACGTACTG GGCGCATAC T CG AAC AGGGTAACCG CGCTGGTAT AGCTCGCAACGAACTGAGGTA ATGGCAGTTGAGTGGTAGGT C GTT GG A AGCT C ACT GT CG AT AC AACAT CGACTGT AGCT CA GTTGG GAAGCGTATAT GC CACTTGTTTG 18w CaMKII eag homer nonA Gene F-primer cpo crl Continued onnext page eg dsx amn ato dnc K r-hl pie pros para fru Table 3.2. qRT-PCR primers

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. GGAAGTGGTTAACTATTATTGGCT CTCTT AGAGCTTGCC GGTCCTTT AGTC GT ACTTGTGGCGCGCGT AAGA CAACGAGATTT ATTGCGGACTTT ACGCCAGGTCTGACTGATCACAAGT CCATACCATGCTGGCTAACGGTGCGCGGAGTGGGTGT ATGCG ATTCTAC CTT CC AT CT TTGAAGGTGGATCGGATGGTGCGCCATGCGTTAACTACAA GCTTCT CGAATGACT GTGCTGT AAGGTT TGTCGGTG AGAGTT GCAT GAGCCACGGGAATCTATGTGA AGGTAAGCAAAAAGCC AATGGA AAACTTCAGGAGCGATATAGTTA GG TCTGGCGCTAATGGAGC tko tra2 to Geneqtc F-primer R-primer tra rut Sh y Table 3.2 (Con’t). qRT-PCR primers

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Table 3.3. Multivariate analysis of variance

Effect Pillai(df) Approximate F(df) P-value

Age 0.99(1) 48.28 (25,9) < 0.0001

Experience 0.76(1) 1.17(25,9) 0.43

Age *Experience 0.87(1) 2.46(25,9) 0.08

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Table 3.4. Linear discriminant loading scores

Gene LD1 LD2 LD3

18w 1.460 3.443 -4.008 amn -0.499 0.722 1.938 ato 0.405 5.796 -1.522 CaMKlI -3.280 1.468 -6.462 cpo 2.657 -2.307 0.025 crl 3.857 -6.141 -1.590 dnc -2.348 3.310 -3.768 dsx -0.139 3.264 -0.557 eag -0.603 -0.301 5.171

eg -5.366 0.431 4.743 fru -1.706 -3.042 1.617 homer -0.925 -9.294 -0.686 Kr-hl 1.038 -8.492 -5.620 nonA 2.130 -1.062 5.847 para 1.242 -4.383 1.275 pie -5.849 -0.354 -4.589 pros -0.433 0.164 -4.068 qtc -0.813 0.198 1.227 rut 4.429 3.229 8.839

Sh -1.627 2.164 -2.815 tko 2.401 -0.025 -2.184 to 0.279 2.893 -2.159 tra -2.065 5.610 -2.944 tra2 4.493 -1.053 5.162

y -1.458 5.393 -0.309

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 0.069 0.924 0.433 0.849 Raw P-value 0.523 0.533 0.830 1.63(1,37) 0.210 0.63(1,37) 3.5(1,38) F-V alue 0.42(1,37) 0.4(1,37) 0.01(1,37) 0.05(1,37) 0.02(1,38) 0.900 0.09(1,37) 0.772 0.421 0.04(1,37) 0.219 0.387 0.01(1,38) 0.918 0.684 0.578 0.291 2.05(1,36) 0.161 1.56(1,38) 1.15(1,36) F-Value Raw P-value 0.77(1,38) 0.03(1,37) 0.858 3.25(1,37) 0.080 0.0500.338 0.06(1,37) 0.89(1,37) 0.806 0.353 0.01(1,37) 0.923 0.295 <0001 *** 0.66(1,37) 0.858 0.99(1,37) 0.326 0.0370.2250.009 * 0(1,37) 0.985 0.29(1,37) 0.592 Age Experience Age*Experience 1.13(1,37) 12.36(1,36) 0.001 ** 1.52(1,37) 7.61(1,38) 0.02(1,37) 0.8810.94(1,37) 8.75(1,38) 0.005 * 0.17(1,37) 0.22(1,38) 0.34(1,38) 0.641 0.563 0.03(1,37) 2.49(1,37)4.09(1,37) 0.1230.51(1,37) 0.47947.37(1,37) 0.32(1,37) 0.26(1,37) 0.616 4.7(1,37) 1- Astericks represent significance after FDR correction formultiple hypothesis testing; * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001 J8w CaMKll eag Gene F-Value Raw P-value1 amn ato cpo crl dnc dsx eg nonA homer Continued on nextpage; Table 3.5. Analysis ofvariance on individual genes Kr-hl fru

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 0.691 0.746 0.805 0.746 0.540 0.407 0.387 0.753 0.01(1,37) 0.904 0.16(1,37) 0.11(1,37) 0.43(1,37)0.06(1,37) 0.515 0.11(1,37) 0.38(1,37) F-Value Raw P-value 0.77(1,37) Raw P-value 0.854 0.34(1,37) 0.565 0.068 0.384 0.7(1,37) 1.56(1,37) 0.220 1.75(1,37) 0.195 3.54(1,37) 0.03(1,37) 0.78(1,37) 2.29(1,37) 0.139 0.02(1,37) 0.901 <.0001 *** <0001 *** 0.002 ** 0.042 Age Experience Age*Experience 10.65(1,37) 80.57(1,37) 7.38(1,37)306.65(1,37) 0.01 * 0.07(1,37) 0.794 0.11(1,37) 0.743 0.04(1,37) 0.839 0.04(1,37) 0.847 0.02(1,37) 0.891 4.11(1,37) 0.050 2.71(1,37) 0.108 0.09(1,37) 0.772 4.44(1,37) 234.15(1,37) <.0001 *** 0.48(1,37) 0.491 0.1(1,37) 1- Astericks1- represent significance after FDR correction for multiple hypothesis testing; * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001 tko to tra2 tra rut Gene F-Value Raw P-value1 F-Value Sh qtc Table 3.5 (Con’t). Analysis ofvariance on individual genes pie y pros para

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. FIGURES

Figure 3.1. Plot of linear discriminant analysis: LD1 v. LD2. “IN”: immature and naive;

“IE”: immature having encountered a female; “MN”: mature and naive; “ME”: mature

with courtship experience.

in 04 O o M&lid u r n o T -10 0 5 10 LD1

88

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Figure 3.2. Plot of linear discriminant analysis: LD1 Vs. LD3.

in CO Q ° IN IEje

o T ■10 0 5 10 LD1

89

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Figure 3.3. Gene expression profiles of age and experience categories. Single factor hierarchical clustering of expression profiles. Red indicates increased expression, blue indicates decreased expression. Three distinct clusters were found, and are identified by the color bars on the right. Full gene names are listed in Table 3.1.

18w ato tra rvonA dnc Sh tra2 amn crl fru eag cpo rut tko dsx to Krhl pros CAMKII para Homer pie eg V

90

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ato eag crl IE M N M E 1.2 0.0 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 0.008 0.006 0.004 0.002 ME

cpo dsx = am n IE MN Category IN

Age: p < 0.05

]—

0 4 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0.0 0.1 . 0.0 0.01 0 051 0.12 0.02 0.01 . 0.08 0.06 0 0.02 0.025 0.015 0 18w dnc CaMKII IE M N M E Expression levels ofindividual genes for each age/experience category. Raw means and standard deviations are IN

0J 0.6 0.5 0.2 0.1 0.0 0.4 1.5 1.0 10 70 2.5 2.0 50 90 0.5 0.0 30 Figure 3.4. reported. Genes with significant differences in expression due to age have the corresponding p-value displayed (see text). 0.0 Xfl s- W> a os O e A e a o x a !*> V u a 4i 3 4 > *p« "© < SO

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. MNME - - p a ra Homer IE IN L_J — — 1.2 1.0 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 4.0 3.0 2.0 0.0 :

fru pros nonA Ili Ili ifc Category Age: p < 0.01 Age: p < 0.01 ---- — 0.1 0.0 0.2 0.3 0.1 0.0 0.12 0.14 0.08 0.06 0.04 0.02 0,75 0.15 0.05 M N ME Age: p < 0.001 Age: p < 0.001 Expression levels ofindividual genes for each age/experience category. eg pie K r-hl Age: p < 0.05 INIE ------—

— —

0 . 0 1 SB 1.0 1.2 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.01 I 1.0 7.0 5.0 0.0 3.0 0.02 0.01 Figure 3.4 (Con’t). 0.025 0.015 0.005 £ CQ i- DU o e es C s o X/i u a x CA J3 "© X < v© to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. -I tra2 tko IE M N ME IN Age: p < 0.05 0.2 0.1 0.0 0.2 0.1 0.0 0.25 0.15 0.05 0.25 0.15 0.05 Sh Category IN IE MN ME

1.5 1.0 2.5 2.0 0.5 0.0 Age: p < 0.001 Expression levels ofindividual genes for each age/experience category. rut IN IE MN ME Age: p < 0.001 1.4 1.2 0.8 0.4 0.0 Figure 3.4 (Con’t). 0.025 0.005 0.015 E a W> O c S s Oi U a

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Chapter 4: Contribution of differential gene expression to

natural variation in male Drosophila melanogaster courtship

Abstract

Behaviors important to fitness often exhibit substantial variation, and thus can be

the targets of selection. In order to evaluate what potential these behaviors have to

evolve, it is important to understand what underlying genetic differences are contributing

to this behavioral variation. Drosophila melanogaster can serve as an excellent model to

answer this question; male courtship behavior is complex and well documented, and

many genes have been identified as necessary for normal male behavior. However, these

previously described genes have been identified using mutant screening, a process that

creates alleles with severe defects. These alleles are unlikely to persist in a natural

population, and thus the role of these genes in natural behavioral variation is unknown.

Subtle gene expression differences are more likely to be present in natural populations,

and these differences can be the target of selection. To investigate the possible role of

differential gene expression in natural variation of male courtship behavior, I identified

10 families with High Male Courtship Progression (MCP) and 10 families with Low

MCP. The full-genome expression profiles of the High and Low categories were

compared using microarrays. Fourteen candidate genes were identified as being

differentially expressed, and none of them had previously been associated with male

courtship behavior in D. melanogaster. Their functions and potential roles in courtship

are discussed. Genes contributing to natural variation in male behavior may not be the

ones previously identified in mutant screening.

94

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Introduction

A fundamental goal in the field of behavior genetics is to understand the

underlying genetic differences contributing to natural variation in behaviors that are

crucial to fitness (Sokolowski 1998). If individual fitness depends on behavior, there

would presumably be strong directional selection minimizing variation in such traits.

However, there have been many studies identifying variation in behavioral traits (Benus

et al. 1991; Petrie and Kempenaers 1998; Anholt 2004; Sisodia and Singh 2005).

Identifying the genetic differences contributing to behavioral variation can help

determine what might be maintaining this variation and what potential a behavioral trait

has to evolve.

Much progress has been made in our understanding of the genes underlying

courtship behavior using the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster, as a model system

(Sokolowski 1998; Greenspan and Dierick 2004). Male courtship behavior consists of a

complex pattern of behaviors that generally follows the sequence of orienting toward the

female, tapping her abdomen, following her, performing a courtship “song”, licking her

genitalia, attempting copulation, and copulation (Hall 1994a). Approximately 45 genes

have been identified as being involved in male courtship behavior (Drysdale et al. 2005);

these courtship deficient genes affect a range of factors, including chemosensation,

vision, courtship song production, and overall vigor and drive (Hall 1994a).

The majority of courtship deficient genes have been identified using mutant

screening (Hall 1994a; Yamamoto and Nakano 1998; Greenspan and Ferveur 2000;

Emmons and Lipton 2003). Mutant screening is useful in identifying genes involved in

95

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. normal courtship behavior. However, this method usually produces null alleles usually

have many pleiotropic in addition to the disruption of normal courtship behavior, and

alleles with such severe mutations are unlikely to persist at an appreciable frequency in

natural populations (Hall 1994b; Greenspan 1997). Thus it would be beneficial to

examine subtler changes in the underlying genetic architecture of natural behavioral

variation to identify potential targets of selection (Greenspan 1997; Sokolowski 1998;

Emmons and Lipton 2003). One such subtle change is gene expression; evolution via

gene expression and mutations in regulatory regions has been proposed as a major

i contributor to phenotypic variation between species (King and Wilson 1975).

Differences in gene expression both between and within species show evidence of

positive and stabilizing selection (Ferea et al. 1999; Oleksiak et al. 2002; Rifkin et al.

2003; but see Khaitovich et al. 2004). Substantial variation in expression has been

identified within populations (Oleksiak et al. 2002; Wayne et al. 2004; Hughes et al.

2006). Therefore, changes in gene expression could be an underlying cause of behavioral

variation. Static baseline expression differences can be targets of selection (e.g.

Fitzpatrick et al. 2007). Additionally, baseline expression of genes could be an important

contributor to variation in male courtship behavior. A screen of changing expression of

previously identified courtship deficient genes showed no differential expression due to

experience with females (E. A. Ruedi, unpublished data). Thus, examining the

correlation of baseline expression differences and behavioral differences of mature males

within a natural population will help identify which genes are potentially contributing to

behavioral variation.

96

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. In this study, I specifically wanted to identify genes contributing to natural

variation in D. melanogaster male courtship progression, or patterns of behavior over

time. Microarray analysis allowed me to scan the entire genome for candidate genes

associated with natural variation, as opposed to limiting my focus to previously identified

genes. I found that the genes originally considered previously associated with courtship

through to mutant analysis do not have significant differential expression associated with

high or low male courtship progression, and identified 14 new candidates that may be

related to natural variation in this behavior.

M ethods

Experimental organisms

For details about the fly stocks, mating design, and behavioral methods used here,

please refer to Chapter 2. To investigate genetic variation in male courtship behavior, I

created 80 full-sibling families from single-pair matings from the New Jersey stock.

These families were produced in four blocks of 20 families, and one block of 20 was

analyzed per week. To produce a full-sibling family, a virgin male and female were

placed on fresh vial of media, allowed to reproduce for four days, and then transferred to

another fresh vial for four more days. This reduced variation in behavior due to

environmental rearing, as full-sibling brothers from both vials were used in courtship

trials. Virgin males were collected from these vials 0-4 hours post-eclosion, and were

kept in fresh vials separated by family and date of emergence. 5-8 of these virgin

brothers were used in courtship trials when they were 5-6 days old.

97

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Females used as courtship targets were collected from a stock in which a

spontaneously-occurring ebony (e) mutation had been introgressed into a non-inbred wild

type genetic background (Ives background). The e/e females used in the behavioral trials

were collected as virgins 0-4 hours post-eclosion and kept in single-sex vials separated by

date of emergence.

Behavioral observations

The day before a courtship trial, males and females were placed in observation

devices modeled after those in Drapeau and Long (2000). Four total devices were set up

per day, and there were four days of trials per week (total of 16 trials/week). A total of 2

males from each family were observed each day. All trials within one week were

considered a “block”, and effects of block were included in my microarray analysis (see

below).

All behavioral observations took place between 8 am and 10:30 am and were

conducted at 24°C ± 1°C, in a room isolated from all other activities. At the start of

observations, the opaque plastic sheet separating the sexes was removed; thus a single

male was exposed to a single female. The behavior of each male was then recorded via

instantaneous scan sampling (Martin and Bateson 1993) every 30 seconds for a total of

30 minutes. At each scan, the male was scored as performing one of the behaviors in an

ordinal scale modeled after the natural male courtship progression (Male Courtship

Progression, MCP; Table 4.1). At the end of a trial, each male was thus characterized by

60 scores in a temporal sequence. All observations were conducted by the same observer

98

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. (E.A.R.), and were blind with respect to the genotype of the male. After the behavioral

trials, all flies were discarded.

Identification of “High” and “Low” MCP families

Genetic variation in male courtship pattern was analyzed using a generalized

linear model with repeated measures, and a cumulative logit link appropriate for ordinal

data with a multinomial distribution (PROC GENMOD, SAS Statistical Analysis

Software v. 9.1; (SAS Institute 2006). Only the first 30 time points (the first 15 minutes)

were used for this analysis, as males generally cease locomotor activity towards the end

of the trial. I identified significant genetic variation in the temporal pattern of behavior

between families (family*time: x2 79=105.12, p = 0.026). Subsequently I wished to

identify 10 “High MCP” and 10 “Low MCP” families to use in gene expression analysis.

To accomplish this, I created a summary index of MCP for each individual by calculating

the rank order for each male at each observation, then calculating the mean rank for the

individual over the first 30 time intervals in the mating trial (SAS PROC RANK). Due to

the nature of the ordinal scale used in this analysis (l=motionless, 8=copulation), a low

rank correlated to a high MCP score. At each time point, tied ranks were assigned the

average rank for the tied males. Individual mean rank was then used as the dependent

variable in a linear model (SAS PROC MIXED) with family as a random effect, and day

and trial within day as fixed effects. A similar model treating family as a fixed effect was

also used to calculate the least-square mean rank for each family in an experiment. There

was significant genetic variance in rank of MCP (Log likelihood ratio test: y?\=l,

p=0.008). I identified the ten families with the highest average MCP/lowest average rank

99

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. and the ten families with the lowest average MCP/highest average rank (Table 4.2; Figure

4.1) to use in the following microarray analysis of gene expression. These will be further

referred to as the “High” and “Low” categories.

Microarray hybridization

Tissue for RNA analysis was collected from full-sibling brothers of males used in

behavioral trials. During each block of behavioral trials, 4 males from each family were

frozen; 2 from the first single-pair mating vial, and 2 from the second single-pair mating

vial. Males from both vials were pooled into one sample per family in subsequent gene

expression analyses. All males were frozen between 2-3 pm. The frozen males had not

experienced courtship or copulation, but were the same age as the males used in courtship

observations. Expression differences due to courtship experience will therefore not be

represented in this analysis. However, an examination of dynamic expression in genes

previously associated with courtship behavior revealed that expression did not change

due to courtship experience (E. A. Ruedi, unpublished data).

I removed the heads of the four individuals frozen from each family and pooled

them together into one biological replicate to yield sufficient RNA for analysis. Heads

were used as opposed to whole flies because I was specifically interested in expression

differences for genes involved in nervous system function. RNA was extracted using the

PicoPure RNA extraction kit (Arcturus) following the manufacturer’s protocol for

extraction using DNaseI (Qiagen) treatment. Quantity and purity of RNA was checked

using a spectrophotometer (Nanodrop); the A 260/A280 for each sample was 1.9 or higher.

Quality was also checked using an Agilent 2100 bioanalyzer chip. mRNA from each

100

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. sample was then labeled and fragmented using the Affymetrix GeneChip® One-Cycle

Target Labeling and Control Reagents kit and manufacturers instructions. Quality of

biotinylated cRNA was checked before and after fragmentation using gel electrophoresis.

Labeling was completed in two different sets that were two days apart. Samples from

both High and Low categories were equally represented in each set. Individual samples

were hybridized to Affymetrix GeneChip® Drosophila Genome 2.0 Arrays and scanned

by the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Keck Center for Functional Genomics.

This provided me with 20 total gene expression profiles, 1 per family, 10 profiles per

category.

Microarray statistics

Probe hybridization intensities underwent MAS5 present/absent calls using the

“mas5calls” function (Affy package) in R Statistical Software v. 2.5 (R Development

Core Team, 2007). GCRMA normalization, transformation, and summation was then

completed using the “gcrma” function (Affy package) for the 12,148 genes called

“present.” The normalized expression levels for each gene were then analyzed using a

mixed model ANOVA (SAS PROC MIXED) with category, block, and set as fixed

effects, and block*category as a random interaction effect. The “block” effect referred to

the week that the family’s behavior was observed; the “set” effect referred to the date that

the RNA was labeled and fragmented. Some blocks did not contain both High and Low

categories, and thus the simple effect of category or the interaction effect of

block*category was not appropriate for calculating the ANOVA test statistics. Therefore,

to estimate expression differences between High and Low categories taking variance due

101

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. to block into account, I used the “estimate” statement in SAS PROC MIXED to calculate

the correct test statistic for each gene. The “estimate” statement allowed me to control

which cat*block effects were included in the estimation of differences between High and

Low categories. To investigate the significance of expression differences for each gene, I

ran 6000 permutations of the data and analyzed those permutations using the same

statistical model and “estimate” statements as above. This allowed me to identify how

many permutations randomly produced test statistics larger than the observed data. I also

calculated the fold-change differences for any significant genes by calculating the ratio of

average expression of H/L categories (these data were not log 2 -transformed).

To examine the ability of the expression profiles from the significant genes to

appropriately classify flies into High and Low categories, I performed a linear

discriminant analysis (Everitt 2005) with the “Ida” function in R statistical software v. 2.5

(MASS package). I then used the “predict” function to classify individual flies based on

leave-one-out cross-validation of the classification using the “CV” command in the “Ida”

function.

To evaluate which biological functions were specifically enriched, and to identify

functionally redundant annotations within my candidates, I used the Gene Ontology

Functional Annotation Clustering tool in DAVID (Dennis et al. 2003). I used

PathwayAssist 3.0 to identify any gene networks associated with my candidate genes, and

the Ingenuity Pathway Analysis 5.0 software to identify any human/mouse/rat homologs

to my candidates and their corresponding pathways.

102

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. R esults

Fourteen genes were identified as candidates contributing to natural variation in

male courtship progression. These genes were differentially expressed between High and

Low families with a probability of false positive of <0.0004 according to the permutation

tests and an FDR of <0.3. Using these cutoffs, four of the fourteen genes are likely false

positives. The names/IDs, locations, known or inferred molecular functions

(flybase.bio.indiana.edu), fold-changes and estimates of differential expression for all

fourteen genes are listed in Table 4.3. Cross-validation of the linear discriminant

analysis for these genes revealed that the expression profiles are very successful at

predicting the category of individuals (High: 90%; Low: 90%). Overall, more genes had

increased expression (13) than decreased expression (1) in the High categories compared

to the Low categories; this trend was significant (x i = 10.29, p < 0.01).

DAVID functional analysis clustering revealed two groups of enriched or

overlapping biological processes among the candidates. Functional group 1 included

cellular, macromolecule, and protein metabolism (enrichment score: 0.99, geometric

group mean P-value = 0.013; genes: pip, CG1531, Tpr2,1(3)1X-14, CG31619, CG10444,

mus312, CG9243)\ functional group 2 included the parent group of metabolism, cellular

and physiological processes (enrichment score: 0.97, geometric group mean P-value=

0.03; genes: pip,jagn, CG9243, CGI531, GluCla, mus312, Tpr2,1(3)1X-14, CG31619,

CG1819, CG10444).

PathwayAssist did not identify any known pathways associated with my candidate

genes. Ingenuity Pathways Analysis identified one pathway in which the mammalian

homolog of pip [prolactin-induced protein ) is involved. The top functions of this

103

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. pathway are cellular development, hematological system development and function, and

immune and lymphatic system development and function. Other genes in this pathway

are early growth response 2 {Drosophila homolog: Krox-20), spleen focus forming virus

proviral integration oncogene spil, transcription factor 3, and CD4 molecule. Their

pathway network is displayed in Figure 4.2.

Discussion

Fourteen candidate genes were identified in this genome-wide analysis, and none

of them have been previously associated with male courtship behavior. At least in this

population, the expression differences of candidate courtship genes discovered using

mutation screening do not appear to be directly involved in natural behavioral variation.

This could indicate that mutation screening is useful in identifying genes required for the

development or maintenance of normal male behavior, it might not be identifying

possible targets for selection. However, I do not know where the polymorphisms causing

these expression differences are located. Thus, I cannot rule out the possibility that the

genes previously identified through mutant screening have a regulatory role in expression

differences of genes identified in this microarray study.

Other microarray analyses of expression differences contributing to female

mating speed (Mackay et al. 2005) and male aggression (Dierick and Greenspan 2006;

Edwards et al. 2006) identified a much larger number of candidate genes than the present

study. These analyses of behavior focused on lines that were artificially selected to

promote extreme differences in phenotype, whereas my analysis used standing variation

for a behavioral trait. This standing variation is much more subtle; additionally “High

104

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. MCP families” (or “Low MCP families”) could be “High” (or “Low”) through different

means. For example, one family might display “High” MCP scores because of their

speedy response to stimuli, while another family might be “High” due to the length of

their courtship song. These differences might be attributed to different molecular

pathways, and could be the cause for such discrepancies in the number of candidate genes

between selection studies and the present study.

The functional annotation clustering revealed two main important biological

functions enriched in my candidate genes: cellular processes and metabolism. Several of

the candidate genes involved in cellular processes have possible roles in neurotransmitter

signaling; for example, GluCla is a glutamate-gated chloride channel (Cully et al. 1996;

Semenov and Pak 1999) and mutants for this gene show a similarity to other bang-

sensitive mutants previously associated with male courtship song (Ma et al. 2001). Other

candidates, such as CG10444 and CG31619, are also putatively involved in cellular

signaling in the brain. A conserved protein domain BLAST search for CG9243, the only

candidate with down-regulated expression in High families, revealed a conserved

Phospholipase D motif, which can be involved in signal transduction

(www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov). These results indicate that variation in neurotransmission

could lead to variation in male courtship behavior, possibly by altering the

responsiveness of the fly to stimuli from the female.

The other functional cluster involved metabolic processes. These can range from

homeostasis to digestion to inhibition of hormones,pipe, the candidate with the largest

fold-change between High and Low families, is a sulfotransferase presumably involved in

metabolism. One of its 10 isoforms, ST-2, is well documented due to its involvement in

105

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. establishing embryonic dorsoventral axes during development through expression in the

follicle membrane (Sergeev et al. 2001). Other isoforms are also expressed in larval

salivary glands (Zhu et al. 2005), and the target of its sulfation is currently unknown.

Thepipe probe set identified in my study is specific to the ST-1 isoform; hence, pipe has

an important function in adult males that has not yet been explored.

Like pipe, many of these candidate genes have been previously associated with

development. Tpr2 is a putative protein suppressor that is important for the development

of the eye (Kazemi-Esfarjani and Benzer 2000). jagunal is an endoplasmic reticulum

(ER) protein that works to condense the ER in the oocyte during oogenesis, which allows

an increase in membrane traffic and appropriate oocyte and bristle growth (Lee and

Cooley 2007). CGI 0444 and CG31619 are both involved in cell communication

important to embryonic development and cell differentiation (Orian et al. 2003; Stark et

al. 2003; Vogel et al. 2003). The role of these genes in adults has also never been

evaluated.

Several of these genes show evidence of behavioral pleiotropy, and warrant a

much more extensive investigation into their role in male courtship progression. CG1531,

CG30492, and CG31619 all show significant differential expression between females

from lines selected for fast and slow mating speed (Mackay et al. 2005); there is

potentially a role for these genes in both male and female mating behavior. CG30492 is

also up-regulated in males from lines of flies selected for increased aggressiveness, and

thus could be involved in many behavioral processes (Dierick and Greenspan 2006).

Another candidate gene from this study, CGI 0444, is one of the down-regulated in these

106

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. highly aggressive lines; down-regulation in the aggressive lines was significantly less

common than up-regulation (Dierick and Greenspan 2006).

Much of the variation between the behavioral patterns of the High and Low lines

appears to occur early in the courtship trial (see Figure 4.1). This early behavioral

variation could potentially correspond to variation in the ability of different families to

detect the presence of a female and respond appropriately in a timely manner. The

candidate genes identified here indicate that cell-cell communication and metabolic

processes in the brain are important factors contributing to natural variation. Perhaps

variation in the expression of these genes corresponds to variation in communicating the

presence of stimuli and triggering appropriate behavioral responses.

This study brings us one step closer to understanding the genetic basis of natural

variation in behavior. Additional studies must be conducted to investigate the role of

expression differences in the fourteen new candidate genes described here in the

evolution of this trait, and the maintenance of variation in male courtship progression.

First, further work must be done over- or under-expressing these candidates and

examining behavioral changes in the flies to further confirm their role in natural

behavioral variation. Future studies could also investigate the presence of single

nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in these new candidates to determine if expression

differences are due to polymorphisms within the coding region or perhaps in regulatory

regions. If there are no SNPs in the coding regions of the new candidates, it would be

interesting to examine sequence differences in the candidates previously identified

through mutant screening to determine if they perhaps serve a regulatory role in

differential gene expression involved in natural behavioral variation.

107

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Acknowledgements

I would like to thank the National Science Foundation for their funding for this project

(NSF DEB 0296177), as well as the University of Illinois Program in Ecology and

Evolutionary Biology. I would also like to thank the members of the Hughes lab for their

help with the flies. I would especially like to thank J. M. Dmevich for her invaluable

help with microarray techniques, and T. M. Felix for learning them with me. Thanks to

the UIUC W. M. Keck Center for Functional Genomics and C. A. Wilson for their advice

and assistance. Finally, 1 would like to thank R. M. Reynolds and A. L. Toth for

reviewing this paper.

List of References

Anholt RRH (2004) Genetic modules and networks for behavior: Lessons from Drosophila. Bioessays 26(12): 1299-1306. Benus RF, Bohus B, Koolhaas JM, Vanoortmerssen GA (1991) Heritable variation for aggression as a reflection of individual coping strategies. Experientia 47(10): 1008-1019. Cully DF, Paress PS, Liu KK, Schaeffer JM, Arena JP (1996) Identification of a

Drosophila melanogaster glutamate-gated chloride channel sensitive to the antiparasitic agent avermectin. Journal o f Biological Chemistry 271(33): 20187-20191. Dennis GJ, Sherman BT, Hosack DA, Yang J, Gao W et al. (2003) David: Database for annotation, visualization, and integrated discovery. Genome Biology 4(5): P3. Dierick HA, Greenspan RJ (2006) Molecular analysis of flies selected for aggressive behavior. Nature Genetics 38(9): 1023-1031. Drapeau MD, Long AD (2000) The Copulatron, a multi-chamber apparatus for observing drosophila courtship behaviors. Drosophila Information Services 83: 194-196.

108

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Drysdale R, Crosby M, Consortium TF (2005) Flybase: Genes and gene models. Nucleic Acids Research 33: D390-D395 http://flvbase.org/. Edwards AC, Rollmann SM, Morgan TJ, Mackay TFC (2006) Quantitative genomics of aggressive behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Plos Genetics 2(9): 1386-1395. Emmons SW, Lipton J (2003) Genetic basis of male sexual behavior. J Neurobiol 54(1): 93-110. Everitt B (2005) An R and S-Plus Companion to Multivariate Analysis; Casella G, Fienberg S, Olkin I, editors. London: Springer-Verlag London Limited. Ferea TL, Botstein D, Brown PO, Rosenzweig RF (1999) Systematic changes in gene expression patterns following adaptive evolution in yeast. Proceedings of the National Academy o f Sciences o f the United States o f America 96(17): 9721-9726. Fitzpatrick MJ, Feder E, Rowe L, Sokolowski MB (2007) Maintaining a behaviour polymorphism by frequency-dependent selection on a single gene. Nature 447(7141): 210-U215. Greenspan RJ (1997) A kinder, gentler genetic analysis of behavior: Dissection gives way to modulation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 7(6): 805-811. Greenspan RJ, Ferveur JF (2000) Courtship in Drosophila. AnnuRev Genet 34: 205-232. Greenspan RJ, Dierick HA (2004) 'Am not I a fly like thee?' - from genes in fruit flies to behavior in humans. Hum Mol Genet 13: R267-R273. Hall JC (1994a) The mating of a fly. Science 264(5166): 1702-1714. Hall JC (1994b) Pleiotropy o f Behavioral Genes. In: Greenspan RJ, Kyriacou CP, editors. Flexability and constraint in behavioral systems: John Wiley & Sons, LTD. pp. 15-27. Hughes KA, Ayroles JF, Reedy MM, Drnevich JM, Rowe KC et al. (2006) Segregating variation in the transcriptome: cis regulation and additivity of effects. Genetics 173(3): 1347-1364. Kazemi-Esfarjani P, Benzer S (2000) Genetic suppression of polyglutamine toxicity in Drosophila. Science 287(5459): 1837-1840. Khaitovich P, Weiss G, Lachmann M, Hellmann I, Enard W et al. (2004) A neutral model of transcriptome evolution. Plos Biology 2(5): 682-689.

109

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. King M-C, Wilson AC (1975) Evolution at two levels in humans and chimpanzees. Science 188(4184): 107-116. Lee S, Cooley L (2007) jagunal is required for reorganizing the endoplasmic reticulum during Drosophila oogenesis. J Cell Biol 176(7): 941-952. Ma E, Gu XQ, Wu XH, Xu T, Haddad GG (2001) Mutation in pre-mRNA adenosine deaminase markedly attenuates neuronal tolerance to o-2 deprivation in Drosophila melanogaster. Journal o f Clinical Investigation 107(6): 685-693. Mackay TFC, Heinsohn SL, Lyman RF, Moehring AJ, Morgan TJ et al. (2005) Genetics and genomics of Drosophila mating behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102: 6622-6629. Martin P, Bateson P (1993) Measuring Behaviour: An introductory guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Oleksiak MF, Churchill GA, Crawford DL (2002) Variation in gene expression within and among natural populations. Nature Genetics 32(2): 261-266. Orian A, van Steensel B, Delrow J, Bussemaker HJ, Li L et al. (2003) Genomic binding by theDrosophila MYC, MAX, MAD/MNT transcription factor network. Gene Dev 17(9): 1101-1114. Petrie M, Kempenaers B (1998) Extra-pair paternity in birds: Explaining variation between species and populations. Trends EcolEvol 13(2): 52-58.

Rifkin SA, Kim J, White KP (2003) Evolution of gene expression in the Drosophila melanogaster subgroup. Nature Genetics 33(2): 138-144. SAS Institute (2006) SAS online documentation v. 9.1. Available: http://support.sas.com/91doc/docMainpage.isp. Accessed 2006. Semenov EP, Pak WL (1999) Diversification of Drosophila chloride channel gene by multiple posttranscriptional mRNA modifications. J Neurochem 72(1): 66-72. Sergeev P, Streit A, Heller A, Steinmann-Zwicky M (2001) The Drosophila dorsoventral determinant pipe contains ten copies of a variable domain homologous to mammalian heparan sulfate 2-sulfotransferase. Developmental Dynamics 220(2): 122-132. Sisodia S, Singh BN (2005) Behaviour genetics of Drosophila: Non-sexual behaviour. Journal of Genetics 84(2): 195-216.

110

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Sokolowski MB (1998) Genes for normal behavioral variation: Recent clues from flies and worms. Neuron 21(3): 463-466. Stark A, Brennecke J, Russell RB, Cohen SM (2003) Identification of Drosophila microRNA targets. Plos Biology 1(3): 397-409. Vogel C, Teichmann SA, Chothia C (2003) The immunoglobulin superfamily in Drosophila melanogaster and Caenorhabditis elegans and the evolution of complexity. Development 130(25): 6317-6328. Wayne ML, Pan YJ, Nuzhdin SV, McIntyre LM (2004) Additivity and trans- acting effects on gene expression in male Drosophila simulans. Genetics 168(3): 1413-1420. Yamamoto D, Nakano Y (1998) Genes for sexual behavior. Biochemical & Biophysical Research Communications 246(1): 1-6. Zhu XJ, Sen J, Stevens L, Goltz JS, Stein D (2005) Drosophila pipe protein activity in the ovary and the embryonic salivary gland does not require heparan sulfate glycosaminoglycans. Development 132(17): 3813-3822.

I l l

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. TABLES

Table 4.1. Male Courtship Progression Scale

Score Behavior

1 Motionless

2 M oving

3 Oriented toward female

4 Following/chasing female

5 Wing vibration

6 Licking genitalia

7 Attempted copulation

8 Copulation

112

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Table 4.2. Average rank and average MCP score for 10 High and 10 Low families.

Family1 Rank2 ± Std. err. Average MCP ± Std. err.

30 380.27 ± 38.88 7.22 ±0.17

153 376.28 ±41.57 7.25 ±0.19

61 373.26 ±38.88 7.23 ±0.17

167 362.60 ±38.88 6.99 ±0.17

113 362.30 ±49.18 7.02 ± 0.22 95 358.60 ±38.88 6.93 ±0.17 57 352.18 ±41.57 6.81 ±0.19

92 352.17 ±38.88 6.87 ±0.17

139 347.37 ± 38.88 6.94 ±0.17

125 344.86 ±41.57 6.96 ±0.19

177 239.18 ±38.88 4.85 ±0.17

12 235.20 ±44.90 4.71 ±0.20 54 233.50 ±38.88 4.89 ±0.17

21 220.80 ±38.88 4.55 ±0.17

32 216.05 ±41.57 4.20 ±0.19

182 203.04 ±41.57 3.90 ±0.19

65 193.22 ±38.88 3.83 ±0.17

15 190.11 ±41.57 3.84 ±0.19

35 170.71 ±38.88 3.28 ±0.17

87 148.79 ±41.57 2.84 ±0.19

1- Families in bold are "High MCP"; families in italic are “Low MCP” 2- Lower ranking indicates higher MCP score

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 0 0.0002 0.0002 0.0002 0.0002 0.04 0.0002 0.04 0.04 0.0002 5.29 0.03 0 4.6 0.46 0.31 0.24 4.59 0.34 4.92 0.04 Effect Size2 t-Value3 Prob-t PFP4

1.46 0.551.24 4.65 0.311.24 4.19 0.05 0 1.42 0.511.18 5.43 0.03 1.27 1.31 0.39 4.54 0.05 (known or inferred) lipase and phospholipase activity -1.26 -0.33 -4.43 0.05 glutamate-gated Cl channel activity; zinc ion binding cholinesterase activity Na-dep. multivitamin transporter act. unknown unknown neurotransmitter receptor activity 3R85F14-85F15 metalloendopeptidase activity; 2L39F1-39F32R 43E5-43E7 procollagen N-endopeptidase act. zinc ion binding 1.49 0.57 4.36 0.05 0 3L 3L 65F4-65F5 protein binding 1.37 3R92B1-92B2 3R 84D8-84D9 carboxylesterase activity; 2R56F11-56F11 cation transporter activity; X X 7C9-7D1 2L 39A7-39A7 X 16B6-16B7 4.3. Genes with significantly different expression between High MCP and Low MCP categories a-EstlO CG30492 CG31619 1(3)1X-14 C G I0444 CG1531 CG9243 GluCla CG12991 mus312 Gene Name/ID Location1 Molecular Function Fold Change Table

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

0.0003 0.0003 0.05 0.05 4.4 4.59 0.04 0.0003 4.55 0.19 3.23 1.14 1.25 0.32 Fold Change Effect Size2 t-Value3 Prob-t PFP4

(known or inferred) heat shock protein bindingATP binding; transmembranemovement; transporter activity 1.21 0.28 4.16 0.05 0.0003 Location1 Molecular Function Genes with significantly different expression between High MCP and Low MCP categories 3L 76A5-76A6 sulfotransferase activity 9.38 3R 83C3-83C3 unknown 2L 36A2-36A2 X N/A (Con’t). 4.3 1 cytologicalChromosome and 1 arm position Tpr2 CG34120 Gene Name/ID with cutoffa of0.0004,< PFP approximately 4 genes out of this list can be expected to be false positive. 3 3 Degrees o = 2 ffreedom for all t-tests Table 2 Average difference ofmRNA abundance in High vs. Low categories on Log2 scale 4 of Probability significantdetecting difference in mRNA expression between High v. Low categories due to chance (out of6000 permutations ofdata); pip jagn

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. without prohibited reproduction Further owner. copyright the of permission with Reproduced Figure 4.1. 4.1. Figure over the first 15 m inutes o f the courtship trial. courtship the f o inutes m 15 first the over I S E R U FIG Average MCP Score M ale courtship progression o f f o progression courtship ale M Time (Minutes) Time 10 116 “High M CP” and and CP” M “High 10 “Low M CP” fam ilies ilies fam CP” M “Low Families Figure 4.2. Pathway of mammalian homolog(proactin-involvedprotein) topipe.

SPI1

T C F 3 ^

\ / " EGR2 \ / V'/C / * / I \ ' / kCD4y

117

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Chapter 5: Conclusion

Natural variation in the progression of male courtship behavior in Drosophila

melanogaster could be the target of selection. Although many genes have been

implicated in contributing to normal male courtship behavior in this species, it is not

known if these are the same genes contributing to natural variation. The goals of my

dissertation were to identify variation in the sequence of male behavior using a natural

population, and investigate the genetic underpinnings of this variation. The latter was

accomplished by examining alterations in expression of previously identified candidate

genes in response to changes in age or experience, and then investigating if the

previously identified candidates were also involved in natural behavioral variation. My

thesis had the following objectives:

Objective 1: Develop a high-throughput assay of male courtship behavior that captures the details of courtship progression for a sample size large enough for genetic and genomic studies. Use this male courtship progression (MCP) assay to determine the presence of genetic variation in the whole sequence of courtship behavior within a natural population.

Genetic differences in MCP were detected whether males were placed with virgin

or previously mated female targets. The new MCP assay I developed was the only metric

able to detect variation in male behavior directed towards mated females, arguably the

type of female most often encountered in the wild. Variation in the sequence of male

courtship has a genetic basis, and thus could be the target of selection. I then wanted to

investigate the possible molecular contributions to natural variation. I focused on

118

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. differential gene expression, as alteration of the expression of a gene can be one target of

selection. Part of this alteration can be due to non-genotypic factors, such as age or

experience.

Objective 2: Examine candidate courtship genes previously identified in the literature for alteration in gene expression due to age or experience with a female. Genes were considered static if age, experience, or the interaction between age and experience had no effect on expression. Genes were considered dynamically expressed if any of these factors had a significant effect on expression.

In the set of previously identified courtship genes, I found that any dynamic

changes in gene expression occur due to age (reproductive maturity), not experience with

females. There was a marginally non-significant effect of the interaction between age

and experience, which hints at the possibility of an expression*environment interaction.

Future work could examine the full-genome expression profiles for more replicates of

isogenic males of different ages and experience levels. This would confirm the results of

my dissertation, as well as identify any new genes responding to experience with a

female.

I continued my investigation of genes contributing to natural variation in male

courtship progression using mature males. I wanted to determine which genes exhibited

differential baseline expression between families with the most extreme differences in

courtship progression from Objective 1.

Objective 3: Use full-genome microarray analysis to find candidate genes associated with High and Low male courtship progression. Compare these candidates with

119

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. previously identified candidates, and examine functional trends in the new set of candidates.

I discovered 14 candidate genes with expression differences correlated to High

and Low MCP; none were previously associated with male courtship behavior. I also

identified which of these new candidates might be co-regulated. Many of the new

candidates have been implicated in cellular communication and development. I suggest

that variation in the expression of these genes could correspond to variation in

communicating the presence of stimuli and triggering appropriate behavioral responses.

Future research will confirm that these candidates are directly associated with male

courtship progression by using functional mutants with decreased (or silenced)

expression to examine their effects on behavior.

120

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix A: Statistical tests for univariate models of single behaviors

E ffect1

Family Day Trial(Day)

Trait

VT Experiment

Motionless 1.78(77,409) * 1.27(14,409) 1.04 (48,409)

Moving 1.68 (77,409) * 2.09(14,409) 1.56(48,409)

Orienting 1.38 (77,409) 1.04(14,409) 1.17(48,409)

Follow ing 0.89 (77,409) 1.07(14,409) 0.78 (48,409)

Wing Vibration 1.09 (77,409) 0.78 (14,409) 0.85 (48,409)

Licking 0.86 (77,409) 1.15 (14,409) 1.21 (48,409)

Attempted Copulation 1.28 (77,409) 1.23 (14,409) 1.51 (48,409)

M T Experiment

Motionless 1.21 (75,374) 1.53 (12,374) 0.97 (48,374)

Moving 1.10(75,374) 1.28 (12,374) 1.85 (48,374)**

Orienting 1.18 (75,374) 1.33 (12,374) 1.08 (48,374)

Follow ing 1.10(75,374) 0.91 (12,374) 0.87 (48,374)

Wing Vibration 0.97 (75,374) 1.02(12,374) 0.76 (48,374)

Licking 1.08 (75,374) 2.08 (12,374) 1.33 (48,374)

Attempted Copulation 1.22 (75,374) 0.67 (12,374) 1.04 (48,374)

* p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001; Sequential Bonferonni used to correct for multiple tests

1 - Family, day, and trail(day) tested with Type III F-statistics

121

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix B: Mean MCP scores for all VT families

Family Average MCP Score Standard Error

3 5.921 0.174

6 6.590 0.186

7 5.075 0.174

8 6.489 0.201

12 4.711 0.201

14 4.696 0.174

15 3.838 0.186

16 5.779 0.174

17 5.775 0.174

18 5.362 0.186

19 5.689 0.201

21 4.546 0.174

26 6.239 0.201

28 5.362 0.186

29 5.911 0.201

30 7.217 0.174

32 4.205 0.186

35 3.275 0.174

38 5.510 0.186

39 7.100 0.220

54 4.892 0.174

57 6.810 0.186

61 7.233 0.174 62 6.750 0.174

64 6.752 0.186

65 3.825 0.174

68 5.575 0.174

72 6.305 0.186

122

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix B (c o n ’t)

Fam ily Average MCP Score Standard Error 74 6.029 0.174 75 5.908 0.174 77 4.814 0.186 78 6.029 0.186 79 6.500 0.174 82 6.533 0.174 83 6.058 0.174 84 6.690 0.186 87 2.843 0.186 88 '5.567 0.174 92 6.871 0.174 95 6.933 0.174 101 5.395 0.186 103 5.605 0.186 107 5.842 0.174 108 6.163 0.174 109 5.961 0.201 112 6.086 0.186 113 7.020 0.220 114 6.546 0.174 115 5.867 0.174 117 6.446 0.174 119 5.588 0.174 120 6.243 0.186 121 5.346 0.174 124 6.088 0.174 125 6.962 0.186

123

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix B (c o n ’t)

Fam ily Average MCP Score Standard Error 135 6.210 0.186 136 5.054 0.174 137 6.714 0.186 139 6.938 0.174 146 4.208 0.174 152 5.846 0.174 153 7.248 0.186 156 6.319 0.186 157 6.117 0.174 159 5.181 0.186 160 6.763 0.174 161 6.396 0.174 163 6.417 0.174 167 6.988 0.174 169 6.519 0.186 170 6.083 0.174

174 6.071 0.174 176 6.810 0.186 177 4.854 0.174 182 3.900 0.186 183 6.067 0.201 184 5.786 0.186 186 5.575 0.174 189 6.646 0.174 193 5.733 0.174

124

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix C: Mean MCP scores for all MT families

F am ily Average MCP Score Standard Error

1 3.750 0.149

4 3.144 0.149

8 3.362 0.138

10 4.100 0.138

13 3.380 0.164

16 4.305 0.138

20 3.367 0.129

21 1.910 0.138

25 4.024 0.138

28 3.921 0.129

34 3.044 0.149

38 3.733 0.138

39 3.433 0.149

40 3.763 0.129

43 2.954 0.129

44 3.804 0.129

46 3.110 0.138

47 2.613 0.129

49 3.325 0.129

53 3.240 0.164

54 4.213 0.164

61 3.710 0.138

62 4.083 0.149 71 2.567 0.129

72 2.706 0.149

74 3.307 0.164

77 2.789 0.149

78 4.506 0.149

125

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix C (c o n ’t)

Fam ily Average MCP Score Standard Error 80 2.756 0.149 81 3.796 0.129 84 4.057 0.138 86 3.978 0.149 90 3.943 0.138 91 3.233 0.138 93 3.583 0.129 94 3.371 0.138 98 2.692 0.183 99 3.852 0.138 100 4.389 0.149 101 3.229 0.129 102 3.375 0.129 103 3.111 0.149 111 3.321 0.129 113 3.217 0.183 114 2.556 0.149 115 4.552 0.138 119 2.652 0.138 121 3.829 0.129 123 4.361 0.149 127 3.200 0.138 129 4.081 0.138 135 3.524 0.138 140 3.467 0.164 143 4.104 0.129 144 3.767 0.149 145 4.548 0.138

126

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix C (c o n ’t)

Fam ily Average MCP Score Standard Error 146 3.790 0.138 148 4.292 0.129 149 3.983 0.149 151 4.524 0.138 152 4.167 0.164 154 3.207 0.164 156 2.983 0.183 157 3.119 0.138 160 2.952 0.138 161 2.889 0.149 162 3.808 0.129 164 3.933 0.138 166 3.519 0.138 167 4.200 0.149 172 3.028 0.149 173 3.690 0.138

174 3.744 0.149 182 2.846 0.129 184 3.489 0.149 185 3.657 0.138 187 2.805 0.138 191 4.086 0.138 193 3.476 0.138

127

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix D: Least square means of gene expression due to age,

experience, or age*experience.1

Age

Gene Age Estimate Std.Err.

18w I -0.469 0.050

18w M -0.482 0.054

amn I -2.016 0.044

amn M -1.946 0.048

ato I -2.354 0.032

ato M -2.347 0.033

CaMKII I 0.133 0.024

CaM KII M 0.189 0.026

cpo I -1.233 0.046

cpo M -1.095 0.050

crl I -0.200 0.031

crl M -0.155 0.034

dnc I 1.779 0.044

dnc M 1.825 0.048

dsx I -0.701 0.041

dsx M -0.525 0.043

eag I -0.266 0.032

eag M -0.164 0.035

eg I -1.765 0.044

eg M -2.208 0.047 fru I -1.296 0.036 fru M -1.111 0.038

hom er I -0.075 0.041

homer M -0.150 0.045

1 Estimates reported are log transformed

128

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix D (co n ’t) Age Gene Age Estimate Std.Err. Kr-hl I -0.444 0.039 Kr-hl M -0.286 0.041 nonA I -0.853 0.052 nonA M -0.817 0.055 para I 0.259 0.033 para M 0.361 0.036 pie I 0.667 0.036 pie M -0.154 0.040 pros I -0.048 0.029 pros M 0.091 0.031 qtc I 11.528 0.064 qtc M 11.371 0.070 rut I -0.133 0.055 rut M 0.032 0.060 Sh I 0.065 0.060 Sh M 0.083 0.065 tko I -0.956 0.048 tko M -0.764 0.052 to I -1.147 0.037 to M -0.196 0.040 tra I -0.855 0.029 tra M -0.846 0.031 tra2 I -0.988 0.072 tra2 M -1.023 0.078

y I -1.698 0.029 y M -2.088 0.032

129

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix D (co n ’t) Experience Gene Experience Estimate Std.Err. 18w N -0.439 0.050 18w E -0.512 0.054 amn N -1.987 0.044 amn E -1.975 0.048 ato N -2.341 0.032 ato E -2.360 0.033 CaMKII N 0.151 0.024 CaMKII E 0.171 0.026 cpo N -1.155 0.046 cpo E -1.172 0.050 crl N -0.199 0.031 crl E -0.156 0.034 dnc N 1.818 0.044 dnc E 1.785 0.048 dsx N -0.630 0.041 dsx E -0.596 0.043 eag N -0.215 0.032 eag E -0.216 0.035

eg N -1.961 0.044 eg E -2.013 0.047 fru N -1.175 0.037 fru E -1.232 0.037 homer N -0.096 0.041 homer E -0.129 0.045 Kr-hl E -0.329 0.041 Kr-hl N -0.401 0.039

130

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix D (c o n ’t)

Experience Gene Experience Estimate Std.Err. nonA N -0.802 0.052

nonA E -0.869 0.055 para N 0.289 0.033 para E 0.331 0.036 pie N 0.275 0.036 pie E 0.238 0.040 pros N -0.005 0.029 pros E 0.048 0.031 qtc N 11.435 0.064

qtc E 11.463 0.070 rut N -0.045 0.055 rut E -0.055 0.060 Sh N 0.133 0.060 Sh E 0.016 0.065 tko N -0.851 0.048 tko E -0.869 0.052 to N -0.723 0.037 to E -0.621 0.040 tra N -0.847 0.029 tra E -0.853 0.031 tra2 N -0.926 0.072 tra2 E -1.086 0.078

y N -1.897 0.029 y E -1.889 0.032

131

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix D (c o n ’t) Age *Experience Gene Age Experience Estimate Std.Err. 18w I N -0.456 0.070 18w I E -0.482 0.070 18w M N -0.422 0.070 18w M E -0.542 0.082 amn I N -1.963 0.063 amn I E -2.069 0.063 amn M N -2.011 0.063 amn M E -1.881 0.074 ato I N -2.315 0.047 ato I E -2.393 0.045 ato M N -2.367 0.045 ato M E -2.327 0.049 CaMKII I N 0.134 0.034 CaMKII IE 0.132 0.034 CaMKII MN 0.168 0.034

CaMKII M E 0.210 0.040 cpo I N -1.228 0.065 cpo I E -1.238 0.065 cpo M N -1.083 0.065 cpo M E -1.107 0.076 crl I N -0.219 0.044 crl I E -0.180 0.044 crl M N -0.179 0.044 crl M E -0.131 0.052

132

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix D (c o n ’t)

Age *Experience Gene Age Experience Estimate Std.Err. dnc I N 1.788 0.063 dnc I E 1.769 0.063 dnc M N 1.849 0.063 dnc M E 1.802 0.073 dsx I N -0.715 0.058 dsx I E -0.688 0.058 dsx M N -0.546 0.058 dsx M E -0.503 0.064 eag I N -0.247 0.045 eag I E -0.285 0.045 eag M N -0.182 0.045 eag M E -0.146 0.053

eg I N -1.745 0.062 eg I E -1.785 0.062 eg M N -2.176 0.062 eg M E -2.241 0.072 fru I N -1.230 0.052 fru I E -1.361 0.050 fru M N -1.121 0.052 fru M E -1.102 0.055 homer I N -0.050 0.058 homer I E -0.101 0.058 homer M N -0.143 0.058 homer M E -0.158 0.068

133

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix D (c o n ’t)

Age *Experience Gene Age Experience Estimate Std.Err. Kr-hl I N -0.426 0.056 Kr-hl I E -0.462 0.056 Kr-hl M N -0.375 0.056 Kr-hl M E -0.197 0.062 nonA I N -0.824 0.074 nonA I E -0.882 0.074 nonA M N -0.780 0.074 nonA M E -0.855 0.082 para I N 0.258 0.046 para I E 0.260 0.046 para M N 0.319 0.046 para M E 0.402 0.054 pie I N 0.677 0.051 pie I E 0.657 0.051 pie M N -0.127 0.051 pie M E -0.181 0.060 pros IN -0.056 0.041 pros I E -0.040 0.041 pros M N 0.045 0.041 pros M E 0.136 0.048 qtc I N 11.543 0.091 qtc I E 11.512 0.091

qtc M N 11.328 0.091 qtc M E 11.414 0.107

134

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix D (c o n ’t)

Age * Experience

Gene Age Experience Estimate Std. Err. rut I N -0.141 0.078 rut I E -0.125 0.078 rut M N 0.051 0.078 rut M E 0.014 0.091 Sh I N 0.141 0.085 Sh I E -0.011 0.085 Sh M N 0.124 0.085 Sh M E 0.042 0.099 tko I N -0.958 0.067 tko I E -0.954 0.067 tko M N -0.743 0.067 tko M E -0.785 0.079 to I N -1.216 0.052 to I E -1.078 0.052 to M N -0.229 0.052 to M E -0.163 0.061 tra I N -0.857 0.041 tra I E -0.852 0.041 tra MN -0.838 0.041 tra M E -0.854 0.048 tra2 I N -0.902 0.101 tra2 I E -1.075 0.101 tra2 M N -0.950 0.101 tra2 M E -1.097 0.119

135

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Appendix D (c o n ’t)

Age * Experience Gene Age Experience Estimate Std. Err.

y I N -1.715 0.041 y I E -1.682 0.041 y M N -2.079 0.041 y M E -2.096 0.049

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Curriculum Vitae Elizabeth Anne Ruedi

Work Address Program in Ecology and Evolution University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign 2414-L Institute of Genomic Biology, MC-195 1206 W. Gregory Drive Urbana, Illinois 61801 (217) 333-5887 (lab) (217)244-1781 (fax) Email: [email protected]

E d u catio n : PhD, Program in Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Illinois at Urbana- Champaign—July 2007. T hesis: Natural variation and gene expression in the male courtship progression o f Drosophila melanogaster. Coursework included: Ecological Genetics; Northern Blot Techniques; Genes and Behavior; Adaptive Genetic Variation in the Wild; Applied Statistical Methods; Genomic Analysis of ; Design and Analysis of Biological Experiments; Interface of Ecology and Evolution; Applied Bioinformatics; Teaching College Biology

B. A. with Biology and English Majors, Lake Forest College. Lake Forest, Illinois— May 2001. Senior Thesis: Reverse Sexual Size and Shape Dimorphism in North American A ccipiter Hawks.

Publications: Hughes, K.A., Ayroles, J.F., Reedy M.M., Drnevich, J.D., Rowe, K.C., Ruedi, E.A., Caceres, C.E. and Paige, K.N. (2006). “Segregating Variation in the Transcriptome: Cis Regulation and Additivity of Effects.” Genetics 173: 1347- 1355.

Olendorf, R, Reudi, B. [s/c], and Hughes, K.A. (2004) “Primers for 12 polymorphic microsatellite DNA loci from the guppy (Poecilia reticulata).” M olecular Ecology N otes 4 (4): 668-671.

Drnevich, J. M., Reedy, M. M., Ruedi, E. A., Rodriguez-Zas, S., and Hughes, K. A. (2004). “Quantitative evolutionary genomics: Differential gene expression associated with male competitive reproductive success in Drosophila melanogaster. ” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 271: 2267- 2273.

137

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Publications (con’t): Ruedi, E. A., Owusu, Y. N., and Dixon, K. (2003). “Male courtship intensity: a new method of Drosophila courtship measurement.” Drosophila Information Services 86.

Presentations Given at Professional Symposia: “Natural genetic variation in the male courtship behavior of Drosophila melanogaster.” E. A. Ruedi and K. A. Hughes. Animal Behavior Society Meeting. Snowbird, Utah. August 2005.

“Evidence for Sexually Antagonistic Interactions in the Genetic Makeup of Drosophila melanogaster.” E. Ruedi. 5th Annual Graduate Student Symposium. Presented by Graduate Students in Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. Urbana, Illinois. February 2003.

“Genetic Influence on the Courtship Behavior of Two Species of Cactophilic Drosophila: Preliminary Data and Future Research Plans.” E. Ruedi. 4lh Annual Graduate Student Symposium. Presented by Graduate Students in Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. Urbana, Illinois. February 2002.

Posters Presented at Professional Symposia and Seminars: “Natural genetic variation in the male courtship behavior of Drosophila melanogaster .” E. A. Ruedi and K. A. Hughes. Gordon Conference: Genes and Behavior. Ventura, California. February 2006.

“Natural genetic variation in the male courtship intensity of Drosophila melanogaster.” E. A. Ruedi and K. A. Hughes. Midwest Drosophila Meeting 2004. Monticello, Illinois. October 2004.

“Genetic influence on male courtship behavior in two species of cactophilic Drosophila.” E. A. Ruedi, C. Tymczyna-Cobbs, K. A. Hughes. 9th International Behavioral Ecology Congress. Montreal, Quebec, Canada. July 2002.

“Reverse Sexual Size and Shape Dimorphism in North American Accipiter Hawks.” E. A. Ruedi and A. Houde. Evolution 2001. Knoxville, Tennessee. June 2001.

138

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Research Experience Research Interests: Behavior, Genetics, and Evolution

Laboratory Assistant - Dr. Kimberly Hughes. University of Illinois. August 2001- Present Responsibilities: Care of Drosophila melanogaster, D. mettleri, and D. micromettleri; experimental design and execution; undergraduate employee training; data collection and analysis.

Research Assistant-Dr. Kimberly Hughes and Dr. Robert Olendorf. Summer 2003 and 2004. Responsibilities: Primer optimization; DNA extraction; running Polymerase Chain Reactions; gel electrophoresis.

Laboratory Assistant - Dr. Anne Houde. Lake Forest College. January 2001- April 2001 Responsibilities: Care of guppies ( Poecilia reticulata) for Dr. Houde’s experiments; care of Betta fish (Betta splendins ), crayfish, and house crickets (Acheta domestica) for Animal Behavior

Skills: Behavioral Observation DNA Extraction RNA Extraction PCR Agarose gel electrophoresis Northern Blot execution Reverse Transcription Quantitative real-time PCR Primer design/optimization RNA amplification and labeling for microarray analysis Experience with: Microsoft Word, Excel, PowerPoint; JMP; SAS; R.

Teaching Experience: Teaching Assistant - Integrative Biology 405, Ecological Genetics; Fall 2006 Responsibilities: Preparing and giving three 1 xh hour lectures, designing and implementing primary literature discussions, holding office hours

Teaching Assistant - Integrative Biology 150/199, Organismal and Evolutionary Biology Merit Scholars Program; Fall 2005 Responsibilities: Running a discussion section for high-achieving students planning a career in science. Focus was on group work and independent problem solving.

139

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Teaching Experience (con’t): Teaching Assistant - Integrative Biology 201, Genetics and Evolution; Spring 2005 Responsibilities: Running laboratory sections focusing on evolution, systematics, and genetics.

Teaching Assistant - Integrative Biology 150, Organismal and Evolutionary Biology; Fall 2003, Spring 2004, Fall 2004; Fall 2006 Responsibilities: Running four discussion sections dedicated to expanding the students’ understanding of lecture materials

Teaching Assistant - Integrative Biology 151, Laboratory on Organismal and Evolutionary Biology; Fall 2002, Spring 2003 Responsibilities: Running laboratory sections designed for non-IB majors

Peer Teacher - Ecology and Evolution, Bio 220. Dr. Anne Houde. Lake Forest College. August 2000- December 2000. Responsibilities: Orchestrating group discussions; lecture planning and lecturing; holding office hours; reviewing and editing student papers.

Teaching Honors: John G. & Evelyn Hartman Heiligenstein Outstanding Teaching Assistant in Integrative Biology 150, UIUC. Spring 2005

Incomplete List of Teachers Ranked as Excellent, University of Illinois at Urbana- Champaign—Fall 2002, Spring 2003, Fall 2003, Spring 2004, Fall 2004, Spring 2005, Fall 2006

Academic Honors: National Science Foundation Doctoral Dissertation Improvement Grant. 2006.

Magna Cum Laude. Lake Forest College Graduation 2001.

Honors in Biology. Lake Forest College, B.A. 2001.

Beta Beta Beta National Biological Honor Society. 2001 - Present.

Sigma Xi Society for Excellence in Scientific Research. 2001 - 2004.

140

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Service and Committee Work: Graduate Students in Ecology and Evolutionary Biology (GEEB) President—Fall 2006- Present

GEEB Vice President—Fall 2006-Spring 2006

PEEB Steering Committee Student Representative—Fall 2004- Spring 2006

GEEB Symposium Committee Member—Fall 2002- February 2003

Social Director—Fall 2002- Fall 2003

Ecolunch Coordinator—Spring 2002- Fall 2002

Reading Group Coordinator—Fall 2001-Spring 2002

Professional References: Kimberly A. Hughes, Ph.D. (Associate Professor) Department of Animal Biology University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign 2404 Institute of Genomic Biology, me-195 1206 W. Gregory Drive Urbana, Illinois 61801 (217) 244-6632 (217) 244-1781 (fax) email: [email protected]

Gene Robinson, Ph.D. G. William Arends Professor of Integrative Biology Director, Neuroscience Program Chair, Genomics of Neural and Behavioral Plasticity Theme, Institute for Genomic Biology Department of Entomology University of Illinois 505 S. Goodwin Ave., Urbana, IL 61801 (217) 265-0309 (217) 244-3499 (fax) email: [email protected]

141

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Professional References (con’t): Kevin Dixon, Ph.D. (Teaching Laboratory Specialist) School of Integrative Biology University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign 300 Natural History Building, me-120 505 South Goodwin Urbana, Illinois 61801 (217) 244- 7350 (217) 244- 1224 (fax) email: kdixon@ 1 ife.uiuc.edu

Anne E. Houde, Ph.D. (Associate Professor, Chairperson of Biology Department) Department of Biology Lake Forest College 555 N. Sheridan Road Lake Forest, Illinois 60045 (847) 735- 6043 email: [email protected]

142

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.