Phonological and Prosodic Bootstrapping 447

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Phonological and Prosodic Bootstrapping 447 Phonological and Prosodic Bootstrapping 447 than do French and English. Unfortunately, phoneme Phonological and Prosodic discrimination studies with bilingual infants are not abundant, especially those including sensitive meth- Bootstrapping ods such as tracking eye movements or measuring brain responses. Research is yet to demonstrate the Around their first birthday, most infants start pro- full extent of the differences and similarities in how ducing words. However, it is typically not until chil- phoneme discrimination develops in bilingual versus dren have acquired some 50 words in their produc- monolingual children. tive vocabulary that they start combining words into utterances. Early theories of syntactic development— Paola Escudero based primarily on corpus work—were consequently University of Western Sydney developed to account for children’s early (syntacti- cally deprived) utterances. By contrast, more recent See Also: Bilingual Language Development; Bilingualism: work has started to focus on children’s comprehen- Interactions Between Languages; Early Second Language sion of grammatical structures at ages that precede Acquisition; Early Word Learning; Electrophysiology productive evidence of syntactic development. Such Methods in Language Development; Phonological work has suggested that, within their first two to three Development; Speech Perception and Categorization; years of life, infants gain sensitivity to many aspects of Statistical Learning. the syntactic structure of their native language. This sensitivity is thus in place even before children have Further Readings acquired a rich vocabulary that may help them tune Escudero, P. “Speech Processing in Bilingual and into the complexities of the ambient language. Multilingual Listeners.” In The Handbook of Laboratory The relatively early sensitivity to grammati- Phonology, A. C. Cohn, C. Fougeron, and M. K. cal structure has raised the question of what cues Huffman, eds. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, enable young, preverbal children to acquire grammar. 2011. According to one account, prosodic information, and Escudero, P., T. Benders, and K. Wanrooij. “Enhanced phonological cues more broadly, immediately per- Bimodal Distributions Facilitate the Learning of ceivable from the speech signal, may be the key ingre- Second Language Vowels.” Journal of the Acoustical dient providing infants’ initial bootstrap into the lan- Society of America, v.130 (2011). guage. The central tenet of this hypothesis, typically Kuhl, P. K., K. A. Williams, F. Lacerda, K. N. Stevens, and referred to as the phonological bootstrapping hypoth- B. Lindblom. “Linguistic Experience Alters Phonetic esis, or bootstrapping from the signal, is based on the Perception in Infants by 6 Months of Age.” Science, idea that crude structural properties of syntax are sig- v.255 (1992). naled by their phonological correlates. That is, chil- Maye, J., J. F. Werker, and L. A. Gerken. “Infant Sensitivity dren can learn certain aspects of the structure of their to Distributional Information Can Affect Phonetic language through an analysis of the surface form of Discrimination.” Cognition, v.82 (2002). the incoming speech signal. Because this approach to Polka, L. and J. F. Werker. “Developmental Changes in bootstrapping into one’s native language requires no Perception of Nonnative Vowel Contrasts.” Journal prior linguistic knowledge, it can potentially explain of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and the earliest steps of language acquisition. Performance, v.20 (1994). The notion that prosodic information in the Rivera-Gaxiola, M., J. Silva-Pereyra, and P. K. Kuhl. speech stream contains cues to syntactic structure “Brain Potentials to Native and Non-Native Speech was first introduced in the 1980s by researchers such Contrasts in 7- and 11-Month-Old American Infants.” as Lila Gleitman, James Morgan, Elissa Newport, Developmental Science, v.8 (2005). Ann Peters, and Eric Warren and forms the basis of Werker, J. “Perceptual Foundations of Bilingual the current phonological bootstrapping hypothesis. Acquisition in Infancy.” Annals of the New York Originally called the prosodic bootstrapping hypoth- © Brooks, Patricia J.; Kempe, Vera, Apr 22, 2014, Encyclopedia of Language Development SAGE Publications, Thousand Oaks, ISBN: 9781483346434 Academy of Sciences, v.1251 (2012). esis, a term introduced by Steven Pinker, the hypoth- Werker, J. F., K. Corcoran, C. T. Fennell, and C. L. Stager. esis mainly focused on infants’ use of prosody (i.e., “Infants’ Ability to Learn Phonetically Similar Words: information such as pitch modulation and rhythmic Effects of Age and Vocabulary Size.” Infancy, v.3 (2002). variation) to bootstrap into the structural properties 448 Phonological and Prosodic Bootstrapping of the language. Later accounts also have started prosodic cues correctly identify clause boundaries to incorporate nonprosodic information available approximately 88 percent of the time. from the speech signal, such as phonetic and phono- Of course, a correlation between the prosodic tactic cues. To integrate this broader view, Morgan information and syntactic structure does not imply and Katherine Demuth referred to the phonological that the two are perfectly aligned. Perhaps one of bootstrapping hypothesis, which is currently the most the most frequently raised criticisms of the prosodic commonly used term. bootstrapping hypothesis concerns the caveat that The idea that a purely perceptual analysis of the there is no one-to-one correspondence between pro- speech signal may reveal grammatical structure rests sodic and syntactic phrases. For example, although on a few critical assumptions: that (1) structural prosodic boundaries typically correspond to syn- properties of sentences are reliably correlated with tactic boundaries, the reverse is not necessarily the prosodic or phonetic features; (2) infants are sensitive case. More specifically, while clause boundaries are to the acoustic correlates of these prosodic features; fairly reliably associated with prosodic cues in child- and (3) infants are able to rely on these acoustic cor- directed speech, intermediate phrasal structure has relates of prosodic features during speech perception much weaker prosodic prominence. A sentence such early in life. We here address each of these assump- as He is eating, for instance, is encapsulated in a single tions and discuss the plausibility of the phonological prosodic unit even though the pronoun is a clear syn- bootstrapping hypothesis in more detail. tactic constituent on its own, leaving the major syn- tactic boundary separating the subject from the verb Relationship Between Phonology and Syntax phrase prosodically unmarked. This suggests that not In order for phonology to cue syntax, prosodic and all syntactic boundaries may be learnable from the syntactic phrases need to reliably coincide, and phonological structure. Nonetheless, the phonologi- phrasal prosody should provide language learn- cal information can provide children with the first ers with cues that help them divide sentences into step to decode the signal and can, as such, be used rudimentary prosodic–syntactic units. Prosodic as a first proxy to syntactic analyses. That is, even if units should furthermore be organized hierarchi- some syntactic boundaries will have no clear prosodic cally according to the depth of their acoustic mark- marker, and will hence not be recovered on first pass, ers. Evidence suggests that this is indeed the case. a phonological analysis of the speech signal would Specifically, phonological words are grouped into provide learners with at least some reliable cues that phonological phrases, which in turn constitute into- may guide their early syntactic analysis of sentences. national phrases. Generally speaking, each of these Once bootstrapped, such initial syntactic informa- levels correlates with syntactic levels of utterances, tion can subsequently be used to analyze the input in such that the prosodic structure might provide a a more fine-grained fashion. natural bracketing of speech into syntactically rel- Prosodic phrase boundaries are not the only source evant phrases and clauses. For example, intonational of information infants could access in the speech sig- phrases—corresponding to clauses or propositions nal to cue the syntactic structure. Within phonologi- within a sentence—tend to be universally marked by cal phrases, the position of prosodic prominence is phonological cues such as phrase-initial strengthen- correlated with word order and could thus provide a ing, phrase-final lengthening, pitch declination, and perceptually available surface cue to the typology of pauses. Phonological phrases—intermediate pro- the language. For example, prosodic prominence is sodic phrases, corresponding to syntactic phrases phrase initial in languages where the object follows typically consisting of a few function and content the verb (e.g., [they] rideV bikesO) and the noun fol- words—are indicated by similar, but reduced, acous- lows the preposition (e.g., inP storesN), such as Eng- tic correlates. Although the exact set of prosodic lish or French, and is marked by a pitch accent on cues that serve to delimit clauses and phrases differs the initial word. By contrast, prosodic prominence is across languages, a reliable correspondence between phrase final in languages where the object precedes © Brooks, Patricia J.; Kempe, Vera, Apr 22, 2014, Encyclopedia of Language Development SAGE Publications, Thousand Oaks, ISBN: 9781483346434
Recommended publications
  • Unsupervised Syntactic Chunking with Acoustic Cues: Computational Models for Prosodic Bootstrapping
    Unsupervised syntactic chunking with acoustic cues: computational models for prosodic bootstrapping John K Pate ([email protected]) Sharon Goldwater ([email protected]) School of Informatics, University of Edinburgh 10 Crichton St., Edinburgh EH8 9AB, UK Abstract tags or punctuation. However, infants do have ac- cess to certain cues that have not been well explored Learning to group words into phrases with- by NLP researchers focused on grammar induction out supervision is a hard task for NLP sys- from text. In particular, we consider the cues to syn- tems, but infants routinely accomplish it. We hypothesize that infants use acoustic cues to tactic structure that might be available from prosody prosody, which NLP systems typically ignore. (roughly, the structure of speech conveyed through To evaluate the utility of prosodic information rhythm and intonation) and its acoustic realization. for phrase discovery, we present an HMM- The idea that prosody provides important ini- based unsupervised chunker that learns from tial cues for grammar acquisition is known as the only transcribed words and raw acoustic cor- prosodic bootstrapping hypothesis, and is well- relates to prosody. Unlike previous work on established in the field of language acquisition unsupervised parsing and chunking, we use (Gleitman and Wanner, 1982). Experimental work neither gold standard part-of-speech tags nor punctuation in the input. Evaluated on the has provided strong support for this hypothesis, for Switchboard corpus, our model outperforms example by showing that infants begin learning ba- several baselines that exploit either lexical or sic rhythmic properties of their language prenatally prosodic information alone, and, despite pro- (Mehler et al., 1988) and that 9-month-olds use ducing a flat structure, performs competitively prosodic cues to distinguish verb phrases from non- with a state-of-the-art unsupervised lexical- constituents (Soderstrom et al., 2003).
    [Show full text]
  • 1. Introduction
    University of Groningen Specific language impairment in Dutch de Jong, Jan IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below. Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Publication date: 1999 Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database Citation for published version (APA): de Jong, J. (1999). Specific language impairment in Dutch: inflectional morphology and argument structure. s.n. Copyright Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Take-down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum. Download date: 25-09-2021 Specific Language Impairment in Dutch: Inflectional Morphology and Argument Structure Jan de Jong Copyright ©1999 by Jan de Jong Printed by Print Partners Ipskamp, Enschede Groningen Dissertations in Linguistics 28 ISSN 0928-0030 Specific Language Impairment in Dutch: Inflectional Morphology and Argument Structure Proefschrift ter verkrijging van het doctoraat in de letteren aan de Rijksuniversiteit Groningen op gezag van de Rector Magnificus, dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Bootstrapping the Syntactic Bootstrapper
    Bootstrapping the Syntactic Bootstrapper Anne Christophe, Isabelle Dautriche, Alex de Carvalho, and Perrine Brusini In 1985, Laudau & Gleitman first outlined the syntactic bootstrapping hypothesis, in their book Language and Experience – Evidence from a blind child, followed in 1990 by Lila Gleitman’s article “The structural sources of verb meanings” (in Language Acquisition). They proposed that young children might learn the meaning of words (and in particular, verbs), by paying attention to the syntactic structures in which these words occur. This highly counter- intuitive hypothesis earned Lila a lot of flak from the community, and paved the way for the broader research framework that is now known as ‘synergies in language acquisition’: the general idea that even impoverished knowledge in one area of language might help children refine their representations in another (e.g., even a very crude proto-lexicon will help you learn your phonological system, see for instance Martin, Peperkamp, & Dupoux, 2013). Thirty years later, syntactic bootstrapping is widely accepted and has been supported by many experimental results – even though a lot remains to be discovered. In this paper, we will examine the ways in which very young children may start gathering the relevant syntactic facts on which to base their acquisition of word meanings – or, in other words, how to ‘bootstrap the syntactic bootstrapper’. In order to exploit syntactic structure to figure out the meaning of unknown words, very young children have to be able to recover at least some elements of the syntactic structure of the sentences they hear, even when they do not know all their content words.
    [Show full text]
  • Mehler Et Al. 31-05-00 a RATIONALIST APPROACH to THE
    Mehler et al. 31-05-00 A RATIONALIST APPROACH TO THE STUDY OF DEVELOPMENT. Humans are helpless at birth but in a few years they learn a great many things, gain increasing autonomy, and acquire motor and perceptual skills as well as language and numerical abilities. How does such a change come about? Observers often marvel at the speed and predictability of growth. For instance, the tri-dimensional organization of space emerges sometime between 16 and 18 weeks after birth (see Held, Birch, & Gwiazda, 1980). Motor learning unfolds predictably, bipedal gait being attained at the end of the first year of life (see Adolph, 1995). Likewise, language unfolds after only a few years of contact with the environment (see Brown, 1973). Major deviations from this schedule usually signal neurological impairment. Psychologists, borrowing terms from biology, tend to refer to these changes as development. It was biologists who first began to describe the predictable changes that take place as the fertilized egg progressively grows into the fully-fledged organism. In this sense, the term development is theoretically neutral : it is simply a word that describes the changes that take place during growth. A survey of the first half of the twentieth century shows that behavioral scientists slighted the study of development until Piaget’s influence began to be felt, roughly, at the time when Cognitive Psychology was beginning to gain a wider audience. Up until that time, most psychologists believed that living creatures learn by extracting regularities from their environment. Association was the mechanism offered to explain learning. It is not readily obvious, however, how association can explain the origin of species-specific behavioral dispositions (see Tinbergen, 1951; and also Gallistel, 1990).
    [Show full text]
  • Epenthesis and Prosodic Structure in Armenian
    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA Los Angeles Epenthesis and prosodic structure in Armenian: A diachronic account A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy in Indo-European Studies by Jessica L. DeLisi 2015 © Copyright by Jessica L. DeLisi 2015 ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION Epenthesis and prosodic structure in Armenian: A diachronic account by Jessica L. DeLisi Doctor of Philosophy in Indo-European Studies University of California, Los Angeles, 2015 Professor H. Craig Melchert, Chair In this dissertation I will attempt to answer the following question: why does Classical Armenian have three dierent reexes for the Proto-Armenian epenthetic vowel word- initially before old Proto-Indo-European consonant clusters? Two of the vowels, e and a, occur in the same phonological environment, and even in doublets (e.g., Classical ełbayr beside dialectal ałbär ‘brother’). The main constraint driving this asymmetry is the promotion of the Sonority Sequenc- ing Principle in the grammar. Because sibilants are more sonorous than stops, the promo- tion of the Sonority Sequencing Principle above the Strict Layer Hypothesis causes speak- ers to create a semisyllable to house the sibilant extraprosodically. This extraprosodic structure is not required for old consonant-resonant clusters since they already conform to the Sonority Sequencing Principle. Because Armenian has sonority-sensitive stress, the secondary stress placed on word-initial epenthetic vowels triggers a vowel change in all words without extraprosodic structure, i.e. with the old consonant-resonant clusters. Therefore Proto-Armenian */@łbayR/ becomes Classical Armenian [èł.báyR] ‘brother,’ but Proto-Armenian */<@s>tipem/ with extraprosodic <@s> becomes [<@s>.tì.pém] ‘I rush’ because the schwa is outside the domain of stress assignment.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 from Sound to Syntax: the Prosodic Bootstrapping Of
    From Sound to Syntax: The Prosodic Bootstrapping of Clauses Item Type text; Electronic Dissertation Authors Hawthorne, Kara Eileen Publisher The University of Arizona. Rights Copyright © is held by the author. Digital access to this material is made possible by the University Libraries, University of Arizona. Further transmission, reproduction or presentation (such as public display or performance) of protected items is prohibited except with permission of the author. Download date 25/09/2021 05:22:33 Link to Item http://hdl.handle.net/10150/283672 1 FROM SOUND TO SYNTAX: THE PROSODIC BOOTSTRAPPING OF CLAUSES by Kara Hawthorne _____________________ Copyright © Kara Hawthorne 2013 A Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of the DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements For the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY In the Graduate College of THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA 2013 2 THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA GRADUATE COLLEGE As members of the Dissertation Committee, we certify that we have read the dissertation prepared by Kara Hawthorne entitled From Sound to Syntax: The Prosodic Bootstrapping of Clauses and recommend that it be accepted as fulfilling the dissertation requirement for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy _______________________________________________________________________ Date: 11/13/2012 Dr. LouAnn Gerken _______________________________________________________________________ Date: 11/13/2012 Dr. Rebecca Gomez _______________________________________________________________________ Date: 11/13/2012 Dr. Heidi Harley _______________________________________________________________________ Date: 11/13/2012 Dr. Diane Ohala Final approval and acceptance of this dissertation is contingent upon the candidate’s submission of the final copies of the dissertation to the Graduate College. I hereby certify that I have read this dissertation prepared under my direction and recommend that it be accepted as fulfilling the dissertation requirement.
    [Show full text]
  • PERSPECTIVES Child Language Acquisition: Why Universal Grammar Doesn’T Help
    PERSPECTIVES Child language acquisition: Why universal grammar doesn’t help BEN AMBRIDGE JULIAN M. PINE ELENA V. M. LIEVEN University of Liverpool University of Liverpool University of Manchester In many different domains of language acquisition, there exists an apparent learnability prob- lem to which innate knowledge of some aspect of universal grammar (UG) has been proposed as a solution. The present article reviews these proposals in the core domains of (i) identifying syntactic categories, (ii) acquiring basic morphosyntax, (iii) structure dependence, (iv) subja- cency, and (v) the binding principles. We conclude that, in each of these domains, the innate UG- specified knowledge posited does not, in fact, simplify the task facing the learner. Keywords: binding principles, child language acquisition, frequent frames, parameter setting, prosodic bootstrapping, semantic bootstrapping, structure dependence, subjacency, syntax, mor- phosyntax, universal grammar 1. Introduction. Many leading theories of child language acquisition assume innate knowledge of universal grammar (e.g. of syntactic categories such as noun and verb, constraints/principles such as structure dependence and subjacency, and parameters such as the head-direction parameter). Many authors have argued either for or against uni- versal grammar (UG) on a priori grounds such as learnability (e.g. whether the child can acquire a system of infinite productive capacity from exposure to a finite set of ut- terances generated by that system) or evolutionary plausibility (e.g. linguistic principles are too abstract to confer a reproductive advantage). Our goal in this article is to take a step back from such arguments, and instead to con- sider the question of whether the individual components of innate UG knowledge pro- posed in the literature (e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Perception and Acquisition of Linguistic Rhythm by Infants
    Speech Communication 41 (2003) 233–243 www.elsevier.com/locate/specom Perception and acquisition of linguistic rhythm by infants Thierry Nazzi a,*, Franck Ramus b a Laboratoire Cognition et Developpement, CNRS, Universite Paris 5, 71, Avenue Edouard Vaillant, 92774 Boulogne Billancourt Cedex, France b Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, UK Abstract In the present paper, we address the issue of the emergence in infancy of speech segmentation procedures that were found to be specific to rhythmic classes of languages in adulthood. These metrical procedures, which segment fluent speech into its constitutive word sequence, are crucial for the acquisition by infants of the words of their native lan- guage. We first present a prosodic bootstrapping proposal according to which the acquisition of these metrical seg- mentation procedures would be based on an early sensitivity to rhythm (and rhythmic classes). We then review several series of experiments that have studied infantsÕ ability to discriminate languages between birth and 5 months, in an attempt to specify their sensitivity to rhythm and the implication of rhythm perception in the acquisition of these segmentation procedures. The results presented here establish infantsÕ sensitivity to rhythmic classes (from birth on- wards). They further show an evolution of infantsÕ language discriminations between birth and 5 months which, though not inconsistent with our proposal, nevertheless call for more studies on the possible implication of rhythm in the acquisition of the metrical segmentation procedures. Ó 2002 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction necting of these sound patterns to the lexical representations stored in the lexicon.
    [Show full text]
  • English-Speaking Preschoolers Can Use Phrasal Prosody for Syntactic Parsing
    de Carvalho et al.: JASA Express Letters [http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.4954385] Published Online 23 June 2016 English-speaking preschoolers can use phrasal prosody for syntactic parsing Alex de Carvalho,1,a) Jeffrey Lidz,2 Lyn Tieu,1 Tonia Bleam,2 and Anne Christophe1 1Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique (ENS, EHESS, CNRS), Departement d’Etudes Cognitives, Ecole normale superieure - PSL Research University, 29 rue d’Ulm, 75005 Paris, France [email protected], [email protected], [email protected] 2Department of Linguistics, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland 20742, USA [email protected], [email protected] Abstract: This study tested American preschoolers’ ability to use phrasal prosody to constrain their syntactic analysis of locally ambigu- ous sentences containing noun/verb homophones (e.g., [The baby flies] [hide in the shadows] vs [The baby] [flies his kite], brackets indicate pro- sodic boundaries). The words following the homophone were masked, such that prosodic cues were the only disambiguating information. In an oral completion task, 4- to 5-year-olds successfully exploited the sen- tence’s prosodic structure to assign the appropriate syntactic category to the target word, mirroring previous results in French (but challenging previous English-language results) and providing cross-linguistic evi- dence for the role of phrasal prosody in children’s syntactic analysis. VC 2016 Acoustical Society of America [DDO] Date Received: January 29, 2016 Date Accepted: May 3, 2016 1. Introduction According to the prosodic bootstrapping hypothesis,1 phrasal prosody (the rhythm and melody of speech) may provide a useful source of information for parsing the speech stream into words and phrases.
    [Show full text]
  • Final Thesis
    The Effect of Meter-Syntax Alignment on Sentence Comprehension, Sensorimotor Synchronisation, and Neural Entrainment Courtney Bryce Hilton The University of Sydney Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences A thesis submitted to fulfil requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Abstract Why rhythm in language? And why is linguistic rhythm grasped by means of meter (a temporal grid structure) in the human mind? And specifically, why does the human mind prefer to align meter to language in certain ways and not others? This thesis examines the alignment of meter to syntactic phrase structure and its effect on language comprehension. This is investigated empirically in four experiments, whose results are situated within the relevant linguistic, musicological, cognitive, and neuroscientific literatures. The first two experiments show that meter-syntax alignment indeed affects sentence comprehension, and the second also shows an effect on sensorimotor synchronisation. The third experiment behaviourally replicates the comprehension result while also recording electroencephalography (EEG). This neural measurement shows how delta oscillations track the perceived meter rather than syntactic phrase structure: contradicting some recent theories. The final experiment applies meter-syntax alignment to an algebraic grouping task. By using simpler (better controlled) non-linguistic stimuli, the results in this experiment better constrain the mechanistic interpretation of the results so far. Specifically, I suggest that the effect of meter (and its alignment to syntax) on comprehension is mediated by an effect on short-term/working memory. The broader theoretical and practical implications of these experiments is finally discussed, especially with regard to theories of language processing, music-language parallels, and education.
    [Show full text]
  • Bootstrap” Into Syntax?*
    Cognirion. 45 (1992) 77-100 What sort of innate structure is needed to “bootstrap” into syntax?* Martin D.S. Braine Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Place. 8th Floor, New York. NY 10003, LISA Received March 5. 1990, final revision accepted March 13, 1992 Abstract Braine, M.D.S., 1992. What sort of innate structure is needed to “bootstrap” into syntax? Cognition, 45: 77-100 The paper starts from Pinker’s theory of the acquisition of phrase structure; it shows that it is possible to drop all the assumptions about innate syntactic structure from this theory. These assumptions can be replaced by assumptions about the basic structure of semantic representation available at the outset of language acquisition, without penalizing the acquisition of basic phrase structure rules. Essentially, the role played by X-bar theory in Pinker’s model would be played by the (presumably innate) structure of the language of thought in the revised parallel model. Bootstrapping and semantic assimilation theories are shown to be formally very similar,. though making different primitive assumptions. In their primitives, semantic assimilation theories have the advantage that they can offer an account of the origin of syntactic categories instead of postulating them as primitive. Ways of improving on the semantic assimilation version of Pinker’s theory are considered, including a way of deriving the NP-VP constituent division that appears to have a better fit than Pinker’s to evidence on language variation. Correspondence to: Martin Braine, Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washing- ton Place. 8th Floor, New York, NY 10003.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction
    Published in M. Bowerman and P. Brown (Eds.), 2008 Crosslinguistic Perspectives on Argument Structure: Implications for Learnability, p. 1-26. Majwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum CHAPTER 1 Introduction Melissa Bowerman and Penelope Brown Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics Verbs are the glue that holds clauses together. As elements that encode events, verbs are associated with a core set of semantic participants mat take part in the event. Some of a verb's semantic participants, although not necessarily all, are mapped to roles that are syntactically relevant in the clause, such as subject or direct object; these are the arguments of the verb. For example, in John kicked the ball, 'John' and 'the ball' are semantic participants of the verb kick, and they are also its core syntactic arguments—the subject and the direct object, respectively. Another semantic participant, 'foot', is also understood, but it is not an argument; rather, it is incorpo- rated directly into the meaning of the verb. The array of participants associated with verbs and other predicates, and how these participants are mapped to syntax, are the focus of the Study of ARGUMENT STRUCTURE. At one time, argument structure meant little more than the number of arguments appearing with a verb, for example, one for an intransitive verb, two for a transitive verb. But argument structure has by now taken on a central theoretical position in the study of both language structure and language development. In linguistics, ar- gument structure is seen as a critical interface between the lexical semantic properties of verbs and the morphosyntactic properties of the clauses in which they appear (e.g., Grimshaw, 1990; Goldberg, 1995; Hale & Keyser, 1993; Levin & Rappaport Hovav, 1995; Jackendoff, 1990).
    [Show full text]