1

2

3 Nest material preferences by spotless

4

* 5 Cristina Ruiz-Castellano , Gustavo Tomás, Magdalena Ruiz-Rodríguez and

6 Juan J. Soler

7

8

9 Departamento de Ecología Funcional y Evolutiva, Estación Experimental de Zonas Áridas

10 (EEZA-CSIC), Ctra. Sacramento s/n, La Cañada de San Urbano, E-04120 Almería, Spain.

11

12 * For correspondence: e-mail: [email protected], Tf: +34 950281045 (Ext: 784)

13

1

14 Abstract

15 The avian nest is an essential structure for offspring development. For adults, nest building

16 entails costs in terms of time, energy and exposure to predators and parasites. Amount and

17 diversity of materials used for nest building depend on their availability and functionality in

18 scenarios of sexual selection and parasitism. Green plants and feathers of different colors

19 have been hypothesized to play key roles in offspring protection against pathogens, and we

20 here experimentally assessed spotless ( unicolor) preferences for pigmented

21 vs. unpigmented feathers and for different green plants (aromatic vs. non-aromatic plants)

22 as nest materials. We predicted a preferential selection of unpigmented feathers and

23 aromatic plants according to the antimicrobial properties of these materials described in the

24 literature. We evaluated these predictions during nest building and during egg-laying

25 stages. As expected, starlings preferentially selected unpigmented feathers both before and

26 during egg laying, while aromatic plants were preferentially selected only during the egg-

27 laying stage. These results suggest that starlings prefer nest materials that enhance

28 antimicrobial protection of their offspring. We also discuss some other, non-exclusive

29 functions that might explain the observed preference for nest materials, especially with

30 regard to their potential role in sexual signaling.

31

32 Keywords: Aromatic plants, Feather pigmentation, Nest building behavior, Nest lining

33 feathers, Nest material preference.

34

2

35 Introduction

36 The avian nest is an essential structure that protects offspring from a wide diversity of

37 environmental challenges during development (Hansell 2000; Moreno 2012; Mainwaring et

38 al. 2014). Nest building entails costs in terms of, for instance, expended time and energy, or

39 exposure to predators and parasites (Hansell 2000; Mainwaring and Hartley 2013).

40 Different nest materials have different properties, so are expected to carefully adjust

41 nest-building behavior to optimize the balance between costs and benefits while selecting

42 the more appropriate combination of materials to maximize fitness (Soler et al. 1998;

43 Hansell 2000).

44 Among the several materials employed by birds to build their nests, the use of green

45 plants and feathers has received special attention in evolutionary ecology research (Hansell

46 2000; Dubiec et al. 2013). Green plants, which are used as structural or lining material, may

47 also have several non-exclusive functions in avian nests that include sexual signaling

48 (Fauth et al. 1991; Brouwer and Komdeur 2004; Polo et al. 2004; Veiga et al. 2006;

49 Moreno 2012; Tomás et al. 2013). Yet, the most studied function of green plants is related

50 to the antimicrobial and antiparasitic properties of their volatile secondary compounds and

51 essential oils that protect offspring from pathogens (Clark and Mason 1985; Lafuma et al.

52 2001; Gwinner and Berger 2005; Shutler and Campbell 2007; Mennerat et al. 2009a;

53 Dubiec et al. 2013; Scott-Baumann and Morgan 2015). Through different metabolic routes,

54 many of these plants can also have therapeutic effects improving nestling growth, health, or

55 immunocompetence (Gwinner et al. 2000; Gwinner and Berger 2005; Mennerat et al.

56 2009b). Therefore, the use of green plants in the nests of birds is considered as a form of

57 self-medication (De Roode et al. 2013). Different plants have different compounds, so birds

3

58 may adjust their nest building behavior according to the selective pressures exerted by

59 pathogens.

60 Feathers are another material commonly used in nests, mainly for nest cup

61 lining, but also as structural material (Hansell 2000). Although the most advocated function

62 of feathers in nests is related to thermal insulation (Møller 1984; Lombardo et al. 1995;

63 Hilton et al. 2004; Pinowski et al. 2006; Dawson et al. 2011; Windsor et al. 2013), they can

64 also be involved in sexual signaling (Polo and Veiga 2006; Sanz and García-Navas 2011;

65 García-López de Hierro et al. 2013; Mainwaring et al. 2016) and offspring protection from

66 microbial infections (Soler et al. 2010). These two last functions are respectively based on

67 attractiveness of feathers of different color (Veiga and Polo 2005; Avilés et al. 2010, Sanz

68 and García-Navas 2011), and on the antimicrobial properties of the bacterial communities

69 that grow digesting the keratin of feathers (Peralta-Sánchez et al. 2014; Ruiz-Castellano et

70 al. 2016). These two functions are not mutually exclusive, and we here concentrate our

71 predictions on the hypothetical antimicrobial functionality of feathers. Apparently, bacterial

72 growth (Goldstein et al. 2004; Gunderson et al. 2008) and antimicrobial properties are

73 higher for bacterial communities of unpigmented feathers (Peralta-Sánchez et al. 2014).

74 Thus, birds may preferentially select unpigmented feathers for nest building.

75 Some of these hypothetical effects of feathers and green plants in the nests of birds

76 have been experimentally demonstrated, but evidence of associated adaptive preference is

77 restricted to a few species. For green plants, previous research on European starlings

78 (Sturnus vulgaris) has suggested olfactory discrimination of plant volatiles (Clark and

79 Mason 1987; Gwinner and Berger 2008). Moreover, an observational study concluded that

80 plant species composition of blue tit (Cyanistes caeruleus) nests results from individual

81 preferences of particular species (Mennerat et al. 2009c). With respect to feathers, Peralta-

4

82 Sánchez et al. (2011) found that barn swallows (Hirundo rustica) preferentially selected

83 white feathers (experimentally offered) for lining their nests. Exploring these preferences in

84 some other species, as well as possible variation depending on environmental conditions,

85 would aid understanding of the functional significance of nests and nest building behaviors.

86 Selection of nest materials by birds can be tackled by looking at the final

87 assemblage of materials in nests after considering the availability of different materials in

88 the nest surroundings (Petit et al. 2002; Mennerat et al. 2009c; Pires et al. 2012). However,

89 birds could also collect nest materials far away from their territories (Bailey et al. 2016),

90 which challenges a proper statistical control of nest material availability. Therefore, an

91 experimental approach might be the most suitable way to study the preference for certain

92 nest materials, which we accomplished here by manipulating their availability. During nest

93 building stage, unlimited pigmented and unpigmented feathers, as well as different

94 aromatic and non-aromatic plant species, were offered to spotless starlings. Later we

95 recorded its presence and abundance in their nests. We predict that birds would select

96 preferentially unpigmented feathers and aromatic plants if antimicrobial protection of the

97 offspring were one of the functions of these nest materials.

98

99 Materials and Methods

100 Study species and area

101 The spotless starling is a medium-sized, hole-nesting that mostly breeds in

102 colonies (Cramp 1998). Green plants and feathers are commonly used as nest materials,

103 with plants and feathers being carried to the nest by males and females respectively (Polo

104 and Veiga 2006). Both materials are embedded in the nests, forming part of both their

105 structural and lining layers (Veiga and Polo 2016). Starlings in our population usually

5

106 commence to build their nests in March, laying eggs at mid-April. Since April 10th nest-

107 boxes were visited every three days until the first egg was laid.

108 The study was performed during the 2013 breeding season in a population located

109 in the old railway station of La Calahorra (37°18’ N, 3°11’ W) within the Hoya de Guadix,

110 a high-altitude plateau 1000 m a.s.l., with a semi-arid climate. Apart from a few sparse

111 almond trees (Prunus dulcis), vegetation in the area is typical of an agro-pastoral steppe,

112 with abandoned fields interspersed within barley (Hordeum vulgare) and lettuce (Lactuca

113 sativa) cultivated fields. There were 80 cork-made nest-boxes (internal height * width *

114 depth: 350 * 180 * 210 mm, bottom-to-hole height: 240 mm) available for spotless

115 starlings, attached to tree trunks or walls at 3-4 m above ground. Nest-boxes were often less

116 than 1 m apart from each other (as in natural holes).

117

118 Experimental procedures

119 Feathers and plants were offered ad libitum to starlings during nest building and laying

120 stages. Feathers were provided in 24 * 24 cm broad plastic meshes placed in the ground,

121 and distributed around the study area, so that all breeding pairs could have access to

122 feathers. Twenty meshes were located in adjacent pairs, containing 50 unpigmented and 50

123 pigmented feathers each. Feathers offered to starlings, of approximately 10 cm long, were

124 from domestic turkeys (Meleagris gallopavo) to distinguish them from chicken feathers

125 that were the most abundant in starling nests in our population. In addition, turkey feathers

126 were marked on the quill with a permanent marker. Meshes were checked daily and

127 feathers were replenished whenever there were 15 feathers or less in the mesh.

128 Plants were provided in plastic containers (height * width * length: 6 * 7 * 9.5 cm)

129 filled with insulating foam and water to maintain them fresh. In each container, four apical

6

130 plant fragments of approximately 10 cm length of four aromatic species (Marrubium

131 vulgare, Artemisia barrelieri, Lamium amplexicaule and Anacyclus clavatus) and one non-

132 aromatic plant (barley, Hordeum vulgare) were provided. Plants were marked in the stem

133 with seal ink. Containers were attached on top of 2/3 of nest-boxes and were replaced every

134 three days to provide fresh plants, when the number of fragments of each plant species

135 collected by starlings was counted. Starlings made use of plants irrespective of whether

136 those were above their own or above adjacent nest-boxes (authors’ personal observation)

137 and, if considering six consecutive containers, plant fragments of all offered species were

138 always available at the time of container replacement.

139 Starting one week before the expected laying date of the first egg (the onset of egg

140 laying of starling pairs in our population is quite synchronous), plants and feathers were

141 provided to starlings during three weeks. Nest-boxes were checked every three days, from

142 the day plants and feathers were offered until eggs were found in the nest. During each

143 visit, the number of feathers present in nests was counted, distinguishing between

144 pigmented and unpigmented, and between experimental and non-experimental feathers.

145 Plants present in nests were also weighted (± 0.1 g) and fragments of experimental plants

146 found in nests were counted. When experimental nest materials were offered for the first

147 time, none of the nests contained eggs. Some nests (N = 21) were detected with eggs three

148 days later, during the second visit, while for 27 nests, eggs were detected in subsequent

149 visits. Since nest material composition during the pre-laying stage was estimated during the

150 visit just before eggs were detected, only that of the latter group of nests could have been

151 influenced by the experimental ad libitum availability of materials. Thus, these two groups

152 of nests serve to test for the influence of the experiment on nest material composition

153 before egg laying.

7

154 In addition, as proxies of the selection made by starlings of feathers and plants, the

155 number of times that each experimental mesh containing either pigmented or unpigmented

156 feathers needed to be replenished, and the number of steams of each plant species that had

157 been collected from the containers, were used.

158 To estimate plant availability around nests, the percentage of a circular area of 20

159 m-radius around each nest-box that was covered by green plants was recorded (hereafter

160 percentage of vegetation coverage). These values were used for statistically controlling

161 plant availability when analyzing variation in mass of green plants found in starling nests.

162

163 Antimicrobial activity of green plants offered to starlings

164 The antimicrobial activity of the five green plants offered to spotless starlings was assessed

165 in the laboratory. First, plants were sterilized using a UV sterilizer chamber (Burdinola BV-

166 100). Second, 1 cm fragments of each species were placed in Brain Heart Agar (BHA,

167 Scharlau Chemie S.A., Barcelona) plates, inoculated with 100 µL of a 24 h culture of the

168 indicator bacteria strain (see below), and then incubated 24 h at 37 ºC. The antimicrobial

169 activity of each plant was revealed by the presence of growth-inhibition halos around the

170 plant fragment. Two kinds of halos were distinguished: a clear halo, where growth of the

171 indicator bacteria was completely inhibited, and a colored halo, presumably due to

172 pigments from plants that do not completely inhibit growth of the indicator bacteria.

173 Antimicrobial activity of each plant was summarized by an index resulting from the

174 addition of standardized values (i.e., mean = 0 and SD = 1) of three different variables

175 describing (i) halo transparency: 0 (no halo), 1 (not clear halo), 2 (clear halo); (ii) size of

176 halo: 0 (no halo), 1 (1-2 mm), 2 (3-4 mm), 3 (5-6 mm) and 4 (>6 mm), and (iii) number of

8

177 indicator bacteria to which the tested plant demonstrated antimicrobial activity (from 0 to

178 11).

179 Antimicrobial activity assays were performed against 11 bacterial typified strains

180 from the Spanish Type Culture Collection (CECT) and from our own laboratory collection:

181 Bacillus licheniformis D13, Enterococcus faecalis MRR10, Listeria inocua CECT340,

182 Listeria monocytogenes CECT4032, Staphylococcus aureus CECT240, Enterococcus

183 faecalis F-58, Enterococcus faecalis JH2, Enterococcus faecalis S47, Enterococcus

184 faecium 115, Lactobacillus paracasei 11-2 and Lactococcus lactis. However, it should be

185 noted that none of the tested plant species showed antimicrobial activity against the last six

186 indicator bacteria.

187

188 Sample size and statistical analyses

189 We used information on feathers and plants in 48 nests at the two different times of interest:

190 once eggs were found in the nest for the first time (hereafter laying stage), and in the

191 previous visit three days before (hereafter pre-laying stage). Total and experimental number

192 of pigmented and unpigmented feathers, as well as mass of plants found in starling nests,

193 were log10 transformed to approach Gaussian distributions. Thus, transformed variables

194 and/or the residuals of the statistical models did follow normal distributions.

195 Due to variability in laying dates, experimental feathers and plants were only

196 available for 27 out of the 48 nests sampled during the pre-laying stage, but for all of them

197 during the laying stage. Thus, to explore differences in number and color composition of

198 feathers in nests due to the presence of ad libitum experimental feathers, we performed two

199 different models. First, we explored the effects of availability of feathers (nests with vs

200 without experimental feathers available, fixed factor) and feather pigmentation on number

9

201 of feathers detected in nests during the pre-laying stage (dependent variable). We used a

202 Repeated Measures ANOVA (RM-ANOVA) with numbers of feathers of each color

203 detected in the same nest as within effects, and availability of experimental feathers as

204 between factor. Furthermore, to explore the effects of reproductive stage (i.e., pre-laying vs

205 laying) and feather composition on total number of feathers in starling nests, we only used

206 nests that had experimental feathers available both before and during egg-laying

207 reproductive stages. These effects were explored in a RM-ANOVA with two within factors,

208 feather color and reproductive stage. The effects of laying date on repeated measures (i.e.,

209 interactions) were estimated in separate models. In addition, to analyze feather color

210 preference by starlings, we used the number of times that meshes of different color were

211 replenished as dependent variable, and feather color as fixed factor in a GLM.

212 The effect of plant availability on the amount of green plants found in starling nests

213 was explored in GLM models with availability of experimental plants as the categorical

214 factor, and the percentage of vegetation coverage as a covariable. The effect of nest stage

215 on the amount of green plants in starling nests was explored using nests with available

216 experimental green material during the pre-laying and laying stages. For this, we used a

217 RM-ANOVA with nest stage as repeated measures variable. The effect of laying date on

218 the repeated measures variable (i.e., interactions) was estimated in a separate RM-ANOVA.

219 In addition, we further explored the preference of starlings for any of the experimental

220 plants offered in a Generalized Linear Model (GLZ) with a binomial distribution and logit-

221 link function while controlling for overdispersion. We used the presence/absence

222 information of experimental plants of each species in starling nests (during the pre-laying

223 and laying stages) as dependent variable, and plant species and nesting stage as categorical

224 independent factors. Plant preferences were also assessed by analyzing number of plants of

10

225 each species that were collected by starlings from the containers. We used the log10

226 number of steams collected by starlings as dependent variable, and species and container

227 identity as factors in a GLM.

228 FDR (false discovery rate) procedure was used to adjust P values for multiple

229 comparisons by p.adjust function of stats package in R 3.4.1 (http://www.r-project.org/).

230 All other statistical analyses were performed with Statistica 8.0 (Statsoft Inc. 2011). Values

231 reported are means ± 95% CI.

232

233 Results

234 Feather color preferences

235 During the pre-laying stage, unpigmented feathers were significantly more abundant in

236 starling nests than pigmented feathers (F1,46 = 6.73, P = 0.029). Moreover, neither number

237 of feathers (F1,46 = 0.41, P = 0.603), nor feather color composition (F1,46 = 0.04, P = 0.604;

238 Fig 1A) depended on availability of experimental feathers before egg laying started.

239 Nest stage (laying vs pre-laying) affected total number of feathers in nests. Feathers

240 were more abundant in starling nests at the time of laying (mean (95% CI) = 9.37 (6.27 –

241 12.47)) than before egg-laying began (mean (95% CI) = 4.89 (2.50 – 7.28)) (F1,26 = 21.84,

242 P < 0.001), with laying date explaining a non-significant proportion of variance (F1,25 =

243 0.57, P = 0.399). In addition, number of feathers in starling nests depended on feather color.

244 Unpigmented feathers (mean (95% CI) = 9.93 ± (6.69 – 13.16)) were significantly more

245 abundant than pigmented feathers (mean (95% CI) = 4.33 (1.19 – 7.18)) (F1,26 = 18.00, P <

246 0.001), and this difference tended to be more pronounced during the laying stage (F1,26 =

247 4.73, P = 0.071, Fig 1B). Laying date affected differences in feather color composition

248 since unpigmented feathers were relatively more abundant than pigmented feathers as the

11

249 season progressed (F1,25 = 4.47, P = 0.071), but did not affect the detected effects of the

250 interaction between feather pigmentation and reproductive stage (F1,25 = 0.12, P = 0.737).

251 Similar results were achieved when only experimental feathers found in nests were

252 considered. Unpigmented experimental feathers were significantly more abundant (mean

253 (95% CI) = 4.81 (2.94 – 6.69)) than experimental pigmented feathers (mean (95% CI) =

254 0.02 (-0.01 – 0.38)) (F1,25 = 48.12, P < 0.001). These differences were more pronounced

255 during the laying stage (Pre-laying: unpigmented feathers, mean (95% CI) = 1.37 (0.14 –

256 2.6); pigmented feathers, mean (95% CI) = 0.04 (-0.04 – 0.11); Laying: unpigmented

257 feathers, mean (95% CI) = 3.44 (2.12 – 4.77); pigmented feathers, mean (95% CI) = 0.15 (-

258 0.03 – 0.33)) (F1,25 = 17.99, P < 0.001).

259 Finally, meshes with unpigmented feathers needed to be replenished more

260 frequently than those with pigmented feathers (unpigmented feathers: range: 0-4, mean ±

261 SE = 1.6 ± 0.5; pigmented feathers: range: 0-1, mean ± SE = 0.2 ± 0.1; F1,18 = 7.35, P =

262 0.014; Fig 1C). Thus, independently of the used variable, all results invariably suggest that

263 starlings preferentially chose unpigmented feathers, mainly during the laying stage.

264

265 Green plant preferences

266 In the pre-laying stage, mass of plants in nests with and without plants offered did not differ

267 significantly (with experimental plants available: mean (95% CI) = 0.90 g (0.35 – 1.46);

268 without experimental plants available: mean (95% 5CI) = 0.44 g (0.15 – 1.74); F1,48 = 0.81,

269 P = 0.557). Moreover, nest stage did not affect mass of plants in starling nests (Pre-laying:

270 mean (%95CI) = 0.90 g (0.35 – 1.46); Laying: (95% CI) = 0.67 g (0.67 – 1.16)) (F1,26 =

271 0.32, P = 0.579), but laying date tended to explain a significant proportion of variance (F1,25

272 = 6.60, P = 0.051). Mass of plants found in nests during the laying stage increased as the

12

273 season progressed, while the opposite trend was detected for the pre-laying stage (Fig 2A).

274 Percentage of vegetation coverage around nests did not significantly affect mass of plants

275 found in nests (mean (95% CI) = 78.93% (71.73 – 86.13); F1,22 = 2.77, P = 0.220).

276 We analyzed the starling preferences for experimental plants during the two nest

277 stages separately. In the pre-laying stage, prevalence of different experimental plant species

2 278 in starling nests did not differ (χ 4 = 3.13, P = 0.579; Fig 2B). However, starlings showed

2 279 some preferences during the laying stage (χ 4 = 14.79, P = 0.030; Fig 2B). L. amplexicaule

280 and M. vulgare were the experimental plants most frequently found in the nests, while A.

281 barrielieri and H. vulgare appeared with the lowest prevalence (paired comparisons: L.

2 2 282 amplexicaule – H. vulgare: χ 2 = 6.00, P = 0.014; M. vulgare – H. vulgare: χ 2 = 4.35, P =

283 0.037; Fig 2B). Similarly, the number of aromatic plant stems collected by starlings from

284 the containers were larger than that of non-aromatic plants (F4,435 = 4.73, P = 0.001; Fig 2C;

285 post-hoc LSD: L. amplexicaule – H. vulgare: P = 0.0001; M. vulgare – H. vulgare: P =

286 0.0002; A. clavatus – H. vulgare: P = 0.004; A. barrelieri – H. vulgare: P = 0.006; Fig 2C).

287 Thus, it is during the laying stage when preference for aromatic plants comes out.

288 Finally, antimicrobial activity assays of green plants offered to starlings showed that

289 A. barrelieri was the plant with the highest antimicrobial activity, followed by A. clavatus,

290 and L. ampleuxicaule. The lowest antimicrobial activity was shown by M. vulgare and H.

291 vulgare (Table 1).

292

293 Discussion

294 The experimental results showed that starlings preferred unpigmented over pigmented

295 feathers and aromatic plants over non-aromatic plants for building their nests. Moreover,

296 these preferences were more clearly detected during the laying stage, when the hypothetical

13

297 protective function of these nest materials for developing offspring would be more

298 important.

299

300 Feather preferences

301 Feathers were more abundant in nests of starlings during the laying stage than during the

302 pre-laying stage. This is in accordance with the common yet often overlooked assumption

303 that nest building in birds does not end with the onset of laying, but continue during

304 posterior stages, likely to accommodate successive selection pressures (Hansell 2000).

305 Independently of the nesting stage, unpigmented feathers were more abundant than

306 pigmented feathers in starling nests. The main studied function of nest feathers concerns

307 their thermoregulatory properties (Møller 1984; Windsor et al. 2013), which for instance

308 would help to reduce clutch and brood cooling rates (Hilton et al. 2004). It is known that

309 pigmentation partially determines thermal insulation properties of structures in nature

310 (Hochscheid et al. 2002; Hetem et al. 2009; for a similar argument in avian eggs see Lahti

311 and Ardia 2016). However, previous studies dealing with insulation properties of feathers

312 in avian nests have not considered the potential effect of feather pigmentation (Hilton et al.

313 2004; Dawson et al. 2011; Windsor et al. 2013). This possibility is out of scope of the

314 present study. However, any hypothetical effect of feather pigmentation on nest

315 thermoregulation will mainly apply to open nests since external radiation reaching nest

316 contents inside holes is limited (Kilner 1999; Hunt et al. 2003; Avilés et al. 2008) and, thus,

317 would hardly apply to starling nests.

318 A second functional explanation for the preference of unpigmented feathers is

319 related to scenarios of sexual selection (Veiga and Polo 2005), with feathers in nests acting

320 as a courtship display affecting mate choice, and/or as a post-mating sexual signal eliciting

14

321 differential reproductive investment in mates (Sanz and García-Navas 2011; but see Veiga

322 and Polo (2011)). Spotless starling females carry feathers to the nest in response to green

323 plants carried by males (Polo and Veiga 2006). We also found that feathers were more

324 abundant during the laying stage than during the pre-laying stage. This would be in

325 accordance with a role for feathers as sexual signals used by females to elicit differential

326 male investment because this period is closer to the early nestling stage, when contribution

327 to parental care by males is much needed (Soler et al. 2008). Moreover, amount of feathers

328 in starling nests is related to female experience (Polo and Veiga 2006), and the

329 experimental addition of feathers resulted in reduced nestling mortality (Veiga and Polo

330 2011). Thus, it is likely that feathers induced differential paternal investment in

331 reproduction (Veiga and Polo 2011).

332 Even if feather color preference is driven by sexual selection, it may also function in

333 additional scenarios. In this sense, trying to figure out possible scenarios explaining the

334 detected positive effects of experimental feathers reducing nestling mortality, Veiga and

335 Polo (2011) discussed the role of feathers as a possible antiparasitic material that would

336 reduce nestling mortality. Thus, it is even possible that the reason underlying the preference

337 for unpigmented feathers is their higher antimicrobial properties that have been

338 demonstrated in laboratory (Peralta-Sánchez et al. 2014) and in the field in nests of barn

339 swallows (Peralta-Sánchez et al. 2010). An alternative explanation for color composition of

340 nest-lining feathers is that it could mirror the availability or detectability of feathers of

341 different color in the nest surroundings. However, this possibility would not explain feather

342 color composition of nests in our study because pigmented and unpigmented feathers were

343 available ad libitum and easily locatable.

15

344 Even knowing the starling preference for unpigmented feathers, and the ad libitum

345 availability of our experimental approach, very few nests (7 out of 48 nests) harbored only

346 unpigmented feathers, which suggest that starlings tried to incorporate feathers to the nest

347 following a certain combination of colors that they prefer. Interestingly, the detected

348 combination of feather colors changed, with a relative increase of unpigmented feathers as

349 the season progressed. Previous studies found that bacterial colonies from pigmented

350 feathers also have antibacterial activity (Peralta-Sánchez et al. 2014) and that the

351 abundance of pigmented feathers has a positive effect on phenotypic quality of spotless

352 starling nestlings (Ruiz-Castellano et al. unpublished). Moreover, antimicrobial activity of

353 bacterial colonies isolated from unpigmented feathers in nests of barn swallows was higher

354 in nests where experimental pigmented feathers were added (Peralta-Sánchez et al. 2014).

355 Thus, it is possible that certain combination of pigmented and unpigmented feathers in

356 nests of birds maximizes feather-derived antimicrobial activity. Moreover, environmental

357 conditions at the end of the breeding season favor bacterial growth in avian nests (Soler et

358 al. 2015; Møller et al. 2015). Therefore, it is possible that starlings tried to compensate such

359 increase in probability of bacterial infection by carrying to the nest feathers with higher

360 antimicrobial activity as the season progressed (Peralta-Sánchez et al. 2014). Further

361 research on antimicrobial properties of bacteria isolated from unpigmented and pigmented

362 feathers in nests of birds with different combinations of feather color is necessary to

363 explore this possibility.

364

365 Green plant preferences

366 Starlings also showed a preference for specific green plants to build their nests. Availability

367 of experimental plants did not affect plant abundance in their nests, but starlings that breed

16

368 later used more plants than early breeders did. This positive association between laying date

369 and amount of green plants in the nest has also been found for European starlings (Clark

370 and Mason 1985; Gwinner and Berger 2005; Dubiec et al. 2013). It could be argued that

371 this is just the outcome of a higher accumulation of plants in nests that commenced egg

372 laying later. However, this explanation is unlikely for several reasons. First, because it is

373 possible that starlings, as some other bird species do, remove part of the old dried plants

374 after some days in the nest (Petit et al. 2002), and second, because for a given starling nest,

375 the amount of plants found during the laying stage was smaller than during the pre-laying

376 stage. In addition, it is known that female starlings remove from the nest the green plants

377 carried by males (Veiga and Polo 2012), which reflect male quality (Veiga et al. 2006).

378 This female behavior has been explained as a mean to difficult the assessment of male and

379 nest quality by neighboring rival females (Veiga and Polo 2012), which usually prospect

380 other nests to gather public information (Parejo et al. 2008). Removing green plants would

381 therefore prevent nest usurpation and reduce nest attractiveness for conspecific brood

382 parasites in European and spotless starlings (Sandell and Diemer 1999; Veiga and Polo

383 2012). Thus, it is possible that the relatively smaller amount of plants detected in starling

384 nests during the laying stage was the consequence of this female behavior. Another

385 explanation could be that males do not incorporate green material to the nest during the

386 laying stage, as has been claimed for European starlings (Gwinner 1997), and for some

387 other populations of spotless starlings (Veiga et al. 2006). However, this explanation is

388 unlikely since spotless starling males in our population continue carrying green material to

389 the nests during the laying and incubation stages (Ruiz-Castellano et al. 2016).

390 In agreement with a possible antimicrobial function of plants, we also found that

391 starlings did not show a marked preference for any of the offered plant species to be carried

17

392 to their nests during the pre-laying stage, but during the egg-laying, males preferentially

393 selected aromatic plants. It is possible that, before egg laying males attract females to their

394 nests by carrying fresh green material with less emphasis on antimicrobial and antiparasitic

395 properties of plants (Hansell 2000). However, once reproduction has started, selecting

396 aromatic plants would have the additional advantage of protecting the nests against

397 parasites (Clark and Mason 1985) and microorganisms (Mennerat et al. 2009a). We know

398 that European starling males discriminate plant volatiles (Clark and Mason 1997) and that

399 this ability is maximized during the courtship and nest building stage (Clark and Smeraski

400 1990), when they select plant materials based on volatile compounds. Thus, it is possible

401 that spotless starling males also select plants with antimicrobial properties relying on their

402 volatile compounds. Most of the aromatic plants that we provided to starlings have strong

403 antimicrobial activity (see Table 1). The most preferred plant species appeared to be L.

404 amplexicaule, and not A. barrelieri that showed the highest antimicrobial activity. We

405 estimated antimicrobial activity of these plants by testing inhibitory potential of growth of

406 11 bacterial strains that include possible avian pathogens. Thus, although antimicrobial

407 activity against different bacterial strains usually covaries (Table 1; see also Al-Bakri and

408 Afifi 2007; Khalil et al. 2009), we cannot rule out the possibility that the most preferred

409 plants were active against some particular bacterial pathogens more abundant in the study

410 area. Additional analyses, ideally with bacteria isolated from nests, would be needed to

411 determine antimicrobial activity of nest plants against bacteria present in starling nests.

412 Independently of the reasons determining preferences for specific aromatic plants, the

413 detected higher amount of green plants during the pre-laying stage, as well as the

414 preferences for aromatic plants during the laying stage, suggest a primary role of this

18

415 material in sexual signaling and nest protection against pathogens, which may be more

416 prominent during the egg laying stage (see Rubalcaba et al. 2016).

417

418 Conclusion

419 Irrespective of differential functionality of the diverse nest materials, and of the possible

420 optimization of nest material composition, we have shown that starlings make a selection of

421 nest building materials in a possible adaptive way. We have shown this preference

422 regarding two common nest materials profusely employed by many bird species from

423 different orders and in different habitats to build their nests, i.e., green plants and feathers

424 (Hansell 2000; Dubiec et al. 2013). Furthermore, these two materials play pivotal roles in

425 the study of nest building behaviors, and may aid a better comprehension of self-medication

426 tool use by (Hansell and Ruxton 2008; Healy et al. 2008; De Roode et al. 2013).

427 Thus, our study help to better understand the adaptive functionality of nest building

428 processes and their involved behaviors.

429

430 Acknowledges

431 This work was supported by Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación, European funds

432 (FEDER) (CGL2010-19233-C03-01, CGL2010-19233-C03-03, CGL2013-48193-C3-1-P,

433 CGL2013- 48193-C3-3-P). MRR received a postdoc from the program “JAE-Doc” and

434 CRC had a predoctoral grant from the Spanish Government. GT was supported by Ramón y

435 Cajal program (Spain).

436

437 Data Accessibility

19

438 Analyses reported in this article can be reproduced using the data provided by Ruiz-

439 Castellano C, Tomás G, Ruiz-Rodríguez M, and Soler JJ. (2017) Data from: Nest material

440 preferences by spotless starlings. Behavioral Ecology. In Dryad Digital Repository,

441 http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.q6ng9

20

442 Reference List

443

444 Al-Bakri AG, Afifi FU. 2007. Evaluation of antimicrobial activity of selected plant extracts by

445 rapid XTT colorimetry and bacterial enumeration. J Microbiol Meth. 68:19-25

446 Avilés JM, Parejo D, Pérez-Contreras T, Navarro C, Soler JJ. 2010. Do spotless starlings place

447 feathers at their nests by ultraviolet color? Naturwissenschaften. 97:181-186

448 Avilés JM, Pérez-Contreras T, Navarro C, Soler JJ. 2008. Dark nests and conspicuousness in color

449 patterns of nestlings of altricial birds. Am Nat. 171:327-338

450 Bailey IE, Morgan KV, Oschadleus HD, DeRuiter SL, Meddle SL, Healy SD. 2016. Nest-building

451 males trade off material collection costs with territory value. Emu. 116:1-8

452 Brouwer L, Komdeur J. 2004. Green nesting material has a function in mate attraction in the

453 European starling. Anim Behav. 67:539-548

454 Clark L, Mason JR. 1985. Use of nest material as insecticidal and anti-pathogenic agents by the

455 European Starling. Oecologia. 67:169-176

456 Clark L, Mason JR. 1987. Olfactory discrimination of plant volatiles by the European starling.

457 Anim Behav. 35:227-235

458 Clark L, Smeraski CA. 1990. Seasonal shifts in odor acuity by starlings. J Exp Zool. 255:22-29

459 Cramp S. 1998. The complete birds of the Western Palearctic. Oxford, Oxford University Press

460 Dawson RD, O'Brien EL, Mlynowski TJ. 2011. The price of insulation: costs and benefits of feather

461 delivery to nests for male tree swallows Tachycineta bicolor. J Avian Biol. 42:93-102

462 De Roode JC, Lefèvre T, Hunter MD. 2013. Self-medication in animals. Science. 340:150-151

21

463 Dubiec A, Gózdz I, Mazgajski TD. 2013. Green plant material in avian nests. Avian Biol Res.

464 6:133-146

465 Fauth PT, Krementz DG, Hines JE. 1991. Ectoparasitism and the role of green nesting material in

466 the European starling. Oecologia. 88:22-29

467 García-López de Hierro L, Moleón M, Ryan PG. 2013. Is carrying feathers a sexually selected trait

468 in house sparrows? Ethology. 119:199-211

469 Goldstein G, Flory KR, Browne BA, Majid S, Ichida JM, Burtt Jr EH. 2004. Bacterial degradation

470 of black and white feathers. Auk. 121:656-659

471 Gunderson AR, Frame AM, Swaddle JP, Forsyth MH. 2008. Resistance of melanized feathers to

472 bacterial degradation: Is it really so black and white? J Avian Biol. 39:539-545

473 Gwinner H. 1997. The function of green plants in nests of European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris).

474 Behaviour. 134:337-351

475 Gwinner H, Berger S. 2005. European starlings: Nestling condition, parasites and green nest

476 material during the breeding season. J Ornithol. 146:365-371

477 Gwinner H, Berger S. 2008. Starling males select green nest material by olfaction using experience-

478 independent and experience-dependent cues. Anim Behav. 75:971-976

479 Gwinner H, Oltrogge M, Trost L, Nienaber U. 2000. Green plants in starling nests: Effects on

480 nestlings. Anim Behav. 59:301-309

481 Hansell M, Ruxton GD. 2008. Setting tool use within the context of construction behaviour.

482 Trends Ecol Evol. 23:73-78

22

483 Hansell MH. 2000. Bird nests and construction behaviour. Cambridge, Cambridge University

484 Press

485 Healy S, Walsh P, Hansell M. 2008. Nest building by birds. Curr Biol. 18:R271-R273

486 Hetem RS, de Witt BA, Fick LG, Fuller A, Kerley GIH, Meyer LCR, Mitchell D, Maloney SK.

487 2009. Body temperature, thermoregulatory behaviour and pelt characteristics of three

488 colour morphs of springbok (Antidorcas marsupialis). Comp Biochem Phys A. 152:379-

489 388

490 Hilton GM, Hansell MH, Ruxton GD, Reid JM, Monaghan P. 2004. Using artificial nests to test

491 importance of nesting material and nest shelter for incubation energetics. Auk. 121:777-

492 787

493 Hochscheid S, Grémillet D, Wanless S, Du Plessis MA. 2002. Black and white under the South

494 African sun: Are juvenile Cape gannets heat stressed? J Therm Biol. 27:325-332

495 Hunt S, Kilner RM, Langmore NE, Bennett ATD. 2003. Conspicuous, ultraviolet-rich mouth

496 colours in begging chicks. Proc R Soc Lon B Biol Sci. 270:S25-S28

497 Khalil A, Dababneh BF, Al-Gabbiesh AH. 2009. Antimicrobial activity against pathogenic

498 microorganisms by extracts from herbal Jordanian plants. J Food Agric Environ. 7:103-

499 106

500 Kilner RM. 1999. Family conflicts and the evolution of nestling mouth colour. Behaviour. 136:779-

501 804

502 Lafuma L, Lambrechts MM, Raymond M. 2001. Aromatic plants in bird nests as a protection

503 against blood-sucking flying insects? Behav Process. 56:113-120

23

504 Lahti DC, Ardia DR. 2016. Shedding light on bird egg color: Pigment as parasol and the dark car

505 effect. Am Nat. 187:547-563

506 Lombardo MP, Bosman RM, Faro CA, Houtteman SG, Kluisza TS. 1995. Effect of feathers as nest

507 insulation on incubation behavior and reproductive performance of tree swallows

508 (Tachycineta bicolor). Auk. 112:973-981

509 Mainwaring MC, Hartley IR. 2013. The energetic costs of nest building in birds. Avian Biol Res.

510 6:12-17

511 Mainwaring MC, Wolfenden A, Read JE, Robson JMA, Tomlinson CJ, Hartley IR. 2016.

512 Feathering the nest: The effects of feather supplementation to Blue Tit nests. Avian Biol

513 Res. 9:89-95

514 Mainwaring MC, Hartley IR, Lambrechts MM, Deeming DC. 2014. The design and function of

515 birds' nests. Ecol Evol. 4:3909-3928

516 Mennerat A, Mirleau P, Blondel J, Perret P, Lambrechts MM, Heeb P. 2009a. Aromatic plants in

517 nests of the blue tit Cyanistes caeruleus protect chicks from bacteria. Oecologia. 161:849-

518 855

519 Mennerat A, Perret P, Bourgault P, Blondel J, Gimenez O, Thomas DW, Heeb P, Lambrechts MM.

520 2009b. Aromatic plants in nests of blue tits: positive effects on nestlings. Anim Behav.

521 77:569-574

522 Mennerat A, Perret P, Lambrechts MM. 2009c. Local individual preferences for nest materials in a

523 passerine bird. PLoS ONE. 4:e5104

524 Møller AP, Soler JJ, Nielsen JT, Galván I. 2015. Pathogenic bacteria and timing of laying. Ecol

525 Evol. 5:1676-1685

24

526 Møller AP. 1984. On the use of feathers in birds' nests: predictions and tests. Ornis Scand. 15:38-42

527 Moreno J. 2012. Avian nests and nest-building as signals. Avian Biol Res. 5:238-251

528 Parejo D, Pérez-Contreras T, Navarro C, Soler JJ, Avilés JM. 2008. Spotless starlings rely on public

529 information while visiting conspecific nests: an experiment. Anim Behav. 75:483-488

530 Peralta-Sánchez JM, Møller AP, Martín-Platero AM, Soler JJ. 2010. Number and colour

531 composition of nest lining feathers predict eggshell bacterial community in barn swallow

532 nests: An experimental study. Funct Ecol. 24:426-433

533 Peralta-Sánchez JM, Møller AP, Soler JJ. 2011. Colour composition of nest lining feathers affects

534 hatching success of barn swallows, Hirundo rustica (Passeriformes: Hirundinidae). Biol J

535 Linn Soc. 102:67-74

536 Peralta-Sánchez JM, Soler JJ, Martín-Platero AM, Knight R, Martínez-Bueno M, Møller AP. 2014.

537 Eggshell bacterial load is related to antimicrobial properties of feathers lining barn

538 swallow nests. Microb Ecol. 67:480-487

539 Petit C, Hossaert-McKey M, Perret P, Blondel J, Lambrechts MM. 2002. Blue tits use selected

540 plants and olfaction to maintain an aromatic environment for nestlings. Ecol Lett. 5:585-

541 589

542 Pinowski J, Haman A, Jerzak L, Pinowska B, Barkowska M, Grodzki A, Haman K. 2006. The

543 thermal properties of some nests of the Eurasian Tree Sparrow Passer montanus. J Therm

544 Biol. 31:573-581

545 Pires BA, Belo AF, Rabaça JE. 2012. Aromatic plants in Eurasian blue tit nests: The 'nest

546 protection hypothesis' revisited. Wilson J Ornithol. 124:162-165

25

547 Polo V, Veiga JP. 2006. Nest ornamentation by female spotless starlings in response to a male

548 display: An experimental study. J Anim Ecol. 75:942-947

549 Polo V, Veiga JP, Cordero PJ, Viñuela J, Monaghan P. 2004. Female starlings adjust primary sex

550 ratio in response to aromatic plants in the nest. PRoc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 271:1929-

551 1933

552 Rubalcaba JG, Polo V, Maia R, Rubenstein DR, Veiga JP. 2016. Sexual and natural selection in the

553 evolution of extended phenotypes: the use of green nesting material in starlings. J Evol

554 Biol. 29:1585-1592

555 Ruiz-Castellano C, Tomás G, Ruiz-Rodríguez M, Martín-Gálvez D, Soler JJ. 2016. Nest material

556 shapes eggs bacterial environment. PLoS ONE. 11:e0148894

557 Sandell MI, Diemer M. 1999. Intraspecific brood parasitism: a strategy for floating females in the

558 European starling. Anim Behav. 57:197-202

559 Sanz JJ, García-Navas V. 2011. Nest ornamentation in blue tits: is feather carrying ability a male

560 status signal? Behav Ecol. 22:240-247

561 Scott-Baumann JF, Morgan ER. 2015. A review of the nest protection hypothesis: does inclusion of

562 fresh green plant material in birds' nests reduce parasite infestation? Parasitology.

563 142:1016-1023

564 Shutler D, Campbell AA. 2007. Experimental addition of greenery reduces flea loads in nests of a

565 non-greenery using species, the tree swallow Tachycineta bicolor. J Avian Biol. 38:7-12

566 Soler JJ, Martín-Vivaldi M, Peralta-Sánchez JM, Ruiz-Rodríguez M. 2010. Antibiotic-producing

567 bacteria as a possible defence of birds against pathogenic microorganisms. Open Ornithol

568 J. 3:93-100

26

569 Soler JJ, Møller AP, Soler M. 1998. Nest building, sexual selection and parental investment. Evol

570 Ecol. 12:427-441

571 Soler JJ, Navarro C, Pérez-Contreras T, Avilés T, Cuervo JJ. 2008. Sexually selected coloration in

572 spotless starlings. Am Nat. 171:183-194

573 Soler JJ, Ruiz-Rodríguez M, Martín-Vivaldi M, Peralta-Sánchez JM, Ruiz-Castellano C, Tomás G.

574 2015. Laying date, incubation and egg breakage as determinants of bacterial load on bird

575 eggshells: experimental evidence. Oecologia. 179:63-74

576 Tomás G, Merino S, Martínez-de La Puente J, Moreno J, Morales J, Rivero-de Aguilar J. 2013.

577 Nest size and aromatic plants in the nest as sexually selected female traits in blue tits.

578 Behav Ecol. 24:926-934

579 Veiga JP, Polo V. 2005. Feathers at nests are potential female signals in the spotless starling. Biol

580 Lett. 1:334-337

581 Veiga JP, Polo V. 2011. Feathers in the spotless starling nests: a sexually selected trait? Behaviour.

582 148:1359-1375

583 Veiga JP, Polo V. 2012. Female Spotless Starlings (Sturnus unicolor) remove green plants from

584 their nests. J Ornithol. 153:291-296

585 Veiga JP, Polo V. 2016. Estornino Negro - Sturnus unicolor. In: Salvador A, Morales MB editors.

586 Enciclopedia Virtual de los Vertebrados Españoles. Madrid: Museo de Ciencias Naturales

587 Veiga JP, Polo V, Viñuela J. 2006. Nest green plants as a male status signal and courtship display in

588 the spotless starling. Ethology. 112:196-204

27

589 Windsor RL, Fegely JL, Ardia DR. 2013. The effects of nest size and insulation on thermal

590 properties of tree swallow nests. J Avian Biol. 44:305-310

591

28

592 Table 1: Antimicrobial activity of the different green plants offered to spotless starlings for

593 nest building. We show the scores for halo transparency, halo size, number of bacteria for

594 which tested plants demonstrated antimicrobial activity, and the index scores for each

595 tested plant.

Number Halo Halo of Index Plant Bacteria transparency size inhibited score bacteria A. barrelieri B. licheniformis 2 4 5 14.990 S. aureus 2 4 L. inocua 2 4 L. monocytogenes 2 1 E. faecalis 2 1 A. clavatus B. licheniformis 1 4 2 -0.054 S. aureus 1 4 L. inocua 0 0 L. monocytogenes 0 0 E. faecalis 0 0 L. ampleuxicaule B. licheniformis 1 4 1 -4.198 S. aureus 0 0 L. inocua 0 0 L. monocytogenes 0 0 E. faecalis 0 0 M. vulgare B. licheniformis 1 2 1 -5.369 S. aureus 0 0 L. inocua 0 0 L. monocytogenes 0 0 E. faecalis 0 0 H. vulgare B. licheniformis 1 2 1 -5.369 S. aureus 0 0 L. inocua 0 0 L. monocytogenes 0 0 E. faecalis 0 0 596

29

597 Fig 1: (A) Feather color composition (± CI 95%) in spotless starling nests during the pre-

598 laying stage, with or without experimental feathers available. (B) Number of pigmented and

599 unpigmented feathers (± CI 95%) in spotless starling nests during the pre-laying and the

600 egg laying stages. (C) Mean number of times (± CI 95%) that meshes containing the

601 experimental pigmented and unpigmented feathers offered to spotless starlings needed to be

602 replenished.

603

604 Fig 2: (A) Relationships between laying date and log10 of plant mass (g) of spotless

605 starling nests recorded both at the pre-laying and at the egg laying stages. Regression lines

606 are shown. (B) Prevalence of different plant species (aromatic: L. amplexicaule, M.

607 vulgare, A. clavatus, A. barrelieri; non-aromatic: H. vulgare) in spotless starling nests at

608 the pre-laying and egg laying stages. (C) Number of experimental plant fragments (± CI

609 95%) of the different species collected from containers by spotless starlings during the

610 three weeks that plants were available.

611

612

30

614 Fig 1:

615

616

617

31

618 Fig. 2

619

620

621

622

32