Emotion Lingers in a Face Intended to Display Neutrality Daniel

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Emotion Lingers in a Face Intended to Display Neutrality Daniel Running head: EMOTION RESIDUE 1 Emotion Residue: Emotion Lingers in a Face Intended to Display Neutrality Daniel N. Albohn & Reginald B. Adams, Jr. The Pennsylvania State University Author Note Daniel N. Albohn, 463 Moore building, University Park, PA 16802, E-Mail [email protected] Reginald B. Adams, Jr., 464 Moore building, University Park, PA 16802, E-Mail [email protected] Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Daniel Albohn, 463 Moore building, University Park, PA 16802 E-mail [email protected], or Reginald Adams, Jr., 464 Moore building, University Park, PA 16802, E-Mail [email protected]. EMOTION RESIDUE 2 Abstract Despite the prevalent use of neutral faces in expression research, the term neutral still remains ill-defined. A general assumption is that one’s overt attempt to pose a nonexpressive face results in a neutral display, one devoid of any expressive information. Ample research has demonstrated that nonexpressive faces do convey meaning through emotion-resembling appearance. Here, we examined whether actual prior expressive information lingers on a face, in the form of emotion residue, despite overt attempts to display a neutral face, and whether these subtle emotion cues influence trait impressions. Across three studies we found that explicit attempts at posing neutral displays retained emotion residue from a prior expression. This residue in turn significantly impacted impressions formed based on these otherwise “neutral” displays. We discuss implications of this work for better understanding how accurate impressions are derived from so- called “neutral” faces and underscore theoretical and methodological considerations for future research. Keywords: Neutral face, Neutral, Facial expressions, Face perception, Impression formation EMOTION RESIDUE 3 Emotion Residue: Emotion Lingers in a Face Intended to Display Neutrality It is difficult to think of a social exchange in which the face does not play an important role. With over 40 muscles that can be arranged into more than 7,000 unique configurations, it is a constantly changing canvas that portrays a wide range of social information such as peoples’ emotions, gender, age, and social interest (Hess, Adams, Grammer, & Kleck, 2009; Kalick, Zebrowitz, Langlois, & Johnson, 1998; Rule & Ambady, 2008; Zebrowitz, 2017). Despite its relatively confined space it is the principal vehicle of human expressive exchange. Even those who are face blind (i.e., prosopagnosic) are able to identify emotion cues in a face (Tranel, Damasio, & Damasio, 1988) and appear to use them to make trait impressions (Todorov & Duchaine, 2008). Likewise, neurotypical perceivers tend to use overt expression and even emotion-resembling facial features in otherwise neutral faces to form stable trait impressions of others (Knutson, 1996; Zebrowitz, 2017). Together this work points to emotion cues, even in a neutral display, as a primary source of the impressions we form of others. Expressions can convey basic social motives such as dominance and affiliation, and more complex information such as intelligence and trustworthiness (Adams, Nelson, Soto, Hess, & Kleck, 2012; Knutson, 1996). Social judgments derived from images of the same individual displaying different expressions at two different times varied widely, sometimes even more so than judgments of two different individuals (Todorov & Porter, 2014). This work highlights the primacy of expressions in impression formation even above and beyond appearance cues, and thus cautions against static photos conveying much accurate information. Yet, people purposefully pose for pictures, and in these cases emotional expression may be revealing of one’s actual character. For example, individuals tend to associate anger more so with straight EMOTION RESIDUE 4 males and conservatives, and happiness with gay males and liberals and can use these associations to identify, at above chance levels, the social group memberships others belong to (Tskhay & Rule, 2015). Further, some degree of expressivity, however subtle, is arguably an expected part of an individual’s facial baseline in typical day-to-day interactions. The lack of any expressivity can even be disturbing. For instance in the classic “still-face paradigm” babies become upset when a parent’s expressive face abruptly becomes “blank” (Tronick, Als, Adamson, Wise, & Brazelton, 1978). A lack of expression on inanimate human-like objects (e.g., mannequins, robots, dolls) is often viewed as unsettling despite approaching a life-like human status, the so-called “Uncanny Valley” effect (Mori, 1970). Taken together, it stands to reason that we do not expect the faces we encounter in our social worlds to be completely devoid of emotionality. We are so attuned to expressive cues that when forming impressions of others based on their neutral facial displays, we tend to focus on expression-resembling appearance features (e.g., large, wide eyes, high eyebrows versus small, beady eyes, low hooded eyebrows), an effect referred to as emotion overgeneralization (Zebrowitz, 2017). Thus, emotion-resembling cues can powerfully bias impressions, even when the observer knows the face they are viewing is emotionally nonexpressive. Here we go further to suggest that actual emotion information may be present in these so-called nonexpressive displays, from which we can derive accurate impressions as well. In a typical face, facial muscles are never fully relaxed, and when they are, this can disrupt face processing. Consider, for example, the characteristic drooping due to facial paralysis. Faces with paralysis preferentially bias attention to specific parts of the face and disrupts normal face processing patterns (Dey, Ishii, Byrne, Boahene, & Ishii, 2014). Thus, a EMOTION RESIDUE 5 typical face, even when posed to be emotionally nonexpressive (and even when subsequently rated by others as being “neutral”), may contain in those contracted muscles some subtle, untended emotional tone. If so, this could help explain a growing body of research suggesting that individuals can consistently and accurately derive certain personality traits and social category judgments even from “neutral” faces (see, Rule & Sutherland, 2017; Zebrowitz, 2017). Residual emotional tone in the neutral display could drive many of these judgments. Overview In the present research, we examine the influence of actual, residual emotional tone on impression formation in otherwise neutral displays. We refer to this as emotion residue, which we define as any observable transient emotional tone remaining on a face once an actor has finished making an expression and has intentionally returned to a neutral baseline. We hypothesized that residual cues remaining after an actor makes an expression and then is explicitly instructed to return to neutral would: 1) be detectable on post-expression neutral facial images, and 2) alter impressions of these faces similar to what has been previously found for overt expressive and expressive-resembling cues in faces. We expect that neutral displays made after a negative expression will be associated with more negative trait impressions, while those made after a positive expression will be evaluated more positively. Study 1: Demonstrating That Emotion Residue is Present After Making an Expression Before testing whether individuals are able to detect emotion residue, and if it influences impressions, it is important to examine what residual information, if any, remains on a post- expressive neutral face in the first place. Study 1 examines this by isolating on a pixel-by-pixel level the differences between the pre- and post-expressive images. Study 1 also tests whether EMOTION RESIDUE 6 participants are able to use this information to make meaningful trait impression judgements related to the prior expression. Method Each face used as stimuli in our studies included both a pre- and post-expressive neutral display. Because we had two images per actor, we can extract from each actors’ post-expressive neutral face what is unique to that image by subtracting from it that actors’ pre-expressive neutral face. Since each image is greyscaled, what remains after subtraction is a matrix of values between 0-1 representing the intensity of the pixel. This matrix of pixel intensities is what is unique to the post-expressive neutral, accounting for the pre-expressive neutral (Figure 1). Supplemental Materials 1 reports detailed procedures for the subtraction method, as well as observational results on areas of significant difference between the two images utilizing pixel- wise tests. Power Analysis We analyzed our data in this study using a linear mixed model. Our fixed effect of interest is the interaction between prior expression (anger vs. happy) and impression valence (positive vs. negative) on scores. Therefore, we obtained an effect size from a prior face rating study that utilized linear mixed effects modeling and had two categorical fixed effects variables (Ebner, 2010). Next, we conducted power estimates based on simulations of our model’s critical two-way interaction term by substituting in our model the observed fixed effect value for one that corresponds roughly to that observed by averaging the absolute value of several of the reported fixed effects coefficients in Ebner (2010). Based on these simulated calculations, we estimated that a minimum of 30 participants would be necessary to detect an effect size of 0.31 at 80% power. We estimated our power (based on our sample size) to be 88.90%, 95 % CIs [86.79,
Recommended publications
  • Very First Impressions
    Emotion Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 6, No. 2, 269–278 1528-3542/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.269 Very First Impressions Moshe Bar, Maital Neta, and Heather Linz Martinos Center at Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School First impressions of people’s personalities are often formed by using the visual appearance of their faces. Defining how quickly these impressions can be formed has critical implications for understanding social interactions and for determining the visual properties used to shape them. To study impression formation independent of emotional cues, threat judgments were made on faces with a neutral expression. Consequently, participants’ judgments pertained to the personality rather than to a certain temporary emotional state (e.g., anger). The results demonstrate that consistent first impressions can be formed very quickly, based on whatever information is available within the first 39 ms. First impressions were less consistent under these conditions when the judgments were about intelligence, suggesting that survival- related traits are judged more quickly. The authors propose that low spatial frequencies mediate this swift formation of threat judgments and provide evidence that supports this hypothesis. Keywords: first impressions, threat, face perception, rapid personality judgment, intelligence Our first impressions of others can be truthful. For example, Until now there has not been a clear definition of such temporal humans are excellent in judging personality traits and complex lower limit on impression formation, but relevant studies have social characteristics such as dominance, hierarchy, warmth, and been conducted with relation to the detection of facial valence.
    [Show full text]
  • Impression Formation and Durability in Mediated Communication
    ournal of the J ssociation for nformation ystems A I S Research Paper ISSN: 1536-9323 Impression Formation and Durability in Mediated Communication Susan A. Brown Management Information Systems, University of Arizona [email protected] Robert Fuller Sherry M. B. Thatcher Accounting and Information Management, Management Department, University of South Carolina University of Tennessee [email protected] [email protected] Abstract: Using literature from impression formation and social information processing theory, we examine the impact of communication style on impression formation and durability in a mediated environment. We leverage common writing styles found in workplace emails—emoticons, uppercase, lowercase, typographical errors—to examine how message receivers evaluate senders using these styles. Via a lab experiment with 748 subjects, including undergraduate students, graduate students, and working professionals, we found that impressions were associated with writing style beyond the email content. Receivers perceived senders of emails containing emoticons, errors, or written entirely in uppercase or lowercase as less functionally competent. They also perceived senders as less methodologically competent when emails used emoticons and less politically competent when emails were all lowercase or contained errors. They perceived senders using a neutral writing style as less sociable than senders using emoticons. In contrast to impression durability in face-to-face environments, receivers positively revised impressions when senders changed their style to neutral from any of the non-neutral styles. We attribute this difference to two characteristics of the IT artifact: symbol variety and reprocessability. Keywords: Computer-mediated Communication, Social Information Processing Theory, Impression Formation, Email Styles. Fred Niederman was the accepting senior editor.
    [Show full text]
  • Jeffrey O. Nuckols, the IMPACT of COGNITIVE COMPLEXITY on IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT (Under the Direction of Dr
    Jeffrey O. Nuckols, THE IMPACT OF COGNITIVE COMPLEXITY ON IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT (Under the direction of Dr. Jennifer Bowler) Department of Psychology, July 2014 This study seeks to add to the knowledge of cognitive complexity by examining its relationship with impression management and social desirability. In light of past studies a positive relationship between cognitive complexity and impression management was expected. This predicted relationship was found to exist, thereby increasing knowledge of the construct of cognitive complexity. Furthermore, relationships between cognitive complexity, social desirability, and impression management were expected, with social desirability moderating the relationship between cognitive complexity and social desirability. The results of this study did not support the hypothesized relationships involving social desirability; in fact, the results ran counter to those predicted. However, these findings raise interesting questions for future research. Both the expected findings and those which were unexpected add to the body of knowledge about cognitive complexity and point to the need for continued research on this topic. The Impact of Cognitive Complexity on Impression Management A Thesis Presented To The Faculty of the Department of Psychology East Carolina University In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in Psychology with a Concentration in Industrial and Organizational Psychology by Jeffrey O. Nuckols July, 2014 ©Copyright 2014 Jeffrey O. Nuckols The Impact of Cognitive Complexity on Impression Management by Jeffrey O. Nuckols APPROVED BY: DIRECTOR OF DISSERTATION/THESIS: _______________________________________________________ Jennifer L. Bowler, PhD COMMITTEE MEMBER: _______________________________________________________ Karl L. Wuensch, PhD COMMITTEE MEMBER: _______________________________________________________ Shahnaz Aziz, PhD CHAIR OF THE DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY: ____________________________________________________________ Susan L.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Perception and Attribution 3 Brian Parkinson
    9781405124003_4_003.qxd 10/31/07 2:55 PM Page 42 Social Perception and Attribution 3 Brian Parkinson KEY CONCEPTS actor–observer difference analysis of non-common effects attributional bias augmenting principle causal power causal schema central trait cognitive algebra configural model consensus information consistency information correspondence bias correspondent inference theory covariation theory depressive realism discounting principle distinctiveness information false consensus bias implicit personality theory learned helplessness theory naïve scientist model peripheral trait primacy effect salience self-fulfilling prophecy self-serving biases 9781405124003_4_003.qxd 10/31/07 2:55 PM Page 43 CHAPTER OUTLINE How can we tell what other people are like? How do we explain their actions and experiences (and our own)? This chapter introduces research intended to answer these questions. Studies of social perception show that impressions of others depend on what information is presented, how it is pre- sented, and on prior assumptions about how it fits together. Research into attribution demonstrates that perceivers consistently favour certain kinds of explanation over others. Our impressions and ex- planations are also shaped by our specific reasons for constructing them. In particular, we present social events in different ways to different people under different circumstances. Both social percep- tion and attribution therefore involve communication in combination with private interpretation. Introduction Can you remember when you first met your closest friend? How quickly did you get a sense of what he or she was like, and of how well you would get on together? Did your impression turn out to be correct, and if not, where and why did you go wrong? Now imagine that instead of meeting another person face to face, you are told about them by someone else.
    [Show full text]
  • Clothing and People-A Social Signal Processing Perspective
    Clothing and People - A Social Signal Processing Perspective Maedeh Aghaei1, Federico Parezzan3, Mariella Dimiccoli 1, Petia Radeva1;2, Marco Cristani3 1 Faculty of Mathematics and Computer Science, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain 2 Computer Vision Center, Barcelona, Spain 3 Department of Computer Science, University of Verona, Verona, Italy Abstract— In our society and century, clothing is not anymore Other studies shows that clothing correlates with the used only as a means for body protection. Our paper builds personality traits of people in a way that people with formal upon the evidence, studied within the social sciences, that cloth- clothing perceive actions and objects, the inter-relationship ing brings a clear communicative message in terms of social signals, influencing the impression and behaviour of others and the intra-relationship between them in a more meaningful towards a person. In fact, clothing correlates with personality manner [36]. Clothing can make a person feel comfortable traits, both in terms of self-assessment and assessments that or not in a social situation [16] and can be considered as a unacquainted people give to an individual. The consequences determinant of how long it takes for strangers to trust one of these facts are important: the influence of clothing on the and how much they may trust them [40]. Aforementioned decision making of individuals has been investigated in the literature, showing that it represents a discriminative factor studies are evidences of the importance of clothing in so- to differentiate among diverse groups of people. Unfortunately, cial signaling. Arguably, clothing can be considered as the this has been observed after cumbersome and expensive manual most evident blueprint of individuals, which is completely annotations, on very restricted populations, limiting the scope of dependent on their conscious choices, is not as transient as the resulting claims.
    [Show full text]
  • Power Can Bias Impression Processes: Stereotyping Subordinates by Default and by Design
    Group Processes & G Intergroup Relations P 2000 Vol 3(3) 227–256 I R Power Can Bias Impression Processes: Stereotyping Subordinates by Default and by Design Stephanie A. Goodwin Yale University Alexandra Gubin University of Massachusetts at Amherst Susan T. Fiske Princeton University Vincent Y. Yzerbyt Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve Powerholders may engage in two stereotyping processes: (a) by default, inattention to stereotype-inconsistent information, due to lack of dependency, and (b) by design, effortful attention to stereotype-consistent information, due to explicit control. Study 1 manipulated control (not dependency) over internship applicants; powerful decision-makers increased attention to stereotypic attributes, consistent with stereotyping by design. Study 2 measured differences in trait dominance as an analog to situational control, replicating Study 1. Study 3 separately manipulated perceiver control and dependency; powerful perceivers increased attention to powerless targets’ stereotypic attributes (by design) and also decreased attention to counter-stereotypic attributes (by default). Study 4 compared powerful perceivers’ ratings of potential subordinates to their own prior ratings of target categories and target traits. Relative to the powerless, powerful perceivers’ impressions were based significantly less on target traits, supporting the attention results. keywords impression formation, intergroup relations, power, stereotyping Powerful people are socially sanctioned to Author’s note judge others, as part of their positions. Jurors, Address correspondence to either Stephanie teachers, and managers, for example, must eval- A. Goodwin, Department of Psychology, Yale uate others to determine guilt, assign grades, or University, New Haven, CT 06520, USA, or promote employees. Powerful people – people Susan T. Fiske, Department of Psychology, who control other’s outcomes – could, like Green Hall, Princeton University, Princeton, anyone else, rely on stereotypes when they are NJ 08544–1010, USA.
    [Show full text]
  • Unintentional Influences in Intentional Impression Formation
    IMPRESSION FORMATION 1 Unintentional Influences in Intentional Impression Formation Bertram Gawronski, Skylar M. Brannon, & Dillon M. Luke University of Texas at Austin 1. Introduction competing theoretical explanations for unintentional influences in intentional impression formation and A substantial body of research suggests that evidence regarding the impact of theoretically derived perceivers spontaneously draw inferences from moderators that make such influences more or less observed behaviors even when they do not have the likely to occur. In the final part, we discuss broader intention to form a social impression. Such implications of the reviewed research for impression unintentional inferences have been found to give rise formation. to impressions of other people’s traits (i.e., spontaneous trait inference; see Uleman, Newman, & 2. Effects of Mere Co-occurrence and Relational Moskowitz, 1996) and goals (i.e., spontaneous goal Information inference; see Moskowitz & Olcaysoy Okten, 2016). Unintentional influences in intentional For example, when learning that Avery received an A impression formation can occur in various forms, as on a math exam, people may spontaneously infer that demonstrated by classic research on halo and priming Avery is smart; and when learning that Alex donated effects in impression formation. In the current chapter, $100 to a local food bank, people may spontaneously we focus on a more recent line work suggesting that infer that Alex had the goal to help. Although these evaluative responses to an object may be jointly impressions can be the result of intentional processes, influenced by (1) the mere co-occurrence of the object the notion of spontaneous inference suggests that they with a pleasant or unpleasant stimulus (e.g., mere co- may also arise from unintentional processes.
    [Show full text]
  • Operations in Forming an Impression of Another Person
    JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 18, 217-234 (1982) The Informational Basis of Social Judgments: Operations in Forming an Impression of Another Person EUGENE BURNSTEIN AND YAACOV SCHUL The University of Michigan Received October 7. 1980 This paper describes four functionally distinct operations carried out in forming an impression of another person: Initial encoding, elaborative encoding, inte- gration, and decision. These processes were identified empirically on the basis of their differential sensitivity to the amount and the consistency of information in a trait description. In Study 1 as the trait set increased in size, processing was hindered to the greatest extent during initial encoding, to a moderate extent during elaborative encoding, and least during the integration-decision interval. Varying consistency produced the opposite pattern of results, that is, an incon- sistent trait description hindered processing the least during initial encoding, a moderate amount during elaborative encoding, and the most during the integra- tion-decision interval. In Study 2 the quantity of information was manipulated via implicit knowledge, that is, the associative structure activated by the trait (rather than set size). Because implicit information is “preintegrated,” impli- cation-rich traits did not hinder initial or elaborative encoding more than impli- cation-poor traits. The decision operation, however, was performed more rapidly with implication-rich traits than with implication-poor ones, which strongly sug- gests that once information is integrated, the “richer” the representation, the easier it is to arrive at a decision. Traditionally, research on social judgments in general, and impression formation in particular, has been concerned with the functional rela- tionship between properties of the judgment (e.g., the extremity of the impression) and properties of the stimulus information (e.g., the number of traits, their consistency).
    [Show full text]
  • Towards a Deeper Understanding of Impression Formation –
    Title page with All Author Information Towards a Deeper Understanding of Impression Formation – New Insights Gained from a Cognitive-Ecological Perspective Johannes Prager1, Joachim I. Krueger2, and Klaus Fiedler 1 1University of Heidelberg 2 Brown University Running head: Cognitive-ecological analysis of impression formation Author Note: The work underlying the present article was supported by a grant provided by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft to the last author (FI 294/23-1). Email correspondence can be addressed either to [email protected] or to [email protected] Masked Manuscript without Author Information Towards a Deeper Understanding of Impression Formation – New Insights Gained from a Cognitive-Ecological Perspective XXXXX Running head: Cognitive-ecological analysis of impression formation Author Note: XXXXX 2 Cognitive-ecological analysis of impression formation Abstract Impression formation is a basic module of fundamental research in social cognition, with broad implications for applied research on interpersonal relations, social attitudes, employee selection, and person judgments in legal and political context. Drawing on a pool of 28 predominantly positive traits used in Solomon Asch’s (1946) seminal impression studies, two research teams have investigated the impact of the number of person traits sampled randomly from the pool on the evaluative impression of the target person. Whereas Norton, Frost, and Ariely (2007) found a “less-is-more” effect, reflecting less positive impressions with increasing sample size n, Ullrich, Krueger, Brod, and Groschupf (2013) concluded that an n-independent averaging rule can account for the data patterns obtained in both labs. We address this issue by disentangling different influences of n on resulting impressions, namely varying baserates of positive and negative traits, different sampling procedures, and trait diagnosticity.
    [Show full text]
  • Self-Presentation and Impression Formation Through Photographs in an LGBT Online Dating Community
    Self-presentation and Impression Formation through Photographs in an LGBT Online Dating Community By: Kai Fat Shum Department of Informatics and Media Media & Communication Studies Uppsala University Abstract This netnographic and autoethnographic study examines self-presentation and impression formation through photographs presented on a gay online dating community, Qruiser. The theoretical framework of Goffman’s performance of self and Asch’s formation of impression was developed based on semiotic signifiers and signified. The study was carried out through participant observation of 200 gay daters’ photographs, online interviews, group discussions and narratives of the author’s personal experience. Observation showed that gay daters primarily presented six categories of photographs: self-portrait, daily pictures, travel pictures, sexually explicit pictures, pictures of objects, and black and white pictures. The daters were asked to justify their photographs in self-presentations, and the responses showed that the signs in the photographs could be both intentional and unintentional. Online interviews and group discussion participants suggested that there were distinctions of central and peripheral signifiers in impression formation. Furthermore, participants felt that misunderstanding of impressions resulting from dating photographs was unimportant, due to the compensation of textual presentation and messages. Keywords: Photograph, Self-presentation, Impression Formation, Misunderstanding, online dating, Qruiser.com i Table of Contents
    [Show full text]
  • "Central Trait" Hypothesis Revisited1 Biii D. Bell and Gary G
    AN INTE RACTIONIST APPRAISAL OF IMPRESSION FORMATION The "Central Trait" Hypothesis Revisited1 BiII D. Bell and Gary Go Stanfield University of Missouri - Columbia This article examines the nature of first impressions from the interactionist perspective. A modified H. H. Kelley design (1950) of student-teacher interaction was employed with a sample of 195 college students. The fi ndi ngs demonstrate the overall complexi ty of the impression-forming process as we II as i IIustrate the Iimi­ tations of the warm-cold variable in predicting actual behcvior, Several central traits are observed to be operative in impression formation. These traits are observed to be a function both of preinformation and response alternatives in the odjecrive check­ list, Symbolic and observational presentation is found to be more influencial in forming impressions with behavioral implications than those formed solely by observational exposure. Both consid­ eration of the context of interaction and central trait identification appear necessary to specify the behavioral component of impressions. TheoreticcI Framework The interactionist perspective sheds needed light on the study of the formation and impli­ cations of first impressions. Briefly, this orientation asserts that social interaction proceeds on the basis of situational and personal definitions (Thomas, 1923j and Blumer, 1962). That is, in order for an individual to interact with another in some meaningful fashion, both must define not only the situational context of the interaction, but each must further define the other relative to the situation as well as their reciprocal relationship to one another. To make this process possible, a continual supply of information from the external environment is needed 0 This information can be obtained directly (experience) or indirectly (attitudes and opinions of others).
    [Show full text]
  • The Causes and Effects of Inferences of Impression Management in Consumption
    THE CAUSES AND EFFECTS OF INFERENCES OF IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT IN CONSUMPTION by Ethan Leigh Pancer A thesis submitted to the Department of Management – School of Business In conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Queen’s University Kingston, Ontario, Canada (August 2013) Copyright ©Ethan Leigh Pancer, 2013 Abstract Observers frequently make inferences of what consumers are like based on the products they use. The general view in social cognition is that the consumption behavior of others is taken at face value, where observers do not question the image being portrayed by product users. This assumption persists even though it is well known that consumers use products to manage the images they create. This dissertation aims to enrich our understanding of this issue by arguing that, under certain conditions, observers will make the inference that consumers use products to deliberately try to create certain impressions (i.e., perceptions as being phony, fake, or a poseur). Specifically, these inferences will undermine the impression, creating more negative attitudes towards the consumer. In fact, this dissertation argues that this inference can also play an important role in consumer decision making, impacting product evaluations directly (i.e., when consumers avoid products because they are concerned that others will infer they are impression managing). Five experiments examined elements of the product, the target, the situation, and the observer themselves to better understand the causes and effects of inferences of impression management. Taken as a whole, this dissertation highlights the central roles of the product’s contribution towards the target’s appearance, the attractiveness of their appearance, product functionality and feelings of observer threat in shaping IM inferences.
    [Show full text]