THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 1

Title: The Emergency of Prosociality: a developmental perspective on altruism and human prosocial behavior in the face of disaster

Short title: The Emergency of Prosociality

Lucie Rose*a a Université de Paris, INCC UMR 8002, CNRS, Paris, [email protected]

Guillaume Dezecacheb b Université Clermont Auvergne, LAPSCO CNRS, Clermont-Ferrand, France [email protected]

Tara Powellc c University of Illinois, Urbana Champaign, USA [email protected]

Sylvie Chokrona,d a Université de Paris, INCC UMR 8002, CNRS, Paris, France d Institut de Neuropsychologie, Neurovision et Neurocognition, Hôpital Fondation Rothschild, Paris, France [email protected]

Klara Kovarskia,d a Université de Paris, INCC UMR 8002, CNRS, Paris, France d Institut de Neuropsychologie, Neurovision et Neurocognition, Hôpital Fondation Rothschild, Paris, France [email protected]

*Corresponding author information: Lucie Rose, Université de Paris, INCC UMR 8002, CNRS, F-75006 Paris, France (e-mail: [email protected]).

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 2

Abstract: No two emergency situations are the same. A combination of human and contextual factors makes each emergency and disaster event unique in its timeline, its aftermath, and especially its impact on affected populations. Common to these situations, however, is a demonstration of prosocial behavior intended to benefit other victims. Can shared features of emergencies, viewed through the lens of evolutionary, cognitive, developmental and social psychology shed light on these prosocial and altruistic actions? In this narrative review, we survey the onset of prosociality and altruistic behavior in typical human development, and we focus on how it is expressed in emergency situations. We consider how prosociality shifts and adapts in the specific context of a pandemic. Finally, we suggest that a multidisciplinary research approach may help overcome methodological limitations and achieve more comprehensive understanding of prosociality’s underlying mechanisms in emergency contexts.

Keywords: prosocial behavior; altruism; development; COVID-19; emergency; disaster.

Data Availability: Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no new data were created or analyzed in this study.

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 3

1. Introduction

Within an evolutionary framework, cognitive and social psychology both have an interest in how individuals act in response to harmful situations. Researchers in social cognition, for example, frequently motivate their research by explaining how fast individuals process information in a hostile environment (see for review Crick & Dodge, 1994; Frankenhuis & de Weerth, 2013;

LoBue, 2009). However, this remains a shortcut of sorts to relate laboratory investigation to evolutionary mechanisms as well as actual events that have complex behavioral, psychological, and social consequences from childhood and beyond. Observing human behavior in emergency settings, including prosociality and altruism, requires stepping out of controlled environments and into real life situations to gain additional and enriched insight. In the present narrative review, we first discuss how emergencies can be defined and classified. Then, we provide an overview on the developmental aspects of prosocial and altruistic behavior in controlled research contexts by describing crucial milestones. Prosocial behaviors in response to emergency situations are described both in children and in adults. To conclude, considerations on the COVID-19 pandemic, and on the challenges of conducting research in these conditions will also be discussed.

2. Classifying emergencies to understand their consequences

Emergency situations are usually referred to using many different terms including emergency, disaster, catastrophe, adversity, accident, extra/non-ordinary event, major incident or crisis. Each has subtle or larger differences compared to neighboring terms leading to frequent misuse.

Classifying such events, however, has not traditionally been in the scope of psychology research, but it has been extensively put forth by management specialists, governments or international aid agencies (International Federation of Red Cross and Red Crescent Societies, 2006). For the sake

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 4

of simplicity, this article will preferentially employ the terms ‘emergency’ and ‘disaster’ to refer

to these situations. The first definitions to introduce disasters through a sociological lens date back

to the post-World War II era. On disasters, Perry (2018) highlights a shift from definitions

underlining uncontrollable aspects of an event towards a more anthropocentric view. Sjoberg

(1962, p. 357) built upon this notion to define a disaster as a “severe, relatively sudden, frequently

unexpected disruption” of the social context (see also Fritz, 1961). The event itself may occur

suddenly, and the magnitude, scope, and effects are often unpredictable. Such is the case with

earthquakes or hurricanes, where a natural hazard can lead to a disaster situation with or without a

secondary, subsequent natural hazard (Gill & Malamud, 2017).

Various factors participating in defining emergency events have been classified in the past

(Cocking & Drury, 2008; Mladjan & Cvetković, 2013; Münch, 2011; Shaluf, 2007). In order to

better understand the varied nature of emergency situations, and in an attempt to shed light on

shared mechanisms and consequences between contexts that appear quite different from an

external point of view, factors characterizing emergency events are listed in Table 1.

Table 1

Selective list of shared factors in emergency events affecting humans, considered in this review as

non-binary and lying on a spectrum. Sub-types, details as well as examples are provided for each

factor

Factor Sub-Types Details Examples

Naturally occurring (geophysical, Geophysical-Earthquake

Natural hazards hydrological, climatological, biological) Hydrological-Floods Causes Climatological-Hurricane

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 5

Biological-Pandemic

Man-made Caused by Humans (Conflict, industrial Conflict-War

/technological accidents) Industrial-Nuclear

Man-made and Natural- exacerbated Hurricane Katrina-caused by a

Hybrid through human response or vice versa hurricane, exacerbated through

insufficient flood defenses

Initiated with the intention to harm other Mass terrorist attacks Voluntary humans Intent No intent of causing harm or destruction Involuntary setting of fires due to Involuntary electrical maintenance

Discrete Specific and identifiable moment in time Momentary touchdown of a tornado

Detrimental consequences build up, Repeated aftershocks following an Timeframe Continuous creating emergency situation, as an event earthquake; Long-term spread of an

unfolds on the longer term epidemic/pandemic

Geographically Neighborhoods and towns School shooting

isolated

Location Geographic regions (e.g. Middle East, Droughts, tsunamis Regional Europe, Africa)

Global Across multiple continents Pandemics

Directly impact from experiencing the Witnessing serious injury or death Direct (Primary) Impact on emergency humans Secondary loss and stressors after an Loss of jobs, disruption in schools, lack of Indirect (Secondary) emergency financial assistance or infrastructure

Results from directly being exposed or Injuries, deaths, malnutrition,

Outcomes Physical Health from environmental factors after an communicable diseases

emergency

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 6

Results from stress due to loss, Depression, post-traumatic stress

Mental Health experiencing threat or compromised disorder, anxiety

safety

Economic hardship Loss of homes, jobs, livelihoods Housing instability, food insecurity

All types of individuals directly or Physical injuries or post-traumatic stress General population indirectly affected by emergency situation disorder following a terror attack,

Children (dependent on caregivers for Loss of family members, disruption from

access to basic needs, fewer coping schools; homelessness due to destruction

Victims resources), socio-economically insecure of housing or furlough following business Vulnerable individuals (increased vulnerability to closures; food insecurity after closure of populations loss of secure housing or employment), neighborhood stores, lack of access to

older adults (chronic health conditions, medicines or adequate healthcare

impaired physical ability)

Immediate, often coinciding with Providing essential services, providing Short-term response food and shelter Recovery Begins weeks after a disaster and can last Redevelopment and restoration of social timeline Long-term years depending on severity of a disaster and economic infrastructure of

communities

Altogether, emergency situations do indeed share some features including unexpectedness and

uncertain severity of possible outcomes for victims, yet they also present crucial differences

highlighting strong heterogeneity due to the wide spectrum of circumstances and the lens under

which they are considered. Accordingly, by considering behavioral aspects, pandemics such as the

one caused by SARS-CoV-2 may belong to a hybrid category: rather than being considered a

natural disaster, their cause is biological, but their propagation partly depends on human behavior,

and consequences are undeniably social. This is also the case when considering devastating

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 7 pathogens that emerge in monocultures with severe consequences on employment and population nutrition, as with the Irish Potato Famine (McDonald & Stukenbrock, 2016). Although existing classifications rarely consider the individual behavioral and psycho-emotional mechanisms rolled out during and after these contexts, a growing body of research has started to focus on these consequences including psychological, health, social or educational distress (see for example

Goldmann & Galea, 2014; Leytham & Powell, 2012; Nguyen & Pham, 2018), as well as on cooperation, prosociality and altruism (Kaniasty & Norris, 2004). These concepts and their definition outside of emergency situations have been at the core of a vast body of literature from an evolutionary perspective in human adults (Preston & de Waal, 2002; West et al., 2007).

The emergence of prosocial behavior and altruism and their milestones in children and into adulthood have also been studied in controlled experiments using a developmental framework

(Batson et al., 1991; Eisenberg et al., 2006; Preston & de Waal, 2002). It is an essential stopover to subsequently decode these more complex situations and is presented in the following section.

Beyond controlled settings, however, real-life dangerous situations present a unique terrain to study prosocial behavior and altruism in children as well as adults in a natural context. Children are often considered as somewhat helpless and relying on adult assistance in this type of situation

(for an evolutionary perspective see Zeveloff & Boyce, 1982), yet studies show that similarly to adults, children and adolescents involved in a disaster develop both an orientation towards helping

(Vezzali et al., 2016) and prosocial behavior within the community (Bokszczanin, 2012).

3. Dynamics of prosocial behavior development

Prosocial behavior is defined as behavior stemming from the intention to benefit another

(Eisenberg, 1986) and has been found to develop beginning in early childhood. Altruistic behavior,

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 8 motivated by the goal to help another yet without any material profit for oneself (Filkowski et al.,

2016), also emerges in the first decade of life. Prosocial and altruistic behaviors, with a primary motive to benefit another individual, can be considered distinct from behaviors intrinsically associated with a certain level of reciprocity such as collaboration. Research has investigated the relationship between empathy and helping behavior, offering clues to understand the early development of prosocial behavior (Eisenberg et al., 2010; Eisenberg & Miller, 1987). From the earliest weeks of life, babies have the ability to empathize and identify with others (Repacholi &

Gopnik, 1997), through perception and imitation of facial and emotional expressions (Field et al.,

1982; Meltzoff & Moore, 1983). Infants prefer to watch and touch puppets that help rather than hinder each other (Hamlin et al., 2008, 2010; Hamlin & Wynn, 2011). In a similar way, toddlers display spontaneous helping behavior from the age of 18 months and pick up objects for an adult

(Over & Carpenter, 2009; Warneken & Tomasello, 2007). Children also exhibit comforting behaviors towards peers in distress with attempts to reach for and touch a crying baby from 8 months of age and both physical and verbal comforting observed from age 2 onwards (Dunfield &

Kuhlmeier, 2013; Hoffman, 2000; Liddle et al., 2015; Zahn-Waxler et al., 1992).

Perspective-taking, the ability to take into account points of view of others on a cognitive and later on an affective level (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009), becomes increasingly robust from age 3 and beyond, and proves especially crucial when considering complex situations where moral decision- making involves high needs of some individuals and high costs to those who help (Sierksma et al.,

2014). The development of such complex skills impacts not only empathy processes, but also comes into play in social responsibility (see for review Imuta et al., 2016). This is considered as a value orientation founded on concern for others and on the development of notions of fairness and justice (Wray‐Lake & Syvertsen, 2011), the latter usually observed as early as age 3 (Vaish et al.,

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 9

2010). Enacting prosocial behavior goes beyond experiencing a sense of empathy and social responsibility. These notions also rely on emotional regulation in order to express the correct amount and timing of emotion and distress, influenced by executive functioning including inhibition, working memory and cognitive flexibility (Astuto & Ruck, 2010; Eisenberg, 2000; Rice et al., 2007).

Around age 6, with school as a key environment, children begin to understand not only the nature of other children’s emotions and reactions, but also what could help them in a specific situation

(Litvack-Miller et al., 1997). At this point, prosocial behavior evolves from a mostly dyadic to a group context (Hepach & Warneken, 2018), and in-group preference when undertaking an altruistic action such as attributing a monetary sum in an experimental donation game is found to increase throughout childhood as social groups become more salient and children’s classification skills improve (Bigler & Liben, 2016; Gummerum et al., 2009).

In early adolescence, prosocial behavior tends to decrease until age 13 before increasing again, influenced and modulated by emotional regulation, empathic concern and perspective-taking

(Carlo et al., 2010, 2015; Davis & Franzoi, 1991; Luengo Kanacri et al., 2013; Padilla‐Walker &

Christensen, 2011; Van der Graaff et al., 2018). The development of prosocial behavior in adolescence in relationship to the community as a whole has also been studied and underlines the role of self-efficacy within the family, a marker of autonomy, as a predictor of future civic involvement (Kanacri et al., 2014). Taken together, research on childhood and adolescent prosocial behavior sheds light on its singular complexity, with the involvement of emotional and cognitive skills as well as the surrounding environment and a rich social web of interactions that follow individuals into adulthood.

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 10

In adulthood, most individuals display altruism aimed at maximizing resource utility in a laboratory setting (Andreoni & Miller, 2002). Levels of displayed prosocial behavior evolve, with an increase in empathic concern and problem-solving skills with age (Sze et al., 2012).

There has been much debate regarding whether altruism or prosocial behaviors are generally stable processes in time in adults, manifesting as an altruistic or a prosocial personality that displays empathic concern for others with subsequent helpful actions (Gergen et al., 1972; Penner &

Finkelstein, 1998). Eisenberg et al. (2002) found in a longitudinal research on individuals ages 4 to 26 that self-reported prosocial dispositions in early adulthood were related to self-reports of empathy, sympathy and helping behavior during childhood.

Behavior intended to benefit others is observed throughout the lifespan, yet it can differ in various conditions. For example, being observed by others influences prosocial actions (Cañigueral &

Hamilton, 2019), as does involvement in an emergency situation with immediate displays of shared solidarity and sense of community (Páez et al., 2007). Considering prosocial behavior in emergency situations also requires examining the wider psychological effects of these contexts and their relationship with the initiation of helping behavior. Children are highly vulnerable to physical and psychological consequences during and after emergencies because of their reliance on caregivers who may be overwhelmed or underprepared during the event, and owing to fewer resources to cope with what they experienced (Goldmann & Galea, 2014; Kousky, 2016).

Understanding the factors that play a part in these consequences, yet that may also give rise to helping behavior, provides important insight on the far-reaching effects of emergencies on behavior.

3.1 Method

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 11

For this review, a classification of the various types of disasters and emergency situations was formulated based on prior disaster classifications and consensus among authors of this article. This classification was then used as a basis for database searches to ensure that all major types of disasters and emergency situations previously discussed were included. Disaster-related searches of English-language publications were conducted between July 2020 and January 2021 in PubMed,

PsycINFO and Google Scholar. A focus was placed on research published in the last decade, as well as findings related to specific and older emergency events and published in the aftermath of these events. Major conceptual findings from earlier years were also included when they helped frame the review. All authors participated in study review and comparison in order to provide a current and updated overview on behavior and consequences of emergency situations.

4. Emergency situations and their impact on children, from psychological consequences

to prosocial behavior

A significant amount of research has examined adverse psychological outcomes among children in emergency contexts. Psychological symptoms associated with disaster exposure include internalizing symptoms such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), depression, or generalized anxiety; and externalizing symptoms such as aggression, conduct problems, and behavior problems (Adams et al., 2014; Pfefferbaum et al., 2014; Rubens et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2013).

A host of secondary stressors increase a child’s risk for experiencing psychological distress, including displacement from home and school, loss or damage to their homes, breakdowns in supportive networks, increased risk of abuse and neglect, poverty, and pre-existing mental health symptoms (Adams et al., 2014; Becker-Blease et al., 2010; Kousky, 2016; Lock et al., 2012;

Pfefferbaum et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2013). While many young people will experience

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 12 psychological distress after an emergency, most will adapt to their environment with only a subset experiencing sustained psychopathology.

Protective factors can buffer the psychological impact of exposure to emergencies and related secondary adversities (Fegert et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2020; Masten & Narayan, 2012; Pfefferbaum et al., 2015). Individual-level as well as collective-level factors interact to promote resilience following a disasters (Cadamuro et al., 2021). Peer and caregiver support, adaptive coping, emotion regulation, perceived sense of control, self-esteem, and prosocial behaviors have all been associated with lower psychopathology after an emergency (Fegert et al., 2020; Lengua et al.,

2005; Lewis et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2020; Masten & Narayan, 2012; Pfefferbaum et al., 2015). For example, La Greca et al. (2013) found that hurricane-affected youth who reported higher social support experienced fewer PTSD symptoms than those with low levels of support.

Some of these factors may increase prosocial behaviors in addition to reducing adverse outcomes in disaster-affected children (Drury & Williams, 2012; Hobfoll et al., 2007; Masten & Narayan,

2012; Pynoos et al., 2007). Higher socio-economic status and greater community acceptance, for example, were found to be predictors of prosocial attitudes among children affected by war

(Betancourt et al., 2013). Interventions designed to help children within emergency contexts gain perceived sense of control, emotion regulation, problem solving, and community resilience by targeting individual (e.g., self-regulation, coping ability) and interpersonal (e.g., trust in peer and caregiver relationships) factors may be effective in increasing prosocial behavior (Jordans et al.,

2010; Sanderson et al., 2016; Tol et al., 2013). In the aftermath of a tornado, Powell and Thompson

(2016) also found that a psychoeducation and solution-focused program delivered to small groups of children led to greater improvements in prosocial behaviors compared to a waitlist control group.

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 13

Although research tends to focus on the short- and long-term consequences of emergencies and disasters on children (Wolmer et al., 2017), their behavior in the midst of such an event and its direct aftermath is equally interesting and important. It appears that youth and adolescents tend to engage in prosocial and helping behavior when confronted with a variety of emergency situations, echoing prosocial tendencies assessed in laboratory settings (Bokszczanin, 2012; Macksoud &

Aber, 1996; Raboteg‐šaric et al., 1994; Vezzali et al., 2016).

In terms of spontaneous prosocial behavior observed in situ, children as young as five or six years old in a context of war developed increased prosocial behavior compared to before the onset of war in a longitudinal study (Raboteg‐šaric et al., 1994). This illustration of general early prosociality in an emergency context contrasts with findings establishing an age difference in children’s prosocial behavior. Li et al. (2013) found that levels of prosocial behavior increased after a major earthquake in older 9-year-old children, yet they decreased in younger children (6 years old). Another illustration can be found among pre-adolescents ages 9 to 12, who expressed helping intentions towards fellow victims of an earthquake and its multiple aftershocks (Vezzali et al., 2016). Similarly, children ages 10 to 16 exposed to wartime separation from parents and witnesses to violent acts reported higher prosocial attitude than children unexposed to violence directed towards others (Macksoud & Aber, 1996). Adolescents tend to engage in spontaneous helping, as illustrated by Bokszczanin (2012) in the case of major flooding and subsequent social support provided by adolescents to the community through material, emotional, and informational assistance. The spotlight has also been put on adolescent altruistic behavior in crisis situations including school shootings (Turkewitz et al., 2019).

In addition to understanding and preventing the negative consequences of exposure to emergency situations, long-term consequences on prosocial behavior itself are an essential parameter. The

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 14 lack of longitudinal study designs due to the difficult task of recruiting disaster victims as subjects and ensuring proper and necessary follow-up (Maki et al., 2019) sheds very little light on this question. Li et al. (2013) found that, in keeping with the “altruistic community” notion that underlines immediate post-disaster helping (Barton, 1969), altruistic tendencies returned to baseline levels several months after exposure to a natural disaster. However, in a wartime context, an example of a long-lasting and continuous crisis situation with exposure to violence and aggression led children to identify with main society-wide helpers and express their desire to become doctors, teachers, or lawyers to foster peace (Raundalen & Dyregrov, 1991). This finding is in line with studies among adults that highlighted increased altruism towards both in-group and outgroup members in war-torn populations over a year after a war (Hartman & Morse, 2020).

Engaging in prosocial actions towards an individual’s own community also appears to increase family and peer relationships and foster overall adjustment, a sense of identity, and self-efficacy

(Pancer et al., 2007).

Research on prosocial behaviors in disaster-affected children has garnered growing attention in recent years, yet additional studies are needed to understand specific influencing factors in order to provide adequate support for subsequent events and in their path towards adulthood. The mechanisms that help explain and understand the onset of prosocial behavior have been considered in adults and are detailed in the following section, adding yet another layer to our understanding of the development of human prosociality in emergency situations.

5. Prosocial behavior and altruism displayed by adults in an emergency context

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 15

Despite the common and persistent belief that adults generally engage in self-preservative behavior in the face of danger (Dezecache, 2015; Drury et al., 2013; Schweingruber & Wohlstein, 2005), we now know that perception of danger is largely associated with subsequent prosociality

(Dezecache, 2015; Drury, 2018). Prosociality during an emergency, when individuals remain under a threat, is somewhat of a puzzle (Dezecache, 2015). At the proximate levels of analysis, it has been proposed that routine social norms continue to prevail (Johnson, 1987a, 1987b) or that the exposure to a common danger fosters a sense of social identity that introduces ad hoc ‘in- group’ supportive norms (Drury, 2018). The question remains of why natural selection favors individuals that possess traits that would make them prone to pay a cost for others, often non- related individuals, at their direct expense in particular when reciprocity is unlikely to occur. It may be that such situations were not part of our ecology, rarely exposing us to high danger together with many non-genetically related individuals. This would mean our spontaneous and prosocial- leaning social heuristics (Rand et al., 2012; Rand & Epstein, 2014) would be maintained because they have proven advantageous over our evolutionary past. Alternatively, actions considered to be prosocial may in fact be strategies that also maximize one’s own immediate benefits, either because social contact is in itself reassuring and a primitive need (Dezecache et al., 2017;

Dezecache, Frith, et al., 2020; Mawson, 2005, 2017) or because we flexibly adjust behavior as the function of contextual features and use prosocial strategies when they are immediately more profitable than non-social or self-preservative ones (e.g. possible escape, see Dezecache, Martin, et al., 2020; but see also Bartolucci et al., 2021).

Despite conceptual quandaries, prosociality is commonly observed in adults during emergencies, that is, shortly after the onset of the emergency, and before it gets resolved (Dezecache, 2015).

Two recent examples from the literature can be used to highlight the reality of these phenomena

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 16 in highly life-threatening situations. In a recent study (Dezecache, Martin, et al., 2020), 32 semi- directed interviews were conducted with survivors from the mass shooting at Bataclan concert hall in Paris, France, in November 2015. On that day, three terrorists armed with rifles entered the concert hall and shot at the 1,500 spectators, before taking hostages. Interviews revealed that prosocial behavior was commonly reported, although affected by the material situation of respondents (e.g., whether they were minimally protected by a wall or not; and whether they could exit or not). Prosocial behavior, possibly specific to this type of danger, included giving information to others (e.g., telling other hostages that the shooter was reloading his weapon, giving a few seconds to escape), providing physical protection (e.g., touching other hostages to provide physical comfort) or emotional comfort (e.g., reassuring others). A tentative analysis further suggested that the bulk of these prosocial behaviors likely reflected altruistic tendencies, providing benefits to others at cost for oneself.

Another recent study examined court hearings from direct witnesses of the disaster in in November 2012 in which a sank after a collision with an underwater rock (Bartolucci et al., 2021). A majority of passengers left their cabins and decided to evacuate, against crew instructions. Cooperation and help, including passing along crucial information, were commonly reported. Again, some particular features of the environment such as the perceived rarity of lifeboats may have promoted competitive behavior among victims. Generally, however, cooperation was the norm (Bartolucci et al., 2021).

It is not easy to draw a firm line between the very moment of the emergency and the end of it, notably because it is not always clear to victims themselves that a threat has disappeared for good, for example in the case of earthquakes that typically entail aftershocks. Research has shown, however, that prosociality is also very common in the aftermath of an emergency. For instance,

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 17 commuters of London buses helped one another following a bombing by providing physical and emotional support to others (Drury et al., 2009). Again, the feeling that there was a shared common fate among victims appeared to be critical in shaping prosocial actions among them, in bombings as in other emergency situations (on earthquakes, see Drury et al., 2016). What happens when physical and emotional support cannot be provided in the traditional ways described above?

Epidemics and pandemics, with the current COVID-19 pandemic as an unfortunate yet timely illustration, tend to highlight that humans are able to engage in prosocial behavior adapted to environmental and contextual constraints.

6. Staying far to remain close: reinventing prosocial behavior in the COVID-19

pandemic

The SARS-CoV-2 virus, identified in January 2020, began a swift worldwide spread as the fifth documented pandemic since the 1918 influenza outbreak (Liu et al., 2020). Lockdowns and shelter-in-place measures implemented by numerous countries drastically altered daily life as children and adults were asked to stay home to stop the spread of the virus, with social isolation, disrupted routines, school closures, mask wearing and separation from peers (Bartlett & Vivrette,

2020; Pfefferbaum & North, 2020). After a short subside in the pandemic’s progression in many countries throughout the summer, cases began rising again in the fall months, launching the

“second wave” of the virus observed at time of writing.

Immediate prosocial behavior displayed by individuals within their communities during the first wave involved spontaneous acts of helping with daily tasks, showing support for essential workers, and fostering an enhanced sense of community through collective behaviors such as clapping at a certain hour (Wolf et al., 2020). Despite the lack of in-person social contact, group affiliation and

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 18 social interaction seeking behaviors were observed during the first wave in many countries, especially via social networks and humor-based content (Amici, 2020; Aresi et al., 2020). This behavior typically creates collective emotion and may increase solidarity (Garcia & Rimé, 2019;

Zaki, 2020).

Beyond the initial burst of compassion, often referred to as a “post-disaster utopia” that characterizes immediate and visible helping behaviors (Fritz, 1961; Morris-Suzuki, 2017), prosocial behavior in the longer-term progression of the pandemic is of particular interest.

Importantly, the pandemic has offered up a novel facet of prosocial behavior, redefining and enriching the notion of what it means to act prosocially or altruistically.

Indeed, as the world realized that the COVID-19 pandemic was a relatively long-term situation with no rapidly available treatment or vaccine and the short-lived sense of “altruistic community” waned, physical distancing and mask wearing became necessary to limit propagation of the virus

(Chu et al., 2020). Physically staying away from others has become the new way of helping.

Prosocial comforting, at least in-person, has temporarily been replaced by prosocial protection (see

Figure 1). Providing emotional comfort must bypass the hurdle of hampered emotional reading due to mask wearing (Carbon, 2020; Nestor et al., 2020).

Figure 1

Possible changes in individual prosocial behavior in an epidemic, based on the developmental model of the three types of prosocial behavior introduced by Dunfield & Kuhlmeier, 2013

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 19

Despite going against this human tendency to provide physical comfort (Liddle et al., 2015), individuals have been found to be receptive to public messages explaining the benefits of these measures and generally act accordingly and prosocially (Abel & Brown, 2020; Heffner et al., 2021;

Jordan et al., 2020). On a more individual level, adopting this long-haul prosocial behavior appears to be associated with greater empathy (Pfattheicher et al., 2020) as well as a heightened sense of social responsibility (Alvis et al., 2020; Oosterhoff & Palmer, 2020). These findings are interesting to characterize prosociality in epidemic and pandemic settings as a whole, yet future research will be necessary to provide a wider outlook on prosocial behavior exhibited from the early stages up until the end of the pandemic as well as in different cultures affected.

7. Discussion and methodological considerations

Altruistic actions in the midst and aftermath of an emergency depend largely on context, immediate danger, and individual capabilities at a given moment (see Table 1). Emergency situations are intrinsically uncertain, with an array of possible impacts on victims and their living conditions,

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 20 livelihood, health and in the most extreme conditions, life itself. They have consequences on an individual level, as well as on an interpersonal, group, and intergroup level, making the question of prosocial actions amongst individuals, both children and adults, all the more essential in these contexts (Cadamuro et al., 2021). Despite individual danger including the risk of losing one’s life, prosocial behaviors are observed in many different emergency situations (e.g., natural disasters, wars, terror attacks…) regardless of their timeframe, location and outcome, although they do not give rise to one single type of behavior. Studies highlight that humans have a natural tendency to act altruistically and do so both in childhood and adulthood despite the many hurdles in emergency and disaster situations, suggesting that prosociality is observed early in development, persists in adulthood and is also observed in different cultures. As observed in laboratory research, prosociality relies on several social and perceptual mechanisms including empathy, executive function, and adhesion to social norms.

However, when physical proximity becomes impossible, and when facial expressions remain hidden (e.g. mask wearing during the COVID-19 pandemic), humans adapt and rely on other cues and strategies, from perspective-taking to the use of humor and its trademark reliance on incongruity, to reduce their own negative affect and that of others as well as develop creative collaborative and prosocial problem-solving (Bailey & Im-Bolter, 2020; Strick et al., 2009;

Warneken et al., 2011). These strategies allow individuals to engage in helping, sharing and comforting behavior, albeit in ways that take into account contextual constraints. It appears that despite significant changes in daily life and the context in which prosocial behavior is usually deployed, humans are drawn to enacting such behavior and developing the means to do so.

Researching prosocial behaviors in response to emergencies, however, presents several major challenges and limitations. Such situations occur suddenly and by definition cannot be studied in

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 21 controlled settings (although video surveillance footage recorded during an event is used in some instances in behavioral research, see Philpot et al., 2020). Consequently, research takes place in a variety of timeframes within a crisis, from a handful of hours to months or even years for other situations. Whereas some studies focus extensively on behavior, or the report thereof, in the midst of an emergency (Dezecache, Martin, et al., 2020), others analyze the immediate aftermath of events, or consequences ranging in the weeks to years after a traumatic event (Masten & Osofsky,

2010).

The complex dynamics of prosociality following a disaster thereby imply that a post-event longitudinal design may be the most appropriate to develop a full view of post-disaster functioning in a social context. However, studies are often retrospective with rare pre-event measures (on the

Sichuan earthquake see Li et al., 2013; on the 2010 earthquake see Drury et al., 2016 and

Maki et al., 2019; on the Christchurch earthquake see Sibley & Bulbulia, 2012). An additional consideration is the waxing and waning of prosocial behavior in emergency contexts, where instant unity in the aftermath of a disaster can be followed by low levels of perceived social support within an affected community as time goes by (Kaniasty & Norris, 2004; Norris, 2005; Palinkas et al.,

1993). Participants can also be hard to track down due to injuries, emotional distress, displacement and poor communication infrastructure in the midst or directly following an emergency

(Goldmann & Galea, 2014). Due to heterogeneity and unpredictability of behaviors, methods are hardly comparable, making accurate replication and generalization challenging. Research methods in groups of children, for example, range from self-reports or teacher reports to experimental procedures more closely resembling techniques used in a laboratory setting such as the dictator game, a measure of altruistic tendency (Benenson et al., 2007; Li et al., 2013). Encouragingly,

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 22 recent studies have adopted multi-informant and multi-method approaches for research on vulnerable and at-risk youth (Shi et al., 2020).

A difference of note between prosociality observed in controlled settings compared to in situ altruistic behavior is the factor of uncertainty. Uncertainty can be manipulated in laboratory experiments, yet it remains fixed in time, much different from the repeated uncertainty that individuals experience in an emergency situation. The effect of uncertainty on prosocial behavior in the long run is a necessary question, as humans collectively progress towards uncertain times when it comes to climate and political stability. Indeed, research has found that uncertainty regarding the possible negative impact of their actions on another’s well-being leads to prosocial actions (Kappes et al., 2018), yet uncertain cost to oneself on the contrary leads to more excuse- driven behavior and lowered generosity in a laboratory setting and in a virtual emergency setting

(Bode et al., 2015; Garcia et al., 2020). Prosociality in the COVID-19 pandemic, for example, seems intrinsically related to a feeling of uncertainty. It may therefore prove to be a valuable real- life illustration for future global events, including considerations of vaccination as a large-scale prosocial solution to the pandemic with an associated and widely-shared element of uncertainty

(Wells et al., 2020). Group belonging and its relationship to prosocial behavior must also be taken into account, as in-group identification tends to act towards reducing or at the very least tolerating uncertainty and also promotes prosocial behavior (Everett et al., 2015; Hogg, 2001; Vives &

FeldmanHall, 2018). A feeling of proximity, whether through group belonging or shared features, may modulate altruistic behavior and may extend beyond physical interaction to adopting new societal rules or choosing when to donate to a specific cause (on the name-letter effect in disaster donations see Albayrak-Aydemir & Gleibs, 2021; Chandler et al., 2008; Duclos & Barasch, 2014;

Tunçgenç et al., 2021).

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 23

8. Conclusion

Despite their great heterogeneity, taken together, studies on prosocial behavior tend to confirm that humans across cultures act altruistically during and immediately in response to harmful situations (Aknin et al., 2013). However, it remains unclear how long such behavior lasts in time and whether acting prosocially has long-term positive consequences at the individual level, given that factors such as type of exposure, i.e. material damage or witnessing human distress, can differentially impact prosocial response (Falk & Graeber, 2020; Vardy & Atkinson, 2019).

On a final and more semantic note, the concept of prosociality itself is used differently in various fields. If prosociality is defined as any behavior that is advantageous to another individual, whether or not the behavior is also advantageous to the agent (i.e., whether it is altruistic or cooperative, see Dezecache, Martin, et al., 2020) may affect which behaviors are considered by researchers. A behavior that is immediately advantageous to a given recipient may also end up being detrimental in the longer run, making prosociality difficult to define precisely across fields of research.

Despite obvious and important methodological challenges, existing studies are of great interest and provide a necessary terrain to bring laboratory measurements and real-life contexts together.

Implementing robust and insightful analysis of prosocial behavior in emergency settings themselves requires prior understanding of what occurs in controlled settings. One needs the another to gain a wider picture of the many variables that affect human reaction to danger as well as the full scope of possible behaviors, from childhood to adulthood.

This full scope can only be reached when researchers from different fields bring their unique approach, method and theory together to help complete the ever-growing puzzle of how humans act in the face of disaster situations. An evolutionary approach provides a robust framework for

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 24 the observation and interpretation of behavior and psychological mechanisms (Muthukrishna &

Henrich, 2019). Apprehending human behavior in response to actual harm is fundamental, and interdisciplinary collaborations between researchers interested in these topics are also an indispensable condition towards a better understanding of prosociality in emergency across the lifespan.

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 25

References

Abel, M., & Brown, W. (2020). Prosocial Behavior in the Time of COVID-19: The Effect of

Private and Public Role Models. In IZA Discussion Papers (No. 13207; IZA Discussion

Papers). Institute of Labor Economics (IZA).

https://ideas.repec.org/p/iza/izadps/dp13207.html

Adams, Z. W., Sumner, J. A., Danielson, C. K., McCauley, J. L., Resnick, H. S., Grös, K., Paul,

L. A., Welsh, K. E., & Ruggiero, K. J. (2014). Prevalence and predictors of PTSD and

depression among adolescent victims of the Spring 2011 tornado outbreak. Journal of

Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(9), 1047–1055. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12220

Aknin, L. B., Barrington-Leigh, C. P., Dunn, E. W., Helliwell, J. F., Burns, J., Biswas-Diener,

R., Kemeza, I., Nyende, P., Ashton-James, C. E., & Norton, M. I. (2013). Prosocial

spending and well-being: Cross-cultural evidence for a psychological universal. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 635–652.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031578

Albayrak-Aydemir, N., & Gleibs, I. H. (2021). Measuring global bystander intervention and

exploring its antecedents for helping refugees. British Journal of Psychology, 112(2),

519–548. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12474

Alvis, L., Shook, N., & Oosterhoff, B. (2020). Adolescents’ Prosocial Experiences During the

COVID-19 Pandemic: Associations with Mental Health and Community Attachments.

https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/2s73n

Amici, P. (2020). Humor in the Age of COVID-19 Lockdown: An Explorative Qualitative

Study. Psychiatria Danubina, 32(Suppl 1), 15–20.

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 26

Andreoni, J., & Miller, J. (2002). Giving According to GARP: An Experimental Test of the

Consistency of Preferences for Altruism. Econometrica, 70(2), 737–753.

Aresi, G., Procentese, F., Gattino, S., Tzankova, I., Gatti, F., Compare, C., Marzana, D.,

Mannarini, T., Fedi, A., Marta, E., & Guarino, A. (2020). Prosocial behaviours under

collective quarantine conditions. A latent class analysis study during the 2020 COVID-19

lockdown in Italy. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/jb5hw

Astuto, J., & Ruck, M. D. (2010). Early Childhood as a Foundation for Civic Engagement. In

Handbook of Research on Civic Engagement in Youth (pp. 249–275). John Wiley &

Sons, Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470767603.ch10

Bailey, K., & Im-Bolter, N. (2020). My way or your way? Perspective taking during social

problem solving. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 66, 101087.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2019.101087

Bartlett, J., & Vivrette, R. (2020). Ways to Promote Children’s Resilience to the COVID-19

Pandemic. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.22081.99681

Bartolucci, A., Casareale, C., & Drury, J. (2021). Cooperative and competitive behaviour among

passengers during the costa concordia disaster. Safety Science, 134, 105055.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2020.105055

Barton, A. H. (1969). Communities in disaster: A sociological analysis of collective stress

situations. Doubleday.

Batson, C. D., Batson, J. G., Slingsby, J. K., Harrell, K. L., Peekna, H. M., & Todd, R. M.

(1991). Empathic joy and the empathy-altruism hypothesis. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 61(3), 413–426. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.61.3.413

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 27

Becker-Blease, K., Turner, H., & Finkelhor, D. (2010). Disasters, victimization, and children’s

mental health. Child Development. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01453.x

Benenson, J. F., Pascoe, J., & Radmore, N. (2007). Children’s altruistic behavior in the dictator

game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(3), 168–175.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.10.003

Betancourt, T. S., Meyers-Ohki, S. E., Charrow, A. P., & Tol, W. A. (2013). Interventions for

children affected by war: An ecological perspective on psychosocial support and mental

health care. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 21(2), 70–91.

https://doi.org/10.1097/HRP.0b013e318283bf8f

Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2016). Developmental Intergroup Theory: Explaining and

Reducing Children’s Social Stereotyping and Prejudice. Current Directions in

Psychological Science. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-

8721.2007.00496.x

Bode, N. W. F., Miller, J., O’Gorman, R., & Codling, E. A. (2015). Increased costs reduce

reciprocal helping behaviour of humans in a virtual evacuation experiment. Scientific

Reports, 5(1), 15896. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep15896

Bokszczanin, A. (2012). Social support provided by adolescents following a disaster and

perceived social support, sense of community at school, and proactive coping. Anxiety,

Stress, & Coping, 25(5), 575–592. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2011.622374

Cadamuro, A., Birtel, M. D., Bernardo, G. A. D., Crapolicchio, E., Vezzali, L., & Drury, J.

(2021). Resilience in children in the aftermath of disasters: A systematic review and a

new perspective on individual, interpersonal, group, and intergroup level factors. Journal

of Community & Applied Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.2500

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 28

Cañigueral, R., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2019). Being watched: Effects of an audience on eye

gaze and prosocial behaviour. Acta Psychologica, 195, 50–63.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.02.002

Carbon, C.-C. (2020). Wearing Face Masks Strongly Confuses Counterparts in Reading

Emotions. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.566886

Carlo, G., Mestre, M. V., Samper, P., Tur, A., & Armenta, B. E. (2010). Feelings or cognitions?

Moral cognitions and emotions as longitudinal predictors of prosocial and aggressive

behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(8), 872–877.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.010

Carlo, G., Padilla-Walker, L. M., & Nielson, M. G. (2015). Longitudinal bidirectional relations

between adolescents’ sympathy and prosocial behavior. Developmental Psychology,

51(12), 1771–1777. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000056

Chandler, J., Griffin, T. M., & Sorensen, N. (2008). In the “I” of the storm: Shared initials

increase disaster donations. Judgment and Decision Making, 3(5), 404–410.

Chu, D. K., Akl, E. A., Duda, S., Solo, K., Yaacoub, S., Schünemann, H. J., Chu, D. K., Akl, E.

A., El-harakeh, A., Bognanni, A., Lotfi, T., Loeb, M., Hajizadeh, A., Bak, A., Izcovich,

A., Cuello-Garcia, C. A., Chen, C., Harris, D. J., Borowiack, E., … Schünemann, H. J.

(2020). Physical distancing, face masks, and eye protection to prevent person-to-person

transmission of SARS-CoV-2 and COVID-19: A systematic review and meta-analysis.

The Lancet, 395(10242), 1973–1987. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)31142-9

Cocking, C., & Drury, J. (2008). The mass psychology of disasters and emergency evacuations:

A research report and implications for the Fire and Rescue Service.

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 29

Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-

processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1),

74–101. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74

Davis, M. H., & Franzoi, S. L. (1991). Stability and change in adolescent self-consciousness and

empathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 25(1), 70–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-

6566(91)90006-C

Dezecache, G. (2015). Human collective reactions to threat. WIREs Cognitive Science, 6(3),

209–219. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1344

Dezecache, G., Frith, C. D., & Deroy, O. (2020). Pandemics and the great evolutionary

mismatch. Current Biology, 30(10), R417–R419.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.04.010

Dezecache, G., Grèzes, J., & Dahl, C. D. (2017). The nature and distribution of affiliative

behaviour during exposure to mild threat. Royal Society Open Science, 4(8), 170265.

https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.170265

Dezecache, G., Martin, J.-R., Tessier, C., Safra, L., Pitron, V., Nuss, P., & Grèzes, J. (2020).

Nature and determinants of social actions during a mass shooting. PsyArXiv.

https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/dk5ah

Drury, J. (2018). The role of social identity processes in mass emergency behaviour: An

integrative review. European Review of Social Psychology, 29(1), 38–81.

https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2018.1471948

Drury, J., Brown, R., González, R., & Miranda, D. (2016). Emergent social identity and

observing social support predict social support provided by survivors in a disaster:

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 30

Solidarity in the 2010 Chile earthquake. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46(2),

209–223. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2146

Drury, J., Cocking, C., & Reicher, S. (2009). The Nature of Collective Resilience: Survivor

Reactions to the 2005 London Bombings. International Journal of Mass Emergencies

and Disasters, 27(1), 66–95.

Drury, J., & Williams, R. (2012). Children and young people who are refugees, internally

displaced persons or survivors or perpetrators of war, mass violence and terrorism.

Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 25(4), 277–284.

https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0b013e328353eea6

Duclos, R., & Barasch, A. (2014). Prosocial Behavior in Intergroup Relations: How Donor Self-

Construal and Recipient Group-Membership Shape Generosity. Journal of Consumer

Research, 41(1), 93–108. https://doi.org/10.1086/674976

Dunfield, K. A., & Kuhlmeier, V. A. (2013). Classifying prosocial behavior: Children’s

responses to instrumental need, emotional distress, and material desire. Child

Development, 84(5), 1766–1776. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12075

Eisenberg, N. (1986). Altruistic emotion, cognition, and behavior. Lawrence Erlbaum

Associates.

Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, Regulation, and Moral Development. Annual Review of

Psychology, 51(1), 665–697. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.665

Eisenberg, N., Eggum, N. D., & Di Giunta, L. (2010). Empathy-related Responding:

Associations with Prosocial Behavior, Aggression, and Intergroup Relations. Social

Issues and Policy Review, 4(1), 143–180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-

2409.2010.01020.x

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 31

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2006). Prosocial Development. In Handbook of

child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development, Vol. 3, 6th ed (pp.

646–718). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Cumberland, A., Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S. A., Zhou, Q., & Carlo,

G. (2002). Prosocial development in early adulthood: A longitudinal study. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 993–1006. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-

3514.82.6.993

Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related

behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101(1), 91–119. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-

2909.101.1.91

Everett, J. A. C., Faber, N. S., & Crockett, M. (2015). Preferences and beliefs in ingroup

favoritism. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9.

https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00015

Falk, A., & Graeber, T. (2020). Delayed negative effects of prosocial spending on happiness.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the of America,

117(12), 6463–6468. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1914324117

Fegert, J. M., Vitiello, B., Plener, P. L., & Clemens, V. (2020). Challenges and burden of the

Coronavirus 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic for child and adolescent mental health: A

narrative review to highlight clinical and research needs in the acute phase and the long

return to normality. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 14(1), 20.

https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-020-00329-3

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 32

Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of

facial expression by neonates. Science (New York, N.Y.), 218(4568), 179–181.

https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7123230

Filkowski, M., Cochran, R. N., & Haas, B. (2016). Altruistic behavior: Mapping responses in the

brain. Neuroscience and Neuroeconomics, Volume 5, 65–75.

https://doi.org/10.2147/NAN.S87718

Frankenhuis, W. E., & de Weerth, C. (2013). Does Early-Life Exposure to Stress Shape or

Impair Cognition? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(5), 407–412.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413484324

Fritz, C. (1961). Disaster. In R. Merton & R. Nisbet (Eds.), Contemporary Social Problems (pp.

651–694). Harcourt.

Garcia, D., & Rimé, B. (2019). Collective Emotions and Social Resilience in the Digital Traces

After a Terrorist Attack. Psychological Science, 30(4), 617–628.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619831964

Garcia, T., Massoni, S., & Villeval, M. C. (2020). Ambiguity and excuse-driven behavior in

charitable giving. European Economic Review, 124, 103412.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroecorev.2020.103412

Gergen, K., Gergen, M., & Meter, K. (1972). Individual Orientations To Prosocial Behavior.

Journal Of Social Issues, 28(3), 105–130. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-

4560.1972.tb00035.x

Gill, J. C., & Malamud, B. D. (2017). Anthropogenic processes, natural hazards, and interactions

in a multi-hazard framework. Earth-Science Reviews, 166, 246–269.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earscirev.2017.01.002

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 33

Goldmann, E., & Galea, S. (2014). Mental health consequences of disasters. Annual Review of

Public Health, 35, 169–183. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-publhealth-032013-182435

Gummerum, M., Takezawa, M., & Keller, M. (2009). The Influence of Social Category and

Reciprocity on Adults’ and Children’s Altruistic Behavior. Evolutionary Psychology,

7(2), 147470490900700. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470490900700212

Hamlin, J. K., & Wynn, K. (2011). Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others. Cognitive

Development, 26(1), 30–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.09.001

Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2008). Social Evaluation by Preverbal Infants. Pediatric

Research, 63(3), 219–219. https://doi.org/10.1203/PDR.0b013e318168c6e5

Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2010). Three-month-olds show a negativity bias in their

social evaluations. Developmental Science, 13(6), 923–929.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00951.x

Hartman, A. C., & Morse, B. S. (2020). Violence, Empathy and Altruism: Evidence from the

Ivorian Refugee Crisis in Liberia. British Journal of Political Science, 50(2), 731–755.

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123417000655

Heffner, J., Vives, M.-L., & FeldmanHall, O. (2021). Emotional responses to prosocial messages

increase willingness to self-isolate during the COVID-19 pandemic. Personality and

Individual Differences, 170, 110420. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110420

Hepach, R., & Warneken, F. (2018). Editorial overview: Early development of prosocial

behavior: Revealing the foundation of human prosociality. Current Opinion in

Psychology, 20, iv–viii. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.001

Hobfoll, S. E., Watson, P., Bell, C. C., Bryant, R. A., Brymer, M. J., Friedman, M. J., Friedman,

M., Gersons, B. P. R., de Jong, J. T. V. M., Layne, C. M., Maguen, S., Neria, Y.,

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 34

Norwood, A. E., Pynoos, R. S., Reissman, D., Ruzek, J. I., Shalev, A. Y., Solomon, Z.,

Steinberg, A. M., & Ursano, R. J. (2007). Five essential elements of immediate and mid-

term mass trauma intervention: Empirical evidence. Psychiatry, 70(4), 283–315;

discussion 316-369. https://doi.org/10.1521/psyc.2007.70.4.283

Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice

(pp. x, 331). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511805851

Hogg, M. A. (2001). Self-categorization and subjective uncertainty resolution: Cognitive and

motivational facets of social identity and group membership. In The social mind:

Cognitive and motivational aspects of interpersonal behavior (pp. 323–349). Cambridge

University Press.

Imuta, K., Henry, J. D., Slaughter, V., Selcuk, B., & Ruffman, T. (2016). Theory of mind and

prosocial behavior in childhood: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology,

52(8), 1192–1205. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000140

International Federation of Red Cross and Red Crescent Societies. (2006). What is VCA? An

introduction to vulnerability and capacity assessment.

https://www.ifrc.org/Global/Publications/disasters/vca/whats-vca-en.pdf

Johnson, N. R. (1987a). Panic at “The Who concert stampede”: An empirical assessment. Social

Problems, 34(4), 362–373. https://doi.org/10.1525/sp.1987.34.4.03a00040

Johnson, N. R. (1987b). Panic and the Breakdown of Social Order: Popular Myth, Social Theory,

Empirical Evidence. Sociological Focus, 20(3), 171–183.

https://doi.org/10.1080/00380237.1987.10570950

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 35

Jordan, J., Yoeli, E., & Rand, D. (2020). Don’t get it or don’t spread it? Comparing self-

interested versus prosocial motivations for COVID-19 prevention behaviors. PsyArXiv.

https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/yuq7x

Jordans, M. J. D., Komproe, I. H., Tol, W. A., Kohrt, B. A., Luitel, N. P., Macy, R. D., & de

Jong, J. T. V. M. (2010). Evaluation of a classroom-based psychosocial intervention in

conflict-affected : A cluster randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 51(7), 818–826.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02209.x

Kanacri, B. P. L., Pastorelli, C., Zuffianò, A., Eisenberg, N., Ceravolo, R., & Caprara, G. V.

(2014). Trajectories of prosocial behaviors conducive to civic outcomes during the

transition to adulthood: The predictive role of family dynamics. Journal of Adolescence,

37(8), 1529–1539. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2014.07.002

Kaniasty, K., & Norris, F. H. (2004). Social support in the aftermath of disasters, catastrophes,

and acts of terrorism: Altruistic, overwhelmed, uncertain, antagonistic, and patriotic

communities. In Bioterrorism: Psychological and public health interventions (pp. 200–

229). Cambridge University Press.

Kappes, A., Nussberger, A.-M., Faber, N. S., Kahane, G., Savulescu, J., & Crockett, M. J.

(2018). Uncertainty about the impact of social decisions increases prosocial behaviour.

Nature Human Behaviour, 2(8), 573–580. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0372-x

Kousky, C. (2016). Impacts of Natural Disasters on Children. The Future of Children, 26(1), 73–

92. https://doi.org/10.1353/foc.2016.0004

La Greca, A. M., Lai, B. S., Llabre, M. M., Silverman, W. K., Vernberg, E. M., & Prinstein, M.

J. (2013). Children’s Postdisaster Trajectories of PTS Symptoms: Predicting Chronic

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 36

Distress. Child & Youth Care Forum, 42(4), 351–369. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10566-

013-9206-1

Lengua, L. J., Long, A. C., Smith, K. I., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). Pre-attack symptomatology

and temperament as predictors of children’s responses to the September 11 terrorist

attacks. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 46(6), 631–

645. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00378.x

Lewis, K., Langley, A., & Jones, R. T. (2015). Impact of Coping Efficacy and Acculturation on

Psychopathology in Adolescents Following a Wildfire. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10826-

013-9838-7

Leytham, S., & Powell, T. (2012). Chronic pain and its association with type of natural disaster,

community support and social-cultural context. International Journal of Interdisciplinary

Social Sciences, 6(6), 233–242. https://doi.org/10.18848/1833-1882/CGP/v06i06/52101

Li, Y., Li, H., Decety, J., & Lee, K. (2013). Experiencing a natural disaster alters children’s

altruistic giving. Psychological Science, 24(9), 1686–1695.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613479975

Liddle, M.-J. E., Bradley, B. S., & Mcgrath, A. (2015). BABY EMPATHY: INFANT

DISTRESS AND PEER PROSOCIAL RESPONSES. Infant Mental Health Journal,

36(4), 446–458. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21519

Litvack-Miller, W., McDougall, D., & Romney, D. M. (1997). The structure of empathy during

middle childhood and its relationship to prosocial behavior. Genetic, Social, and General

Psychology Monographs, 123(3), 303-. Gale Academic OneFile.

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 37

Liu, J. J., Bao, Y., Huang, X., Shi, J., & Lu, L. (2020). Mental health considerations for children

quarantined because of COVID-19. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 4(5), 347–

349. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30096-1

Liu, Y.-C., Kuo, R.-L., & Shih, S.-R. (2020). COVID-19: The first documented coronavirus

pandemic in history. Biomedical Journal, 43(4), 328–333.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bj.2020.04.007

LoBue, V. (2009). More than just another face in the crowd: Superior detection of threatening

facial expressions in children and adults. Developmental Science, 12(2), 305–313.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00767.x

Lock, S., Rubin, G. J., Murray, V., Rogers, M. B., Amlôt, R., & Williams, R. (2012). Secondary

stressors and extreme events and disasters: A systematic review of primary research from

2010-2011. PLoS Currents, 4.

https://doi.org/10.1371/currents.dis.a9b76fed1b2dd5c5bfcfc13c87a2f24f

Luengo Kanacri, B. P., Pastorelli, C., Eisenberg, N., Zuffianò, A., & Caprara, G. V. (2013). The

development of prosociality from adolescence to early adulthood: The role of effortful

control. Journal of Personality, 81(3), 302–312. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12001

Macksoud, M. S., & Aber, J. L. (1996). The war experiences and psychosocial development of

children in Lebanon. Child Development, 67(1), 70–88.

Maki, A., Dwyer, P. C., Blazek, S., Snyder, M., González, R., & Lay, S. (2019). Responding to

natural disasters: Examining identity and prosociality in the context of a major

earthquake. British Journal of Social Psychology, 58(1), 66–87.

https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12281

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 38

Masten, A. S., & Narayan, A. J. (2012). Child development in the context of disaster, war, and

terrorism: Pathways of risk and resilience. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 227–257.

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100356

Masten, A. S., & Osofsky, J. D. (2010). Disasters and their impact on child development:

Introduction to the special section. Child Development, 81(4), 1029–1039.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01452.x

Mawson, A. R. (2005). Understanding mass panic and other collective responses to threat and

disaster. Psychiatry, 68(2), 95–113. https://doi.org/10.1521/psyc.2005.68.2.95

Mawson, A. R. (2017). Mass Panic and Social Attachment: The Dynamics of Human Behavior.

Routledge.

McDonald, B. A., & Stukenbrock, E. H. (2016). Rapid emergence of pathogens in agro-

ecosystems: Global threats to agricultural sustainability and food security. Philosophical

Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1709).

https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0026

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child

Development, 54(3), 702–709.

Mladjan, D., & Cvetković, V. (2013). Classification of emergency situations—Klasifikacija

vanrednih situacija (p. 291).

Morris-Suzuki, T. (2017). Disaster and Utopia: Looking Back at 3/11. Japanese Studies, 37(2),

171–190. https://doi.org/10.1080/10371397.2017.1350920

Muench, M. (2011). Classification of Emergency Scenarios. ArXiv:1108.2634 [Cs].

http://arxiv.org/abs/1108.2634

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 39

Muthukrishna, M., & Henrich, J. (2019). A problem in theory. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(3),

221–229. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0522-1

Nestor, M. S., Fischer, D., & Arnold, D. (2020). “Masking” our emotions: Botulinum toxin,

facial expression, and well‐being in the age of COVID‐19. Journal of Cosmetic

Dermatology. https://doi.org/10.1111/jocd.13569

Nguyen, C. V., & Pham, N. M. (2018). The impact of natural disasters on children’s education:

Comparative evidence from Ethiopia, , , and Vietnam. Review of Development

Economics, 22(4), 1561–1589. https://doi.org/10.1111/rode.12406

Norris, F. (2005). Range, Magnitude, and Duration of the Effects of Disasters on Mental Health:

Review Update 2005.

Oosterhoff, B., & Palmer, C. A. (2020). Attitudes and Psychological Factors Associated With

News Monitoring, Social Distancing, Disinfecting, and Hoarding Behaviors Among US

Adolescents During the Coronavirus Disease 2019 Pandemic. JAMA Pediatrics.

https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.1876

Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show increased helping following

priming with affiliation. Psychological Science, 20(10), 1189–1193.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02419.x

Padilla‐Walker, L. M., & Christensen, K. J. (2011). Empathy and Self-Regulation as Mediators

Between Parenting and Adolescents’ Prosocial Behavior Toward Strangers, Friends, and

Family. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21(3), 545–551.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00695.x

Páez, D., Basabe, N., Ubillos, S., & González‐Castro, J. L. (2007). Social Sharing, Participation

in Demonstrations, Emotional Climate, and Coping with Collective Violence After the

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 40

March 11th Madrid Bombings1. Journal of Social Issues, 63(2), 323–337.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2007.00511.x

Palinkas, L., Downs, M., Petterson, J., & Russell, J. (1993). Social, Cultural, and Psychological

Impacts of the Exxon Valdez Oil Spill. Human Organization, 52(1), 1–13.

https://doi.org/10.17730/humo.52.1.162688w475154m34

Pancer, S. M., Pratt, M., Hunsberger, B., & Alisat, S. (2007). Community and political

involvement in adolescence: What distinguishes the activists from the uninvolved?

Journal of Community Psychology, 35(6), 741–759. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.20176

Penner, L. A., & Finkelstein, M. A. (1998). Dispositional and structural determinants of

volunteerism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(2), 525–537.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.525

Perry, R. (2018). Defining Disaster: An Evolving Concept (p. 3). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-

319-63254-4_1

Pfattheicher, S., Nockur, L., Böhm, R., Sassenrath, C., & Petersen, M. B. (2020). The Emotional

Path to Action: Empathy Promotes Physical Distancing and Wearing of Face Masks

During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Psychological Science, 0956797620964422.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620964422

Pfefferbaum, B., Jacobs, A. K., Houston, J. B., & Griffin, N. (2015). Children’s disaster

reactions: The influence of family and social factors. Current Psychiatry Reports, 17(7),

57. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-015-0597-6

Pfefferbaum, B., Noffsinger, M. A., Wind, L. H., & Allen, J. R. (2014). Children’s coping in the

context of disasters and terrorism. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 19(1), 78–97.

https://doi.org/10.1080/15325024.2013.791797

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 41

Pfefferbaum, B., & North, C. S. (2020). Mental Health and the Covid-19 Pandemic. The New

England Journal of Medicine, 383(6), 510–512. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMp2008017

Philpot, R., Liebst, L. S., Levine, M., Bernasco, W., & Lindegaard, M. R. (2020). Would I be

helped? Cross-national CCTV footage shows that intervention is the norm in public

conflicts. American Psychologist, 75(1), 66–75. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000469

Powell, T., & Thompson, S. J. (2016). Enhancing Coping and Supporting Protective Factors

After a Disaster: Findings From a Quasi-Experimental Study. Research on Social Work

Practice, 26(5), 539–549. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049731514559422

Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. The

Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(1), 1–20; discussion 20-71.

https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x02000018

Pynoos, R., Steinberg, A., & Brymer, M. J. (2007). Children and disasters: Public mental health

approaches. Textbook of Disaster Psychiatry, 48–68.

Raboteg‐šaric, Z., Žužul, M., & Keresteš, G. (1994). War and children’s aggressive and prosocial

behaviour. European Journal of Personality, 8(3), 201–212.

https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2410080305

Rand, D. G., & Epstein, Z. G. (2014). Risking Your Life without a Second Thought: Intuitive

Decision-Making and Extreme Altruism. PLOS ONE, 9(10), e109687.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0109687

Rand, D. G., Greene, J. D., & Nowak, M. A. (2012). Spontaneous giving and calculated greed.

Nature, 489(7416), 427–430. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature11467

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 42

Raundalen, M., & Dyregrov, A. (1991). Helping the child. In C. P. Dodge & M. Raundalen

(Eds.), Reaching Children in War: Sudan, Uganda and Mozambique (pp. 89–91). Nordic

Africa Institute.

Repacholi, B. M., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14- and

18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 12–21. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-

1649.33.1.12

Rice, J. A., Levine, L. J., & Pizarro, D. A. (2007). “Just stop thinking about it”: Effects of

emotional disengagement on children’s memory for educational material. Emotion

(Washington, D.C.), 7(4), 812–823. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.812

Rubens, S. L., Felix, E. D., & Hambrick, E. P. (2018). A Meta-Analysis of the Impact of Natural

Disasters on Internalizing and Externalizing Problems in Youth. Journal of Traumatic

Stress, 31(3), 332–341. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.22292

Sanderson, R. C., Gross, S., Sanon, J. G., & Janairo, R. (2016). Building resilience in children

and their communities following disaster in a developing country: Responding to the

2010 earthquake in Haiti. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma, 9(1), 31–41.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s40653-015-0077-5

Shaluf, I. (2007). Disaster types. Disaster Prevention and Management, 16, 704–717.

https://doi.org/10.1108/09653560710837019

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Perry, D. (2009). Two systems for empathy: A

double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus

versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 132(Pt 3), 617–

627. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awn279

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 43

Shi, Q., Ettekal, I., Liew, J., & Woltering, S. (2020). Predicting differentiated developmental

trajectories of prosocial behavior: A 12-year longitudinal study of children facing early

risks and vulnerabilities. International Journal of Behavioral Development,

0165025420935630. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025420935630

Sibley, C. G., & Bulbulia, J. (2012). Faith after an Earthquake: A Longitudinal Study of Religion

and Perceived Health before and after the 2011 Christchurch Earthquake.

PLOS ONE, 7(12), e49648. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049648

Sierksma, J., Thijs, J., Verkuyten, M., & Komter, A. (2014). Children’s Reasoning About the

Refusal to Help: The Role of Need, Costs, and Social Perspective Taking. Child

Development, 85(3), 1134–1149. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12195

Sjoberg, G. (1962). Disasters and social change. Man and Society in Disaster, 356–384.

Strick, M., Holland, R., Baaren, R., & Knippenberg, A. (2009). Finding Comfort in a Joke:

Consolatory Effects of Humor Through Cognitive Distraction. Emotion (Washington,

D.C.), 9, 574–578. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015951

Sze, J. A., Gyurak, A., Goodkind, M. S., & Levenson, R. W. (2012). Greater Emotional Empathy

and Prosocial Behavior in Late Life. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 12(5), 1129–1140.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025011

Tol, W. A., Song, S., & Jordans, M. J. D. (2013). Annual Research Review: Resilience and

mental health in children and adolescents living in areas of armed conflict--a systematic

review of findings in low- and middle-income countries. Journal of Child Psychology

and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 54(4), 445–460.

https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12053

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 44

Tunçgenç, B., Zein, M. E., Sulik, J., Newson, M., Zhao, Y., Dezecache, G., & Deroy, O. (2021).

Social influence matters: We follow pandemic guidelines most when our close circle

does. British Journal of Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12491

Turkewitz, J., Healy, J., & Mazzei, P. (2019, May 8). Colorado School Shooting Victim Died

Trying to Stop the Gunman (Published 2019). .

https://www.nytimes.com/2019/05/08/us/colorado-shooting-victims.html

Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young Children Selectively Avoid Helping

People With Harmful Intentions. Child Development, 81(6), 1661–1669.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01500.x

Van der Graaff, J., Carlo, G., Crocetti, E., Koot, H. M., & Branje, S. (2018). Prosocial Behavior

in Adolescence: Gender Differences in Development and Links with Empathy. Journal of

Youth and Adolescence, 47(5), 1086–1099. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0786-1

Vardy, T., & Atkinson, Q. D. (2019). Property Damage and Exposure to Other People in Distress

Differentially Predict Prosocial Behavior After a Natural Disaster. Psychological Science,

30(4), 563–575. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619826972

Vezzali, L., Drury, J., Versari, A., & Cadamuro, A. (2016). Sharing distress increases helping

and contact intentions via social identification and inclusion of the other in the self:

Children’s prosocial behaviour after an earthquake. Group Processes & Intergroup

Relations, 19(3), 314–327. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430215590492

Vives, M.-L., & FeldmanHall, O. (2018). Tolerance to ambiguous uncertainty predicts prosocial

behavior. Nature Communications, 9(1), 2156. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-

04631-9

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 45

Wang, C.-W., Chan, C. L. W., & Ho, R. T. H. (2013). Prevalence and trajectory of

psychopathology among child and adolescent survivors of disasters: A systematic review

of epidemiological studies across 1987–2011. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric

Epidemiology, 48(11), 1697–1720. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-013-0731-x

Warneken, F., Lohse, K., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young Children Share the

Spoils After Collaboration. Psychological Science, 22(2), 267–273.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610395392

Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Helping and Cooperation at 14 Months of Age. Infancy,

11(3), 271–294. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00227.x

Wells, C. R., Huppert, A., Fitzpatrick, M. C., Pandey, A., Velan, B., Singer, B. H., Bauch, C. T.,

& Galvani, A. P. (2020). Prosocial polio vaccination in . Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences, 117(23), 13138–13144.

https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1922746117

West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007). Social semantics: Altruism, cooperation,

mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology,

20(2), 415–432. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01258.x

Wolf, L. J., Haddock, G., Manstead, A. S. R., & Maio, G. R. (2020). The importance of (shared)

human values for containing the COVID‐19 pandemic. The British Journal of Social

Psychology, 59(3), 618–627. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12401

Wolmer L, Hamiel D, Pardo-Aviv L, & Laor N. (2017). Addressing the Needs of Preschool

Children in the Context of Disasters and Terrorism: Assessment, Prevention, and

Intervention. Current Psychiatry Reports, 19(7), 40–40. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-

017-0792-8

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 46

Wray‐Lake, L., & Syvertsen, A. K. (2011). The developmental roots of social responsibility in

childhood and adolescence. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development,

2011(134), 11–25. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.308

Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of

concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 126–136.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.126

Zaki, J. (2020). Catastrophe Compassion: Understanding and Extending Prosociality Under

Crisis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(8), 587–589.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.05.006

Zeveloff, S. I., & Boyce, M. S. (1982). Why Human Neonates Are So Altricial. The American

Naturalist, 120(4), 537–542.

THE EMERGENCY OF PROSOCIALITY 47