<<

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 1

RUNNING HEAD: AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM

The Implicit Legacy of American Protestantism

Eric Luis Uhlmann†

HEC Paris

Jeffrey Sanchez-Burks†

University of Ross School of

† Author contribution was equal

WORD COUNT: 5,582

CONTACT: Eric Luis Uhlmann HEC Paris - School of Management Management and Human Resources Department 1, Rue de la Libération 78351 Jouy-en-Josas Tel: 33 (0)1 39 67 97 44 E-mail: [email protected]

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 2

Abstract

The heritage of a nation founded by devout Puritan-Protestants has had wide ranging effects on

U.S. culture and, as experimental evidence suggests, continues to exert an implicit influence on the feelings, judgments, and behaviors of contemporary . The is distinguished by a in individual and traditional values uncommon among economically developed , both of which have been traced, in part, to the moral ideals of the founding Protestant communities. Calvinist Protestantism has further profoundly shaped American workways, including the moralization of work and the manifestation of professional norms that prescribe impersonal and unemotional workplace interactions. The implicit influence of traditional Protestant beliefs extends not only to devout American

Protestants, but even non-Protestant and less religious Americans.

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 3

“It seems to me, that I can see the entire destiny of America contained in the first Puritan who came ashore.” -- Alexis de Tocqueville, in America (1840/1990)

Founder effects in evolutionary biology occur when a new population is established by a small group from a much larger population (Mayr, 1952, 1954). Such effects often take place when groups migrate over long distances and settle in a new location. These early representatives of a series have disproportionate effects on the characteristics of all subsequent generations. As a consequence, the characteristics of the new population can be distinct from the original parent population. Mayr (1954) used his theory to explain variations in the morphology (size, bill shape, and feathering) of birds of the same species that colonized isolated islands in small groups.

However, founder effects are also observed in human populations. For example, one of the

British settlers who colonized the island of Tristan da Cunha in 1814 carried a gene for retinitis pigmentosa. Even today, blindness due to this disease is ten times as common on Tristan da

Cunha as in Britain (Thompson, 1978).

Although usually thought of in the context of biology, founder effects are also relevant to our understanding of culture (Cohen, 2001; Oishi, 2010). In other words, the early members of a culture may lay the foundation for the traditions and values of all subsequent generations.

Because children and younger newcomers tend to assimilate, albiet imperfectly, to the traditions of the broader society, initial values are absorbed and carried forward (Cheung, Chudek, &

Heine, 2011; Lim, Yeh, Liang, Lau, & McCabe, 2009; Minoura, 1992; Tsai, Ying, & Lee, 2000).

Thus, we may observe a certain degree of cultural inertia such that the earliest communities of an emerging society exert a disproportionate influence on the culture’s fundamental nature.

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 4

The present empirical review shows that a heritage as a nation founded by Puritan-

Protestant settlers has had wide ranging effects on U.S. culture, and despite the worldwide trend toward modernization and urbanization appears to continue to exert an implicit influence on the feelings, judgments, and behaviors of contemporary Americans. In the United States, trends towards hedonism in popular culture co-exist with deep seated moral intuitions based on the traditional values of the founding communities. In making this case we draw on both international surveys and experimental laboratory research. As we describe, American values are distinguished by strikingly traditional morals relative to other contemporary economically developed democracies as well as a strong faith in individual merit, both of which are traceable in part to the founding Protestant communities. Calvinist Protestantism has also profoundly shaped American workways, including the moralization of work and professional norms that prescribe impersonal and relatively unemotional workplace interactions. Consistent with prior research and theory on implicit social cognition (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995), an unconscious influence of traditional Protestant beliefs can and does appear to occur even among individuals whom explicitly reject traditional values, and therefore extends even to Americans with no explicit religious beliefs or affiliations. Thus, the cultural legacy of America’s founding communities and the operation of basic social cognitive processes help explain the persistence of

Protestant influences in the contemporary United States.

America’s Protestant Heritage

The Protestant began in in the 16th century and cleaved the world of

Western in two, leading to political upheaval, religious wars, and cultural changes.

Protestant leaders such as and decried what they saw as the

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 5 corruptions and heresies of the , such as the sale of indulgencies and clerical offices, as well as and they felt only distracted from the pursuit of true faith. The

English Reformation resulted in the overthrow of Catholic authority and the establishment of the

Anglican Church. However, some felt the Anglican Church had not gone nearly far enough in its reforms. Calling themselves “The Godly,” they advocated for greater piety, spiritual purity, and adherence to the ideals of the . Their political opponents derisively labeled them “,” branded them religious fanatics, and retaliated against them by passing restricting their religious practices.

Fleeing such persecution and hoping to create a spiritually pure , many separatist

Puritans emigrated to America, establishing some of the earliest British colonies in the New

World. Alexis de Tocqueville (1840/1990) likened the influence of Puritanism on the culture of

America to a fire set on a high hill whose light “still tinge(s) the furthest reaches of the horizon”

(p. 31-32). Even at U.S. independence in 1776, over a century and a half after the founding of

Plymouth colony, three-quarters of Americans were Puritans (Gelertner, 2007; Morone, 2003).

Scholars of American culture have written that Puritan leaders were “as close to an intellectual ruling class as America has ever had” (Hofstadter, 1962, p. 59), and that “Before the Civil War,

Puritanism remained the country’s dominant spiritual influence” (Gelertner, 2007, p. 153).

Although few, if any, Americans explicitly view themselves as Puritans today, the

Puritan-Protestant tradition appears to have had an indelible influence on the values and ideals of the United States. In a cultural analog of the founder effects observed in evolutionary biology

(Cohen, 2001), the Puritan-Protestant settlers and their spiritual descendants set the tone of

American culture for centuries to come. Although further waves of immigration and myriad

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 6 other events and influences have likewise shaped U.S. culture, the legacy of the Puritan-

Protestant founding remains evident today.

Implicit Cultural Cognition

One psychological reason traditional cultural values can persist so stubbornly is the nature of implicit social cognition. Contemporary dual process models distinguish between explicit (i.e., deliberative, controlled, and logical) and implicit (i.e., spontaneous, intuitive, and automatic) attitudes and beliefs (Epstein, 1994; Fazio, 1990; Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006;

Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Wilson, Lindsey, & Schooler, 2000). Consistent with a dual process framework, striking dissociations are often observed between implicit and explicit measures of attitudes (Nosek, 2005; Rudman, 2004). Such dissociations can occur when individuals who explicitly reject cultural beliefs nonetheless internalize them at an implicit level. For example, even individuals who consciously reject cultural stereotypes can think and act in accordance with such stereotypes when they are implicitly activated (Bargh, 1999; Devine, 1989; Greenwald &

Banaji, 1995; Nisbett, Peng, Choi, & Norenzayan, 2001). In one study, for example, college students subliminally exposed to African-American faces were subsequently more likely to behave in a hostile manner, in line with cultural stereotypes of Americans as hostile and aggressive (Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996).

The influence of traditional Protestant religious beliefs on contemporary Americans has been shown to operate implicitly as well (Sanchez-Burks, 2002, 2005; Uhlmann, Poehlman, &

Bargh, 2009; Uhlmann, Poehlman, Tannenbaum, & Bargh, 2011). In certain instances, this influence takes the of automatic associations which the does not consciously endorse (e.g., an association between work and divine ; Uhlmann et al., 2011).

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 7

However, it more often takes the form of unrecognized influences on conscious judgments. Haidt

(2001, p. 818) describes “the sudden appearance in consciousness of a moral judgment, including an affective valence (good-bad, like-dislike), without any conscious awareness of having gone through steps of search, weighing evidence, or inferring a conclusion.” Such intuitive judgments are often driven by implicit cultural mores (Haidt, 2001; Haidt, Koller, & Dias, 1993; Graham et al., 2013; Shweder & Haidt, 1993; Shweder, Mahapatra, & Miller, 1987). Thus, Americans may seek to uphold the ethic of individual merit, support traditionalist positions on issues like the death penalty, and experience deep negative feelings toward individuals who violate the

Protestant work ethic, all without recognizing the ideological and cultural influences on their judgments. If so, then traditional Protestant beliefs should influence the judgments and behaviors of not only devout American Protestants, but also non-Protestant and less religious Americans.

America’s Distinctive Values

International surveys identify two key dimensions along which American values are distinctive relative to those of other nations. The first is religious traditionalism. Americans display higher levels of religiosity, moral absolutism, and endorsement of conservative values on issues such as the death penalty, homosexuality, and suicide, than members of most other economically developed democracies (Inglehart & Welzel, 2005; Norris & Inglehart, 2004). The second dimension is an individualistic ethos. U.S. culture is characterized by its commitment to individualistic values and strong faith in individual merit. Part of what makes this set of values so remarkable is that, as seen in Figure 1, individualism and traditional religiosity are negatively correlated across nations (Hofstede, 2001; Inglehart, 1997; Inglehart & Welzel, 2005). As we

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 8 will argue, both of these distinctive aspects of the American are traceable in part to the religious convictions of the founding communities of the United States.

Religious Traditionalism

As societies become progressively more affluent, traditional religious morality fades in favor of more secular-liberal values (Inglehart & Welzel, 2005). A prominent exception is the

United States, which has remained deeply religious despite high levels of economic development

(Norris & Inglehart, 2004). The explosive economic growth in the wake of the Protestant

Reformation led to widespread in most historically Protestant countries (Inglehart,

1997; Inglehart & Welzel, 2005; Norris & Inglehart, 2004). In contrast, the United States remains just as religious today as in the 1950s (Gallup & Linday, 1999; Greeley, 1991; Norris &

Inglehart, 2004). Nineteen out of twenty contemporary Americans believe in , seven in ten believe in the devil, approximately half believe that the world is less than 10,000 years old, and four out of ten attend church on a weekly basis (Baker, 2005; Dawkins, 2006; Harris, 2006;

Lipset, 1996; Sheler, 2006). Indeed, ten times as many Americans are extremely religious as one would expect based on the economic prosperity of the United States (i.e., based on the per capita gross domestic product of the U.S. relative to comparison countries; Wald, 1987).

Such high degrees of religiosity were originally due in part to a self-selection process in which especially religious Puritan Protestants—often persecuted for their extreme beliefs in their home countries—emigrated to the New World (Fukuyama, 1995; Lipset, 1996; Morone, 2003;

Norris & Inglehart, 2004). Although these religious settlers were eventually dwarfed in number by immigrants seeking economic opportunities, their early arrival provided them a founding influence on American religious traditions. Indeed, out of the nearly seventy nations covered by the , the U.S. and Brazil— an economically developing Catholic country

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 9 with an increasing Protestant population— were the only two nations where a in god and related religious convictions have not diminished significantly over the last quarter-century

(Norris & Inglehart, 2004).

Across both nations and individuals, high levels of religiosity are associated with traditional views on moral issues, such as divorce, euthanasia, , and homosexuality

(Baker, 2005; Norris & Inglehart, 2004). The sustained religiosity of the United States is evidenced in how Americans espouse values considerably more traditional than the world average and among the most traditional values of any economically developed Western democracy (Figure 2). Americans further exhibit the most traditional values of any historically

Protestant country (Norris & Inglehart, 2004). Moreover, unlike in every other Western country,

U.S. values have not become any more liberal since 1980 (Baker, 2005).

Religious faith leads not only to the endorsement of conservative positions on moral issues, but also an absolutist view of the very nature of morality (Norris & Inglehart, 2004).

Moral absolutism is measured in the World Values Surveys via endorsement of the item: “There are absolutely clear guidelines about what is good and evil. These always apply to everyone, whatever the circumstances.” Departing dramatically from the moral relativism of other wealthy democracies, more than half of Americans endorse the absolutist position (Baker, 2005). The percentage of Americans who are moral absolutists is more comparable to that in (60%) than (19%).

As noted earlier, of course a great deal has changed since the time of the Puritan settlers, many of whom, for example, banned Christmas celebrations as overly decadent (West, 2003). A sizeable number of contemporary Americans explicitly reject traditional values and embrace hedonistic pleasure and moral relativism (Baker, 2005). College educated Americans are

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 10 particularly likely to endorse moral views that depart from the mores of their broader culture

(Haidt et al., 1993). Consistent with dual process models, however, such individuals continue to experience intuitive feelings that reflect traditional Puritan-Protestant values. Uhlmann et al.

(2011, Experiment 1) primed American and British college students with either deliberative, intuitive, or neutral concepts using a sentence-unscrambling task (Srull & Wyer, 1979). All participants then reported the degree of respect they felt toward a young woman who had numerous sex partners. American participants condemned promiscuous sexuality while in an intuitive or neutral mindset, but reported permissive sexual attitudes while in a deliberate mindset. Reflecting the secular-liberal values of their culture, British participants expressed permissive sexual attitudes regardless of what mindset they were in. Thus, educated Americans appear to exhibit a degree of “hedonic ambivalence” or tension between their implicit and explicit moral beliefs regarding sexuality. The phenomenon of hedonic ambivalence suggests that the culture wars in America may not be something that only occurs between different ideological factions in society. It can also take place within individual , as liberal, nontraditional explicit beliefs clash with more conservative, traditionalist gut feelings.

An Individualistic Ethos

More specific aspects of Protestant values contribute to a second important way in which

American values are distinctive. In contrast to the collectivistic orientation common across much of the world, Western countries are characterized by a more individualistic ethos (Bellah,

Madsen, Sullivan, Swidler, & Tipton, 1985; Inkeles, 1983; Markus & Kitayama, 1991).

However, even among Westerners, Americans often stand out for their strong individualism

(Hofstede, 1980, 2001; Trompenaars & Hampden-Turner, 1997). American individualism is reflected not only in responses to questions on international surveys, but also on more behavioral

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 11 and indirect measures (Heine, 2008; Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010). Suggesting an implicit component to such beliefs, individualistic values can be automatically activated and influence judgments and behaviors without the person’s awareness (Brewer & Gardner, 1996;

Oyserman & Lee, 2008).

American individualism, too, appears to stem in part from a Protestant religious heritage.

Catholic tradition emphasizes a community of believers whose relationship with God is mediated by the collective authority of the church hierarchy. In response to the perceived corruptions of the Catholic establishment, the Protestant reformers sought a more individual relationship with

God (McNeill, 1954). Martin Luther, for instance, wrote that each individual is “a perfectly free lord, subject to none” (as quoted in Sampson, 2000, p. 1427). Cross-national comparisons indicate that even today, historically Protestant countries are characterized by higher levels of individualism than non-Protestant countries (Hampden-Turner & Trompenaars, 1993; Hofstede,

1980, 2001; Trompenaars & Hampden-Turner, 1997).

Protestant values also contribute to the strong belief in individual merit in the United

States. Most major , such as Catholicism, promise rewards for pious behavior in the afterlife. In contrast, early Protestant held that material success in this life is evidence of God’s grace (McNeill, 1954; Weber, 1904/1958). The Protestant notion of a moral marked by material wealth is an important contributor to the American belief in meritocracy

(Baker, 2005; Schuck & Wilson, 2008).

Americans widely believe that their country is a land of opportunity where hard work is rewarded with an ascent up the economic ladder (Baker, 2005; Schuck & Wilson, 2008;

Trompenaars & Hampden-Turner, 1997). Ninety-six percent of Americans believe children should be taught that “with hard work... anyone can succeed in America” (Baker, 2005). More

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 12 than half of Americans believe economic competition is desirable because it motivates people to work hard, compared to only one third of Spaniards and the French (Schuck & Wilson, 2008).

Seventy-seven percent of American managers favor terminating an incompetent longtime employee, but less than half of British managers and less than a third of German, Italian, French, and Korean managers feel the same way (Hampden-Turner & Trompenaars, 1993; Trompenaars

& Hampden-Turner, 1997). Two-thirds of French, and Italians believe whether a person succeeds in life is mainly due to “forces outside their own control,” but only one in three

Americans agrees this is true (Schuck & Wilson, 2008). Demonstrating that Protestant values make a causal contribution to the belief in meritocracy, experimentally activating the (e.g., by completing scale items assessing Protestant work values or unscrambling sentences related to hard work) leads participants to make negative attributions about members of low status groups (Biernat, Vescio, & Theno, 1996; Katz & Hass, 1988; McCoy & Major,

2007; Quinn & Crocker, 1999). Ironically, despite this strong American belief in meritocracy, the U.S. is characterized by less economic mobility than most other Western countries (Burtless

& Haskins, 2008; DeParle, 2012).

Of course, any phenomenon as complex and multifaceted as American individualism is multiply determined. Cultures that developed under frontier conditions, where a lack of formal legal authority makes self-reliance a basic necessity, are characterized by an individualistic ethos

(Kitayama, Ishii, Imada, Takemura, & Ramaswamy, 2006; Vandello & Cohen, 1999). For example, Japanese from the northern island of Hokkaido, which was settled under frontier conditions, express values almost as individualistic as those of Americans (Kitayama et al.,

2006). At the same time, Americans from regions that once formed the nation’s Western frontier are more likely to endorse individualistic values (Vandello & Cohen, 1999). In addition, societies

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 13 marked by high levels of immigration and residential mobility, such as the United States, are more individualistic (Oishi & Kisling, 2009). Thus, a Protestant heritage is only one of a number of factors that have shaped the individualistic ethos of the United States.

In sum, American values are distinguished by a simultaneous endorsement of individualistic values and commitment to traditional religious morality. Although this national profile is undoubtedly the product of numerous events and influences from the last four centuries of American history, both cross-national comparisons and experimental evidence suggest

Protestant values are one important contributor. As reviewed in the next section, another legacy of American Protestantism is the distinctive nature of U.S. workways.

Implicit American Workways

Workways describe “a culture’s signature pattern of workplace beliefs, mental models, and practices that embody a society’s ideas about what is true, good and efficient within the domain of work” (Sanchez-Burks & Lee, 2007, p. 346). Research on the cultural psychology of workways identifies unique ideologies that shape cultural understandings of how people should think, feel, and act with regard to their work (Sanchez-Burks & Lee, 2007). These include the

Korean tradition of chaebol or ‘company familism’ (Kim, 1988), the Chinese networking system, guanxi (Solomon, 1999), and the cultural script of simpatia for interpersonal work relations in (Triandis, Marin, Lisansky, & Betancourt, 1984). Importantly, these prior literatures identify cultural workways that are all profoundly relational in nature, emphasizing the importance of personal connections, attention to social and emotional information, and the interpersonal context in which work occurs.

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 14

Much prior scholarship has addressed ways in which a Protestant heritage has shaped

Americans’ norms and values related to work (Fischer, 1989; Fukuyama, 1995; Lipset, 1996;

Tocqueville, 1840/1990; Trompenaars & Hampden-Turner, 1997; Weber, 1904/1958). However, only recently have experimental studies provided causal evidence of an effect of Protestant religious ideas on American workways. As will be described in greater depth below, in contrast to members of other cultures, Americans value working beyond material reason and even implicitly associate work with divine salvation (Uhlmann et al., 2009, 2011). At the same time,

Americans implicitly conform to norms of workplace professionalism that prescribe unemotional and politely impersonal workplace interactions (Sanchez-Burks, 2002, 2005).

The Moralization of Work

A key aspect of the Protestant ethic is that work is moralized for non-instrumental reasons— individuals are expected to labor even in the absence of any material need to do so

(McNeill, 1954; Weber, 1904/1958). Although many cultures worldwide value work, they often do so for pragmatic or group-centered reasons (Inglehart, 1997; Inglehart & Welzel, 2005). For instance, hard work is a requirement of basic survival in some economically underdeveloped countries, making productive labor a familial obligation. Also, in the Korean workway of chaebol the entire company is characterized as an extended family, and high work productivity is a matter of ingroup loyalty (Kim, 1988). Protestantism, in contrast, places work firmly in the domain of deontological morality— values pursued for their own sake, without regard for material consequences for the self and ingroups. Consistent with these traditional beliefs, contemporary Americans labor for more hours per year than members of most other economically developed countries (Friedman, 2008), a pattern of behavior observed even in the

1800s by Alexis de Tocqueville (1840/1990). This cultural commitment to hard work shows no

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 15 sign of fading: today’s Americans actually work a full 20 days a year more than a quarter century ago (Linstedt, 2002; Wessel, 2003).

Experimental evidence further suggests Americans implicitly attribute a religious significance to work. One of the most distinctive aspects of the Protestant faith is the link drawn in Calvinist theology between hard work and divine salvation (McNeill, 1954; Tocqueville,

1840/1990; Weber, 1904/1958). Calvin and other influential Protestant thinkers characterized work as a spiritual calling, and even wrote of the “ of work” (Gelertner, 2007, p. 61). To the extent a psychological link between hard work and salvation survives anywhere in the historically Protestant , it should do so in the still highly religious United States.

Uhlmann et al. (2011, Experiment 2) tested this hypothesis using an implicit prime-to-behavior paradigm (Bargh et al., 1996). American, Canadian, German, Argentinian, and Italian participants completed a scrambled sentences puzzle (Srull & Wyer, 1979), which either contained words related to divine salvation (e.g., almighty, redeem) or equally positive but nonreligious words. As expected, American participants— and only American participants— responded to the implicit salvation prime by working harder on a subsequent task. This

“salvation prime” effect indicates that Protestant work values have not become fully secular in

American culture, and continue to be linked to religious ideas.

Because Americans associate sex with religious piety (a link shared with many other cultures), and associate work and divine salvation (a link based in traditional Puritan-Protestant values), they may link sexual values and work values with each other. An implicit association between work and sex would be expected to occur based on basic principles of cognitive balance, which hold that if A is linked to B, and C is linked to B as well, then A and C should in turn be linked (Festinger, 1957; Greenwald et al., 2002; Heider, 1958). Cultures that lack one or

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 16 more of these links (between hard work and salvation, for instance) should not exhibit an implicit link between work and sex morality. Supporting this reasoning, bicultural Asian-Americans responded to an implicit work prime by expressing more conservative attitudes toward sexuality, but only when their American identity was first made salient. When their Asian identity was made salient, they exhibited no implicit link between work and sex morality (Uhlmann et al.,

2011, Experiment 3).

As predicted based on prior research and theory on implicit social cognition (Bargh,

1999; Devine, 1989; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995) not only devout American Protestants, but even non-Protestant and non-religious Americans exhibit such implicit Puritanism (Uhlmann et al.,

2009, 2011). Cultural mores appear to seep into implicit cognitions, affecting all members of a culture to at least some degree.

Workplace Relational Ideology

In addition to placing a high value on work, U.S. culture has distinctive ideals and norms regarding the appropriate approach to carrying out one’s work. Early Protestant thinkers such as

John Calvin believed that emotions and personal relationships distract from the pursuit of work as a moral calling (Bendix, 1977; Daniels, 1995; McNeill, 1954; Weber, 1904/1958). In line with this Protestant Relational Ideology (Sanchez-Burks, 2005), American norms of workplace professionalism prescribe the importance of attending to the task rather than the social emotional dimension of work situations. Consistent with a cultural workways approach, however, such norms only apply to workplace interactions. In social contexts, Americans appear to be just as attentive to social emotional cues as are members of other cultures.

Experimental evidence provides strong support for the existence of Protestant Relational

Ideology. Although work values that reflect Calvinist theology seem to characterize Americans

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 17 in general, this influence may be strongest for individuals with an especially high degree of exposure to Calvinist Protestantism specifically. American Presbyterians and Methodists

(members of two denominations within modern Protestantism with historical ties to ;

Fischer, 1989; McNeill, 1954) are less likely than non-Protestant Americans to automatically attend to emotion conveyed in spoken words and to engage in non-conscious behavioral mimicry while working, providing evidence of a reduced focus on relational cues in such contexts. But in social contexts, Calvinist Protestants are just as attuned to emotional information and eager to create rapport as are non-Protestant Americans (Sanchez-Burks, 2002).

Although most readily observed among individuals with very high degrees of exposure to

Calvinist Protestantism (e.g., Presbyterians and Methodists) American culture as a whole bears the imprint of Protestant Relational Ideology (Sanchez-Burks, 2005). American norms of workplace professionalism frown on making reference to one’s life outside of work in workplace settings, for example by prominently displaying family pictures in one’s office space. The

Chinese, in contrast, believe that displaying pictures of one’s family at work brings Quan Jia Fu or “Whole Family Fortune.” In an empirical investigation of the effects of U.S. culture on non- work role referencing, a multicultural sample of managers was asked to create an image of the office space of a “very professional” or “unprofessional” employee (Uhlmann, Heaphy, Ashford,

Zhu, & Sanchez-Burks, in press; Experiment 1). Managers with a greater degree of exposure to

U.S. culture were more likely to construct “unprofessional” office spaces that included non-work artifacts such as family pictures and other personal effects.

Failure to conform to such culturally prescribed norms of workplace professionalism can have severe repercussions. American, but not Indian participants discriminated against a job candidate who indicated he would discuss his family in order to build rapport with a potential

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 18 client (Uhlmann et al., in press, Experiment 2). Cultural differences were greatest among participants who had prior recruiting experience, and thus were more knowledgeable about relevant professional norms. This experiment provides direct evidence that Protestant Relational

Ideology resides not only in the heads of individuals but is manifested in institutional practices that, in turn, work to reinforce and perpetuate this cultural ideology. The findings reveal that those individuals who most closely conform to the cultural — that is, maintaining the implicit divide between the personal and the professional, work and non-work— are more likely to be accepted into positions of influence in society. Such an individual-organizational process is one reason the United States continues to fit Tonnies (1887/1957) description of a society where people separate gemeinschaft und gesellschaft.

The clear line drawn in the American mind between work and non-work— what

Hampden-Turner and Trompenaars (1993, p. 133) called “a split between the machine and the suburban garden”— reflects itself not only in workspace artifacts, hiring decisions, and recollections of work meetings, but also in who Americans chose to form emotionally meaningful relationships with. Individuals from the U.S. are significantly less likely than members of other cultures to form close personal friendships with colleagues from work

(Kacperczyk, Baker, & Sanchez-Burks, 2013; Mor Barak, Findler, & Wind, 2003; Morris,

Podolny, & Ariel, 2000).

Again, however, this impersonal orientation toward others only applies to the workplace

(Daniels, 1995; Weber, 1904/1958). Outside of work Americans appear just as sensitive to the social context as are members of other cultures. In a relevant study, American and East Asian managers read the following passage: “Overall the evaluation indicates your strengths are in communication skills, anticipating events, and creativity. The other areas are not as strong as

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 19 these—some are poor, but it’s difficult to evaluate those areas. Good job!”. Participants were either told the passage came from a supervisor’s performance evaluation of an employee (work context condition) or a friend telling another friend the results of the latter’s personality test

(social context condition). Americans in the social condition and East Asians regardless of condition perceived the feedback as negative but couched in face saving terms. Only Americans in the work condition interpreted the feedback literally and assumed the supervisor had a positive opinion of the employee (Sanchez-Burks, Lee, Choi, Nisbett, Zhao, & Jasook, 2003).

Further evidence suggests a paradox in Americans’ approach to collaborations. Since

Tocqueville (1840/1990), Americans have been perceived as being highly social when it comes to forming groups and joining teams with strangers, provided there is a common task interest such as generating funds for Tsunami relief or forming a startup venture. Whereas it is common across many cultures to rely on preexisting groups for charity efforts (e.g., an existing church or state agency) or business ventures (e.g., one’s guanxi network), Americans appear far more willing to collaborate with people that share nothing in common but a mutual task interest.

On the one hand, Americans appear highly relational by virtue of this penchant for working in groups. On the other hand, Americans have been shown to be relatively non- relational in their approach to working relationships. How is it possible to rely so heavily on collaborative efforts when relationships are given such little weight? One series of cross-cultural experiments provides some clues. An important hurdle to an effective collaboration is to successfully navigate interpersonal conflict that appears from time to time in most teams.

Although extensive research clearly establishes that such conflicts hamper team performance

(including that of American teams), it appears Americans are comparatively unlikely to believe such conflict really matters. In one study (Sanchez-Burks, Neuman, Ybarra, Kopelman, Goh, &

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 20

Park, 2008), Americans and were assessed on their lay beliefs (i.e., folk wisdom) about the consequences of task-focused and interpersonal conflict on team performance. Americans found it as intuitive as for East Asians that task-focused conflict hampers team performance.

However, in the U.S. there is far less conviction that relationship conflict can have a negative effect. Further studies showed that this remarkable optimism drives decisions about whether to begin certain collaborations. Americans appear particularly willing to join a workgroup that is highly likely to experience interpersonal friction so long as team members bring specific expertise to the group. These studies documented how PRI can not only produce a distinctive folk wisdom but one that is empirically inaccurate in light of robust empirical evidence.

Nonetheless, this optimism does provide an advantage. Anticipated interpersonal friction will not always materialize and whereas other cultural groups may forego a collaboration based on such a prediction, Americans would benefit from taking the chance. Thus, there appears to be an upside to this facet of American beliefs about work.

In sum, a Protestant-Puritan heritage appears to have profoundly shaped the implicit workways of contemporary America. Consistent with traditional Calvinist teachings, Americans implicitly view work as a moral imperative, even associating it with divine salvation. At the same time, American norms of workplace professionalism frown on expressing emotions and sharing personal information as unwelcome distractions from the pursuit of one’s calling.

Although the implicit influence of Calvinist Protestantism is in some cases strongest among those who explicitly endorse such teachings (e.g., Sanchez-Burks, 2002), it also demonstrably affects many non-Protestant and non-religious Americans. research should more systematically assess variability within the U.S. population in the implicit influence of

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 21

Protestantism, focusing on potential religious (Cohen & Rozin, 2001; Sanchez-Burks, 2002) and regional (Fischer, 1989; Rentfrow et al., in press) variation.

Conclusion

Traveling through the United States in the 1800s, French political thinker and historian

Alexis de Tocqueville marveled at the many aspects of U.S. culture that struck him as exceptional, among these Americans’ religious devotion, individualism, and dedication to hard work. It is a testament to both his genius and the enduring nature of culture that so many of these observations still hold true today. Indeed, de Tocqueville’s insights are supported by a growing body of empirical evidence demonstrating significant differences between the values of

Americans and those of other nations, including other Western countries.

Moreover, as argued by de Tocqueville, these seemingly disparate aspects of American exceptionalism are all traceable in part to the founding Puritan-Protestant communities of the

United States. A self-selection process in which especially devout Protestants emigrated to the

New World helps explain the religiosity, traditional values, and moral absolutism of contemporary Americans. American individualism and faith in individual merit stem in part from

Protestantism’s emphasis on an individual relationship with God and earthly rewards for pious behavior. Finally, Protestant work values, as articulated by John Calvin, underlie the American view of work as a moral calling and norms prescribing impersonal and unemotional workplace interactions.

The influence of traditional Protestant values on the judgments and behaviors of contemporary Americans is often implicit in nature. Americans exhibit intuitive gut feelings toward sexually promiscuous individuals that are more negative than their deliberatively

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 22 endorsed views (Uhlmann et al., 2011), implicitly associate work with divine salvation

(Uhlmann et al., 2011), and are less likely to automatically attend to social and emotional information in work than in non-work settings (Sanchez-Burks, 2005; Sanchez-Burks et al.,

2003). Reflecting their basis in broader cultural mores, implicit cognitions consistent with traditional Protestant values are observed not only among devout American Protestants, but also non-Protestant and less religious Americans.

Researchers who study culture have long been frustrated by the “ultimate origins” dilemma of how to identify the founding influences that have shaped a culture’s fundamental nature. Societies founded comparatively recently in provide a unique opportunity to begin to identify such influences with greater certainty than might otherwise be possible.

Much like the bills and feathering of the birds of an isolated island, the culture of the United

States continues to bear the imprint of its founding members.

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 23

References

Baker, W. (2005). America’s crisis of values. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University press.

Bargh, J. A. (1999). The cognitive monster: The case against controllability of automatic

stereotype effects. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social

psychology. New : Guilford.

Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of

trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 71, 230-244.

Bellah, R.N., Madsen, R., Sullivan, W.M., Swidler, A., & Tipton, S.M. (1985). Habits of

the heart: Individualism and commitment in American life. Berkeley, CA: University of

California press.

Bendix, R. (1977). : An intellectual portrait. Berkeley, CA: University of

Press.

Biernat, M., Vescio, T.K., & Theno, S.A. (1996). Violating American values: a “value

congruence” approach to understanding outgroup attitudes. Journal of Experimental

Social Psychology, 32, 387–410.

Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. L. (1996). Who is this "we"? Levels of collective identity and self

representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 83-93.

Burtless G., & Haskins, R. (2008). Inequality, economic mobility, and . In Peter H.

Schuck & James Q. Wilson (Eds). Understanding America: The anatomy of an

exceptional nation. (pp. 495-538). : Public Affairs.

Cheung, B. Y., Chudek, M., & Heine, S. J. (2011). Evidence for a sensitive window for

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 24

acculturation. Younger immigrants report acculturating at a faster rate. Psychological

Science, 22, 147-152.

Cohen, D. (2001). Cultural variation: Considerations and implications. Psychological Bulletin,

127, 451–471.

Cohen, A. B., & Rozin, P. (2001). and the morality of mentality. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 81, 697-710.

Daniels, B. C. (1995). Puritans at play. New York: St. Martin's Griffin.

Dawkins, R. (2006). The God delusion. , MA: Houghton Mifflin Co.

DeParle, J. (2012). Harder for Americans to rise from lower rungs. www.nytimes.com. Retrieved

at: http://www.nytimes.com/2012/01/05/us/harder-for-americans-to-rise-from-lower-

rungs.html?_r=1&hp

Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5-18.

Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American

Psychologist, 4, 709–724.

Fazio, R.H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE

model as an integrative framework. In M.P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental

Social Psychology (Vol 23, pp. 75-109). , CA: Academic Press.

Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Fischer, D. (1989). Albion's seed: Four British folkways in America. New York: Oxford

University Press.

Friedman, B.M. (2008). The economic system. In Peter H. Schuck & James Q. Wilson (Eds).

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 25

Understanding America: The anatomy of an exceptional nation. (pp. 87-120). New York:

Public Affairs.

Fukuyama, F. (1995). Trust: The social virtues and the creation of prosperity. New York, NY:

Simon & Schuster.

Gallup, G., & Linday, D.M. (1999). Surveying the religious landscape: Trends in U.S. beliefs.

Harrisburg, PA: Morehouse publishing.

Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G.V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in

evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological

Bulletin, 132, 692-731.

Gelertner, D. (2007). : The fourth great western religion. New York, NY:

Doubleday.

Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2013). Moral Foundations

Theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 55-

130.

Greeley, A.M. (1991). American exceptionalism: The religious phenomenon. In B.E.

Scafer (Ed.) Is America different? A new look at American exceptionalism. (pp. 94-115).

New York, NY: Oxford University press.

Greenwald, A.G., & Banaji, M.R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and

stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4-27.

Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., & Mellot, D. S.

(2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, beliefs, self-esteem and self-concept.

Psychological Review, 109, 3-25.

Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 26

judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814-834.

Haidt, J., Koller, S., & Dias, M. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your

dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 613-628.

Hampden-Turner, C., & Trompenaars, F. (1993). The seven cultures of : Value

systems for creating wealth in the United States, Britain, Japan, , France,

Sweden, and the . New York, NY: Doubleday.

Harris, S. (2006, November 13). The case against faith. Newsweek, p. 42-43.

Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.

Heine, S. J. (2008). Cultural psychology. W. W. Norton.

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world?

Behavioral and Brain , 33, 61-83.

Hofstadter, R. (1962). Anti-intellectualism in American life. New York, NY: Knopf.

Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values.

Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.

Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and

organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Inglehart, R. (1997). Modernization and postmodernization: Cultural, economic, and political

change in 43 societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University press.

Inglehart, R., Basáñez M., Díez-Medrano, J., Halman, L. & Luijkx, R. (2004). Human beliefs

and values: A cross-cultural sourcebook based on the 1999-2002 values surveys. Buenos

Aires, : Siglo Veintiuno Editores.

Inglehart, R., & Welzel, C. (2005). Modernization, cultural change, and democracy: The

human development . Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University press.

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 27

Inkeles, A. (1983). The American character. Center Magazine, November/December,

pp. 25-39.

Kacperczyk, A., Sanchez-Burks, J., & Baker, W. (2013). Social isolation at work? A longitudinal

and cross-cultural analysis of coworker multiplexity and their social benefits.

Unpublished Ross School of Business Working Paper No. 4535-341.

Katz, I. & Hass, R.G. (1988). Racial ambivalence and American value conflict: correlational and

priming studies of dual cognitive structures. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 55, 893-905.

Kim, S. U. (1988). The role of social values and competitiveness in economic growth: With

special reference to Korea. In D. Sinha & H. S. R. Kao (Eds.), Social values and

development: Asian perspectives (pp. 76-92). New Delhi: Sage.

Kitayama, S., Ishii, K., Imada, T., Takemura, K., & Ramaswamy, J. (2006). Voluntary settlement

and the spirit of independence: Evidence from Japan's “northern frontier”. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 369-384.

Lim, S.-L., Yeh, M., Liang, J., Lau, A. S., & McCabe, K. (2009). Acculturation gap,

intergenerational conflict, parenting , and youth distress in immigrant Chinese

American families. & Family Review, 45, 84-106.

Linstedt, S. (2002). Wonder where your time goes? You probably spent it at work. Buffalo

News, Buffalo, N.Y., October 14, p. B7.

Lipset, S.M. (1996). American exceptionalism: A double edged sword. New York, NY: W.W.

Norton & Co.

Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion,

and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253.

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 28

Mayr, E. (1942). Systematics and the origin of species. New York, NY:

Press.

Mayr, E. (1954). Change of genetic environment and evolution. In J. Huxley, A. C. Hardy, and

E. B. Ford (Eds.) Evolution as a process. (pp. 157–180). London: Allen & Unwin.

McCoy, S.K., & Major, B. (2007). Priming meritocracy and the psychological of

inequality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 341-351.

McNeill, J. T. (1954). The history and character of Calvinism. New York, NY: Oxford

University Press.

Minoura, Y. (1992). A sensitive period for the incorporation of a cultural meaning system: A

study of Japanese children growing up in the United States. Ethos, 20, 304-339.

Mor Barak, M. E., Findler, L., & Wind, L. H. (2003). Cross-cultural aspects of diversity and

well-being in the workplace: An international perspective. Journal of Social Work

Research and Evaluation, 4, 49-73.

Morone, J.A. (2003). Hellfire nation: The of sin in American history. New Haven, CT:

Yale University Press.

Morris, M. W., Podolny, J. M., & Ariel, S. (2000). Missing relations: Incorporating relational

constructs into models of culture. In C. P. Earley and H. Singh (Eds.), Innovations in

International and Cross-Cultural Management. (52-89). Thousand Oaks, CA, Sage.

Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought:

Holistic vs. analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291-310.

Norris, P., & Inglehart, R. (2004). Sacred and secular: Religion and politics worldwide. New

York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Nosek, B. A. (2005). Moderators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluation.

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 29

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 565-584.

Oishi, S. (2010). The psychology of residential mobility: Implications for the self, social

relationships, and well-being. Perspectives on Psychological , 5, 5–21.

Oishi, S., & Kisling, J. (2009). The mutual constitution of residential mobility and individualism.

In R. S. Wyer, Jr., C-Y. Chiu, Y. Y. Hong, and S. Shavitt (Eds.), Understanding Culture:

Theory, Research, and Application (p.223-238). Psychology Press: New York.

Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects

of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311-342.

Quinn, D.M., & J. Crocker, J. (1999). When ideology hurts: effects of belief in the Protestant

ethic and feeling overweight on the psychological well-being of women. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 402–414.

Rentfrow, P. J., Gosling, S. D., Jokela, M., Stillwell, D. J., Kosinski, M., & Potter, J. (in press).

Divided we stand: Three psychological regions of the United States and their

political, economic, social, and health correlates. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology.

Rudman, L. A. (2004). Sources of implicit attitudes. Current Directions in Psychological

Science, 13, 79-82.

Sampson, E.E. (2000). Reinterpreting individualism and collectivism: Their religious roots and

monologic versus dialogic person-other relationship. American Psychologist, 55, 1425-

1432.

Sanchez-Burks, J. (2002). Protestant Relational Ideology and (in) attention to relational cues in

work settings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 919-929.

Sanchez-Burks, J. (2005). Protestant Relational Ideology: The cognitive underpinnings and

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 30

organizational implications of an American anomaly. Research in Organizational

Behavior, 26, 265-305.

Sanchez-Burks, J. & Lee, F. (2007). Culture and workways. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.).

Handbook of Cultural Psychology (Vol 1, pp. 346-369). New York: Guilford.

Sanchez-Burks, J., Lee, F., Choi, I., Nisbett, R., Zhao, S. & Jasook, K. (2003). Conversing across

cultures: East-West communication styles in work and non-work contexts. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 363-372.

Sanchez-Burks, J., Neuman, E., Ybarra, O., Kopelman, S., Goh, K., & Park, H. (2008). Cultural

folk wisdom about relationship conflict. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research

1, 55-78.

Schuck, P.H., & Wilson, J.Q. (2008). Looking back. In Peter H. Schuck & James Q. Wilson

(Eds). Understanding America: The anatomy of an exceptional nation. (pp. 627-643).

New York: Public Affairs.

Sheler, J.S. (2006). The lure of the prophetic world. In P.W. Bernstein, & A. Swan (Eds.)

U.S. News & World Report special edition: Mysteries of faith: The (pp. 4-5).

Washington, DC: U.S. News & World Report.

Shweder, R., & Haidt, J. (1993). The future of moral psychology: Truth, intuition, and the

pluralist way. Psychological Science, 4, 360-365.

Shweder, R. A., Mahapatra, M., & Miller, J. (1987). Culture and moral development. In J. Kagan

& S. Lamb (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young children (pp. 1-83). :

University of Chicago Press.

Solomon, R. H. (1999). Chinese negotiating behavior: Pursuing interests through 'old friends'.

Washington, D. C.: United States Institute of Peace Press.

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 31

Srull, T.K., & Wyer, R.S. (1979). The role of category accessibility in the interpretation of

information about persons: Some determinants and implications. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 37, 1660–1672.

The World Factbook (2011). Washington, DC: Central Intelligence Agency.

Thompson, E.A. (1978). Ancestral inference. II. The founders of Tristan da Cunha. Annals of

Human Genetics, 42, 239-253.

Tocqueville, A.D. (1840/1990). Democracy in America. New York: Vintage Books.

Tonnies, F. (1887/1957). Community and society. Somerset, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

Triandis, H. C., Marín, G., Lisansky, J., & Betancourt, H. (1984). Simpatía as a cultural script of

. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(6), 1363-1375.

Trompenaars, F., & Hampden-Turner, C. (1997). Riding the waves of culture: Understanding

diversity in global business. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Tsai, J. L., Ying, Y., & Lee, P. A. (2000). The meaning of “being Chinese” and “being

American”: Variation among Chinese American young adults. Journal of Cross-Cultural

Psychology, 31, 302-332.

Uhlmann, E.L., Heaphy, E., Ashford, S.J., Zhu, L., & Sanchez-Burks, J. (in press). How

culturally bounded norms regarding non-work role referencing shape perceived

professionalism and hiring evaluations. Journal of Organizational Behavior.

Uhlmann, E.L., Poehlman, T.A., & Bargh, J.A. (2009). American moral exceptionalism. In

J.T. Jost, A.C. Kay, & H. Thorisdottir (Eds.) Social and Psychological Bases of Ideology

and System Justification. (pp. 27-52). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Uhlmann, E.L., Poehlman, T.A., Tannenbaum, D., & Bargh, J.A. (2011). Implicit Puritanism in

American moral cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 312-320.

Vandello, J. A., & Cohen, D. (1999). Patterns of individualism and collectivism across

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 32

the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 279-292.

Wald, K.D. (1987). Religion and politics in the United States. New York, NY: St. Martin's

press.

Weber, M. (1904/1958). The Protestant ethic and the spirit of capitalism. New York, NY:

Charles Scribner’s Sons.

Wessel, H. (2003). A 40-hour workweek just a to many. Chronicle, November

16, Houston, .

West, J. (2003). Drinking with Calvin and Luther! A history of alcohol in the church. Lincoln,

CA: Oakdown Books.

Wilson, T.D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T.Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological

Review, 107, 101-126.

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 33

Figure 1. Individualism and religiosity across countries. Nation-level individualism scores are taken from Hofstede’s (2001) individualism index. Religiosity scores are taken from the World

Values Survey (Inglehart, Basáñez, Díez-Medrano, Halman, & Luijkx, 2004).

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 34

Figure 2. Endorsement of traditional moral values and economic development (per-capita GDP) across countries. Traditionalism scores are taken from the World Values Survey (Inglehart et al.,

2004) and reflect opposition to homosexuality, abortion, euthanasia, and suicide, as well as support for the death penalty and other conservative social positions. Per-capita GDP figures are from the World Factbook (2011). Western countries are indicated with a + and non-Western countries with an X.

AMERICAN PROTESTANTISM 35

JCCP Information Concerning Authors

Additional authors may be noted on a separate page.

Address for Reprints Name: Jeffrey Sanchez-Burks Address: Stephen M. Ross School of Business, Department of Management & Organizations University of Michigan 701 Tappan Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1234

Author #1 Name: Eric Luis Uhlmann Address: HEC Paris - School of Management Management and Human Resources Department 1, Rue de la Libération 78350 Jouy-en-Josas, France

Affiliation (University, Institute, etc.): HEC Paris Telephone: 33 6 29 89 16 04 Fax: N/A E-mail: [email protected]

Author #2 Name: Jeffrey Sanchez-Burks Address: Stephen M. Ross School of Business, Department of Management & Organizations University of Michigan 701 Tappan Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1234

Affiliation (University, Institute, etc.): University of Michigan Telephone: (734) 615- 6351 Fax: N/A E-mail: [email protected]