The Development of Mind

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Development of Mind The Development of Mind Selected Works of Aleksei Nikolaevich Leontyev with a preface by Mike Cole Marxists Internet Archive P.O. Box 1541; Pacifica, CA 94044; USA. Aleksei Nikolaevich Leontyev (1904-1979) 1. Psychology, 2. Activity Theory, 3. Marxism ISBN 978-0-9805428-6-8 CC-SA (Creative Commons Attribution-Share Alike 3.0) 2009 by Marxists Internet Archive. Cover design by Joan Levinson. Set by Andy Blunden in Garamond. Printed by Bookmasters, Inc., Ohio. Distributed exclusively by Erythrós Press and Media. Table of Contents Note on the Work of A. N. Leonteyv vi Preface by Mike Cole................................................................................. vii 1. The Problem of the Origin of Sensation ............................................. 1 I. The Problem............................................................................. 1 II. Hypothesis ...........................................................................19 III. The Functional Evolution of Sensitivity.........................44 IV. Discussion of the Results and Certain Conclusions ...103 2. The Biological and Social in Man’s Psyche ..................................... 115 3. An Outline of the Evolution of the Psyche .................................... 137 I. The Evolution of the Psyche in Animals.........................137 II. The Origin of Human Consciousness............................181 III. The Historical Development of Consciousness..........198 4. The Historical Approach to Study of the Human Psyche ............ 245 5. The Development of Higher Forms of Memory ........................... 295 6. The Psychological Principles of Preschool Play............................. 331 7. The Theory of the Development of the Child’s Psyche ............... 355 8. Child Development and the Problem of Mental Deficiency........ 379 9. Activity and Consciousness .............................................................. 395 Note on the work of A N Leontyev The selected papers of A. N. Leontyev published in this edition express the main line of his theoretical and experimental research. After his first work, devoted to an experimental study of affective responses – An Investigation into the Objective Symptoms of Affec- tive Responses (jointly with A. R. Luriya) in Sovremennye problemy psik- hologii (Moscow, 1926); Experience of the Structural Analysis of Associative Chain Series, Russko-Nemetsky meditsinsky zhurnal, 1928, 1 and 2; Ekzamen i psikhika (Examination and the Mind) (jointly with A. R. Luriya Moscow, 1929, he began to work under L. S. Vygotsky within the context of the latter’s conception of research into the on- togenetic development of the psyche – Mediated Remembering in Children with a Deficient or Morbidly Altered Intellect, Voprosy defek- tologii, 1928, 4; Development of the Internal Structure of Higher Be- haviour in Psikhonevrologicheskaya nauka (Leningrad, 1930); Razvitie proizvol’nogo vnimania u detei (The Development of Voluntary Attention in Children), Moscow, 1930. In this period he also published his first major monograph Razvitie pamyati (The Development of Memory), Moscow, 1931. From 1932 Prof. Leontyev’s work took a new path. Heading a group of young psychologists in Kharkov (V. I. Asnin, L. I. Bozhovich, P. J. Halperin, A. V. Zaporozhets, P. I. Zinchenko, O. M. Kontsevaya, G. D. Lukov, V. V. Mistyuk, K. E. Khomenko, and oth- ers), he directed research into the development of the child’s practical intellectual activity and consciousness. On this basis he and his asso- ciates worked on the problem of relating the structure of activity to forms of psychic reflection. Several of the theoretical problems that arose in this connection, prompted Prof. Leontyev to start research in the field of various psychophysiological and zoopsychic problems. At the same time at the suggestion of the Kharkov Polygraphic Institute, he organised and directed a cycle of work of a primarily practical na- ture on children’s perception of illustrations. A number of papers were prepared under his direction in this period and published in Nauchnye zapiski Khar’kovskogo pedagogicheskogo instituta (Vol. I, X, 1939; Vol. II, X, 1941), Nauchnye zapiski Khar’kovskogo instituta inostrannykh yazyko v (Vol. II, X, 1939), Trudy konferentsii po psikhologii, Vol. I (Kiev, 1941), and in a number of later works, some of which appear in the present volume. After resuming work in Moscow in 1935, Prof. Leontyev devoted his main attention to the problem of the origin of sensitivity and the general theory of the evolution of the psyche, completing his experi- mental genetic research into the origin of sensation in 1940. During World War II Leontyev devoted his efforts to the urgent problem of restoring motor functions damaged by gunshot wounds. For that purpose he organised a rehabilitation hospital, of which he became scientific head. (The results of this work were published in A. N. Leontyev and A. V. Zaporozhets. Vosstanovlenie dvizheniya (The Restoration of Movement), Moscow, 1945, and in a number of spe- cial papers by Leontyev and his colleagues in Uchonye zapiski MGU, 1947, 3. Apart from its practical value this experimental research into the restoration of motor processes also played an important role in elabo- ration of the theory of functional development, enabling the author later to advance a hypothesis of the systemic structure of psychic functions (1954). In the postwar years the author again returned to the problem of child and educational psychology. At the same time he worked on several questions of general psychology. The joint session of the USSR Academy of Sciences and RSFSR Academy of Pedagogical Sci- ences held in 1951 on Pavlov’s physiological teaching, turned his at- tention to study of the psyche’s reflex mechanisms, which found reflection in his work: in the following period – On the Materialist and Subjective Idealist Reflex Conceptions of the Psyche, Sovetskaya pedagogika, 1951, 10; The Dependence of Associative Connections on the Content of Action (jointly with T. V. Rozanova), Sovetskaya peda- gogika, 1951, 10; On the Systemic Nature of Mental Functions. In: Tezisy doklado v na yubileinoi sessii Moskovskogo Universiteta (Moscow, 1955); On an Effect of the Forming of a Motor Chain Habit (jointly with M. I. Bobneva). Doklady APN RSFSR, 1958, 1; and the work cited below on analysis of the systemic structure of aural perception. His work on educational psychology, and problems of general psy- chology are not included in the present volume. * * * The Progress Publishers edition, published in the USSR in 1981 has been reproduced with the addition of “Activity and Conscious- ness,” published by Progress Publishers in 1977, with corrections and some changes to section numbering not affecting the content, by the Marxists Internet Archive, with a preface by Mike Cole. References to Marx and Engels have been amended so as to ref- erence the corresponding passage in Marx Engels Collected Works (MECW) published by Lawrence & Wishart (London) and Interna- tional Publishers (New York) 1975-2005. Other references have been left as per the Progress Publishers edition. Preface Alexei Leontyev must be counted among the most influential So- viet psychologists of the 20th century. As a student/colleague of Lev Vygotsky, he helped to form what he referred to as a “socio- historical” approach to psychology which famously features the cen- trality of the category of human activity as its central concept. As he put it in a widely read article on this topic, Human psychology is concerned with the activity of con- crete individuals, which takes place either in a collective – i.e., jointly with other people – or in a situation in which the subject deals directly with the surrounding world of objects – e.g., at the potter’s wheel or the writer’s desk. ... With all its varied forms, the human individual’s activity is a sys- tem in the system of social relations. It does not exist without these relations. The specific form in which it exists is determined by the forms and means of material and mental social interaction that are created by the development of production and that can not be real- ised in any way other than in the activity of concrete people. It turns out that the activity of separate individuals depends on their place in society, on the conditions that fall to their lot, and on idiosyncratic, individual factors. The work contained in this volume emphasises both the central- ity of human activity as cause and consequence of specifically human higher psychological processes, but also emblemises a central theo- retical tenet of the entire line of psychology derived from Vygotsky’s ideas, captured in the aphorism that “to understand behaviour, one must know the history of behaviour.” Leontyev, more than any of Vygotsky’s colleagues, pursued this idea back into the primal origins of the organization of living matter while continuing to pursue its implications for the development of human society and human chil- dren as well. For further reading see: Engeström, Y. (1987). Learning by Expanding. Helsinki; Ko- sultat Oy. Leontyev, A.N. (1979). The problem of activity in psy- chology (pp. 37-71). In J.V. Wertsch (Ed.). The concept of ac- tivity in Soviet psychology. Armonk, NY: Sharpe. Mike Cole February 2009 1. The Problem of the Origin of Sensation* I. The Problem 1 The origin, i.e. the genesis proper, of the psyche, and its subse- quent evolution, are closely related problems. Our general approach to psychic development is, therefore, directly characterised
Recommended publications
  • Activity Theory and the Transformation of Pedagogic Practice
    Educational Studies in Japan: International Yearbook No.1, December, 2006, pp.77-90 Activity Theory and the Transformation of Pedagogic Practice YAMAZUMI, Katsuhiro* Today, work and other societal practices are experiencing accelerating paradigm shifts from mass-production-based systems toward new systems based on networking between organizations, collaboration, and partnerships. This shift requires new paradigms in the fields of education, learning, and develop- ment. As human activity quickly changes to networking and partnering among diverse cultural organizations, we need to ask ourselves whether schools and other actors are equipped to prepare people for such practices. We also need to think about what kind of learning can generate critical and creative agency among learners. Such agency will help people shape their own lives and future. In this paper, I discuss the potential offered by cultural-historical activity the- ory for analyzing and redesigning new, expanded pedagogic practices in schools. Putting the third generation of activity theory to pedagogic practice, I propose that new forms of expansive learning that are transforming pedagog- ic activity structures in schools can occur in advanced networks of learning that transcend institutional boundaries of schools, turning them into societal agents of change. To concretize the notion of expansive learning as one new form of pedagogy in which boundary-crossing networks of learning and the changing agent role of schools emerge, I will illustrate and analyze a children’s after-school activity project called New School: a multi-activity collaboration in which a university, an elementary school, families, and expert groups and community organizations outside the school cooperate to create advanced learning networks.
    [Show full text]
  • Modeling Social Learning: an Agent-Based Approach
    Old Dominion University ODU Digital Commons Computational Modeling & Simulation Computational Modeling & Simulation Engineering Theses & Dissertations Engineering Fall 2019 Modeling Social Learning: An Agent-Based Approach Erika G. Ardiles Cruz Old Dominion University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.odu.edu/msve_etds Part of the Computer Engineering Commons, and the Social Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Ardiles Cruz, Erika G.. "Modeling Social Learning: An Agent-Based Approach" (2019). Doctor of Philosophy (PhD), Dissertation, Computational Modeling & Simulation Engineering, Old Dominion University, DOI: 10.25776/ppbs-8751 https://digitalcommons.odu.edu/msve_etds/53 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Computational Modeling & Simulation Engineering at ODU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Computational Modeling & Simulation Engineering Theses & Dissertations by an authorized administrator of ODU Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. MODELING SOCIAL LEARNING: AN AGENT-BASED APPROACH by Erika G. Ardiles Cruz B.S. December 1998, Universidad San Antonio Abad A Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of Old Dominion University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY MODELING AND SIMULATION OLD DOMINION UNIVERSITY December 2019 Approved by: John Sokolowski (Director) Roland Mielke (Member) Bryan Paine (Member) ABSTRACT MODELING SOCIAL LEARNING: AN AGENT-BASED APPROACH Erika G. Ardiles Cruz Old Dominion University, 2019 Director: Dr. John Sokolowski Learning is the process of acquiring or modifying knowledge, behavior, or skills. The ability to learn is inherent to humans, animals, and plants, and even machines are provided with algorithms that could mimic in a restricted way the processes of learning.
    [Show full text]
  • The Presence and Significance of Imaginative Play in the Lives of Mexican-American Adults
    The Presence and Significance of Imaginative Play in the Lives of Mexican-American Adults BY ANTHONY T. PERONE III A.B., Cornell University, 1994 M.Ed., University of Illinois at Chicago, 2005 THESIS Submitted as partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Educational Psychology in the Graduate College of the University of Illinois at Chicago, 2013 Chicago, Illinois Committee: Artin Göncü, Chair and Advisor Pamela Quiroz, Educational Policy Studies Stacey Horn William Schubert, Curriculum and Instruction Suzanne Gaskins, Northeastern University This dissertation is dedicated to communities who create and share playful, meaningful experiences. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This dissertation could not have been done without the support of so many people. First, I would like to thank those whose support of my research has brought me to this point. My advisor, chair, and mentor, Artin Göncü, has been instrumental and steadfast in supporting my initial interests in a cultural approach to imaginative play and education and in developing those elementary ideas into robust and engaging scholarship. It has been an honor to work with him over these years and I could not have imagined a more dedicated, nurturing, and compassionate advisor. I also thank my committee members Suzanne Gaskins, Stacey Horn, Pamela Quiroz, and William Schubert for their interest in my work, thoughtful comments, offers to help, and suggestions for growth that have made me and this dissertation better. In addition to my committee, other scholars have also been inspirational to my approaches to teaching, learning, and research. I am fortunate to have learned from Bill Ayers and his consistent reminder to learn the ―meaning for actors in situations,‖ guides the way I learn from the participants in my research.
    [Show full text]
  • Situated Cognition
    Roth, W.-M., & Jornet, A. G. (2013). Situated cognition. WIREs Cognitive Science, 4, xxx–xxx. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1242 Article type: Overview Article title: Situated cognition First author: Full name and affiliation; plus email address if corresponding author Wolff-Michael Roth*, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC, V8W 3N4, Canada [email protected] Second author: Full name and affiliation; plus email address if corresponding author Alfredo Jornet, InterMedia, University of Oslo, 0318 Oslo, Norway Abstract Following the cognitive revolution, when knowing and learning have come to be theorized in terms of representations stored and processed in the mind, empirical and theoretical developments in very different scholarly disciplines have led to the emergence of the situated cognition hypothesis, which consists of a set of interlocking theses: cognition is embodied, fundamentally social, distributed, enacted, and often works without representations. We trace the historical origins of this hypothesis and discuss the evidential support this hypothesis receives from empirical and modeling studies. We distinguish the question of where cognition is located from the question of what cognition is, because the confounding of the two questions leads to misunderstandings in the sometimes-ardent debates concerning the situated cognition hypothesis. We conclude with recommendations for interdisciplinary approaches to the nature of cognition. At the end of the 1980s, just about at the time when the cognitive revolution—based on information processing and the mind as computer metaphor—had become the dominant approach to cognition and learning, a new way of theorizing human performance emerged: situated cognition. There are claims that situated cognition view has grown rapidly over the past decade, including in the areas of cognitive and social neuroscience and (cognitive, social, and developmental) psychology.1 Such claims are substantiated by citation counts.
    [Show full text]
  • Vygotskian and Post-Vygotskian Views on Children's Play •
    Vygotskian and Post-Vygotskian Views on Children’s Play • E B D J. L e authors argue that childhood played a special role in the culturalhistorical theory of human culture and biosocial development made famous by Soviet psy- chologist Lev S. Vygotsky and his circle. ey discuss how this school of thought has, in turn, inuenced contemporary play studies. Vygotsky used early childhood to test and rene his basic principles. He considered the make-believe play of pre- schoolers and kindergartners the means by which they overcame the impulsiveness of toddlers to develop the intentional behavior essential to higher mental functions. e authors explore the theory of play developed by Vygotsky’s colleague Daniel Elkonin based on these basic principlies, as well as the implications for play in the work of such Vygotskians as Alexei Leontiv, Alexander Luria, and others, and how their work has been extended by more recent research. e authors also discuss the role of play in creating the Vygotsky school’s “zone of proximal development.” Like these researchers, old and new, the authors point to the need to teach young children how to play, but they caution teachers to allow play to remain a childhood activity instead of making it a lesson plan. Key words : childhood devlopment; culturalhistorical psychology; Lev S. Vygotsky; preschool play; zone of proximal development A -, - from Russian psychiatrist Lev S. Vygotsky states: “In play a child is always above his average age, above his daily behavior; in play it is as though he were a head taller than himself. As in the focus of a magnifying glass, play contains all developmental tendencies in a condensed form; in play it is as though the child were trying to jump above the level of his normal behavior” (1967, 16).
    [Show full text]
  • 6. Activity Theory, History and Philosophy of Science, and Ict Technologies in Science Teaching Applications
    EFTHYMIS STAMOULIS & KATERINA PLAKITSI 6. ACTIVITY THEORY, HISTORY AND PHILOSOPHY OF SCIENCE, AND ICT TECHNOLOGIES IN SCIENCE TEACHING APPLICATIONS INTRODUCTION This paper is part of a study which focused on the restructuring of scientific literacy (Roth & Lee, 2004), in the sense that it proposes a new methodological tool for designing the teaching of scientific concepts to young children. This new approach comes under the umbrella of activity theory. However, if activity theory is employed mainly in the analysis of activities (Engeström, 1987), in the present study it is employed in an attempt to design the activities of students. In the present study, we apply the expansive cycle which Engeström (1999b) suggests and which he accepts as the acceptable practice of an action, and step by step we are led – through the resolution of contradictions – to the application of new practices. These new practices help us to design activities in the teaching of Science to pupils of primary education. In implementing these we use elements derived from the History of Science and teaching practices which are suggested in the literature and are relevant to their introduction in the teaching of science. All activities, implemented by the pupils with the help of ICT, do not take place in the traditional environment of a class. Much research on this subject has been undertaken internationally. It shows that the incorporation of elements from the History of Science and the use of ICT in the teaching of science to young pupils stimulate the interest of the children and enhance the understanding of the concepts of science.
    [Show full text]
  • Cultural-Historical Activity Theory As Practical Theory
    1 Cultural-Historical Activity Theory as Practical Theory: Illuminating the Development of a Conflict Monitoring Network Published in Communication Theory, Vol. 11, No. 1, February 2001, pp. 56-83 Kirsten A. Foot University of Washington [email protected] 2 Abstract As the number and intensity of conflicts increased around the world during the latter part of the twentieth century, scholars, policymakers, and practitioners of nonviolent conflict management strategies created conflict monitoring networks to track the escalation of tensions in conflict-prone regions. This essay demonstrates how cultural-historical activity theory (CHAT), as developed from the work of Vygotsky (1978) and Leont’ev (1978; 1981) by Engestrom (1987; 1990) and others, was employed in the service of a conflict monitoring network on the territory of the former Soviet Union. Based upon historical and participant observation research on the development of the Network for Ethnological Monitoring and Early Warning between 1990-1999, a CHAT-based analysis of the Network’s systemic contradictions illuminates its development through one expansive cycle and into a second. Summaries of findings are presented on relations within the Network, the evolution of the Network’s complex object, and the Network’s development of tools for monitoring ethnic relations and building an epistemic community. The essay concludes with an analysis of the correspondence between the CHAT framework and the five features of practical theory laid out by Cronen (1995). Key words: activity theory, network analysis, discourse analysis, organizational studies. Introduction As the number and intensity of conflicts increased around the world during the latter part of the twentieth century, scholars, policymakers, and practitioners of nonviolent conflict management strategies created conflict monitoring networks to track the escalation of tensions in conflict-prone regions.
    [Show full text]
  • “Vygotsky S Neglected Legacy”: Cultural-Historical Activity Theory
    Manuscript accepted for publication in Review of Educational Research “Vygotskys Neglected Legacy”: Cultural-Historical Activity Theory Wolff-Michael Roth1, Yew-Jin Lee2 1University of Victoria, 2National Institute of Education, Singapore All correspondence concerning this paper should be addressed to Wolff-Michael Roth, Lansdowne Professor, Applied Cognitive Science, MacLaurin Building A548, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC, Canada V8W 3N4. E-mail: [email protected]. Tel: 1-250-721-7885 FAX: 1-250-721-7767 Running Head: Cultural-historical activity theory Cultural-historical activity theory 2 “Vygotskys Neglected Legacy”: Cultural-Historical Activity Theory ABSTRACT: We describe an evolving theoretical framework that has been called one of the best-kept secrets of academia: cultural-historical activity theory (CHAT) is the result of proposals Lev Vygotsky first articulated, but which his students and followers substantially developed to constitute much expanded forms in its second and third generation. Besides showing that activity theory transforms how research should proceed regarding language, language learning, and literacy in particular, we demonstrate how it is a theory for praxis, thereby offering the potential to overcome some of the most profound problems that has plagued both educational theorizing and practice. Cultural-historical activity theory 3 Introduction More than seven decades ago, the Russian psychologist Lev S. Vygotsky noted that (educational) psychology was in a state of crisis because of the “atomistic and functional modes of analysis . [that] treated psychic processes in isolation” (Vygotsky, 1986, p. 1). Specifically, he pointed out that the separation of intellect and affect “as subjects of study [was] a major weakness of traditional psychology, since it [made] the thought process appear as an autonomous flow of ‘thoughts thinking themselves,’ segregated from the fullness of life, from the personal need and interests, the inclinations and impulses of the thinker” (p.
    [Show full text]
  • Nilsson, Ferholt & Lecusay, the Playing Exploring Child
    CIE0010.1177/1463949117710800Contemporary Issues in Early ChildhoodNilsson et al. 710800research-article2017 Article Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood 1 –15 ‘The playing-exploring child’: © The Author(s) 2017 Reprints and permissions: Reconceptualizing the relationship sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav https://doi.org/10.1177/1463949117710800DOI: 10.1177/1463949117710800 between play and learning in early journals.sagepub.com/home/cie childhood education Monica Nilsson Jönköping University, Sweden Beth Ferholt Brooklyn College, City University of New York, USA Robert Lecusay Jönköping University, Sweden Abstract In this article, the authors problematize the dichotomization of play and learning that often shapes the agenda of early childhood education research and practice. This dichotomization is driven in part by the tendency to define learning in terms of formal learning (i.e. learning as an outcome of direct instruction and school-based approaches that focus on teacher-led, goal-directed activities and declarative knowledge). The authors argue for a reconceptualization of early childhood education that understands learning and development not as an outcome, primarily, of instruction and teaching, but as an outcome of play and exploration. They develop this argument by drawing on Vygotsky’s theories of play, imagination, realistic thinking and creativity. These theories challenge another dichotomy – that between imagination and reality – by arguing that imagination is implicated in the meaning-making of both play and exploration. Instead of relating play to learning where play is characterized by imagination and learning by reality, the authors’ reconceptualization relates play to exploration and proposes that learning, defined as leading to human development, is an outcome of both of these activities.
    [Show full text]
  • Cultural-Historical Activity Theory in the Framework of the “Functional Paradigm” Dmitry A
    Культурно-историческая психология Cultural-Historical Psychology 2020. Т. 16. № 2. С. 19—24 2020. Vol. 16, no. 2, pp. 19—24 DOI: https://doi.org/10.17759/chp.2020160203 DOI: https://doi.org/10.17759/chp.2020160203 ISSN: 1816-5435 (печатный) ISSN: 1816-5435 (print) ISSN: 2224-8935 (online) ISSN: 2224-8935 (online) Cultural-Historical Activity Theory in the Framework of the “Functional Paradigm” Dmitry A. Leontiev National Research University, Higher School of Economics, Moscow, Russia, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2252-9805, e-mail: [email protected] The aim of the paper is to highlight the peculiarity of the present state of the Cultural-historical activity theory (CHAT) in psychology, as compared to previous periods of its development in the general context of theory development in psychology. The author sees this peculiarity in that CHAT exemplifies an anti-Aristote- lian paradigm which suggests that the actual functioning of living systems cannot be fully deduced from a priori existing morphological and psychological structures. This emphasis unites CHAT with two other influential approaches to the explanation of human conduct, existentialism and the systems approach to autoregulating systems, which can be found from early cybernetics to synergetics and the theory of complexity of our own day. Although they each occupied marginal positions in human sciences in the middle of the last century, all three approaches now find themselves articulating the same message in different words; basic similarities between them allow us to speak of their confluence into what may be called the functional paradigm. The functional paradigm states the primacy of the process, actual functioning, activity, or existence, the absoluteness of un- certainty and changeability, and thus seems to be the most relevant paradigm for the challenges of our times.
    [Show full text]
  • Activity Theory in a Nutshell
    3 Activity Theory in a Nutshell 3.1 INTRODUCTION The inspiration for this chapter comes from Aleksey Leontiev’s Activity, Consciousness, and Personality (1978), the most authoritative exposition of activity theory. This ‘‘small theoretical book,’’ as Leontiev himself described it, is not an introduction to activity theory, but a collection of essays, each focusing on a limited set of fundamental theoretical con- cepts. Leontiev specifically emphasized that many issues were mentioned only in passing and not clearly articulated in the book. Currently there is no standard systematic, entry-level introduction to activity theory. There is a clear need for such an introduction, especially in interdisciplinary fields such as interaction design where not everyone may have encoun- tered original works by Leontiev, Vygotsky, and other key contributors. This chapter is intended as a primer in activity theory. It introduces the reader to key ideas, concepts, and principles of activity theory. The chap- ter is different from most other short introductions to activity theory (e.g., Wertsch 1981; Davydov 1990a; Bødker 1991; Kuutti 1992; Nardi 1992, 1996a, 1998; Blackler 1995; Kaptelinin, Kuutti, and Bannon 1995; Kaptelinin 1996a; Kaptelinin and Nardi 1997; Verenikina and Gould 1998; Bertelsen and Bødker 2003). These works typically summa- rize the basic ideas of the theory, while giving the historical develop- ment of the ideas much less attention. Such summaries appear to be the only feasible approach, given the space limitations of a journal article or a conference paper. We ourselves have used this approach on more than one occasion. However, according to our experience, this way of intro- ducing activity theory is not always effective.
    [Show full text]
  • Activity Theory and Human-Computer Interaction
    1 Activity Theory and Human-Computer Interaction Bonnie A. Nardi What is activity theory, and how will it benefit studies of human-computer interaction? This book addresses these questions. Many HCI researchers are eager to move beyond the confines of traditional cognitive science, but it is not clear exactly which direction to move in. This book explores one alternative for HCI research: activity theory, a research framework and set of perspectives originating in Soviet psychology in the 1920s. Just as HCI research is concerned with practical problems of design and evaluation, activity theorists from the outset have addressed practical needs, applying their research efforts to the problems of mentally and physically handicapped children, educational testing, ergonomics, and other areas. Following the lead of dialectical materialism, activity theory focuses on practice, which obviates the need to distinguish ``applied'' from ``pure'' science—understanding everyday practice in the real world is the very objective of scientific practice. Activity theory is a powerful and clarifying descriptive tool rather than a strongly predictive theory. The object of activity theory is to understand the unity of consciousness and activity. Activity theory incorporates strong notions of intentionality, history, mediation, collaboration and development in constructing consciousness (see Kaptelinin, chapter 5; Kuutti, this volume). Activity theorists argue that consciousness is not a set of discrete disembodied cognitive acts (decision making, classification, remembering), and certainly it is not the brain; rather, consciousness is located in everyday practice: you are what you do. And what you do is firmly and inextricably embedded in the social matrix of which every person is an organic part.
    [Show full text]